Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Opinion

Understanding and Treating


Unwanted Trauma Memories in
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Anke Ehlers
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London, UK

Distressing and intrusive reexperiencing of the trauma is a with successful therapy (Hackmann, Ehlers, Speckens, &
hallmark symptom of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Clark, 2004; Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005b;
American Psychiatric Association, 1994). However, Speckens, Ehlers, Hackmann, & Clark, 2006).
unwanted memories of trauma are not a sign of pathology (1) Nowness: Trauma survivors with PTSD describe to a
per se. In the initial weeks after a traumatic experience, greater extent that their intrusive memories appear to happen
intrusive memories are common. For most trauma survivors, in the here and now than those without PTSD (Michael et al.,
intrusions become less frequent and distressing over time. 2005b). Most dramatically, in a posttraumatic ashback peo-
A central question for understanding and treating patients ple lose all contact with current reality and respond as if the
with PTSD is therefore what maintains distressing intrusive trauma was happening at that moment (see, e.g., Ehlers,
reexperiencing in these people. Three factors appear to be Hackmann, & Michael, 2004). To a lesser degree, this sense
important: (1) memory processes responsible for the easy of nowness is also characteristic of other intrusive trauma
triggering of intrusive memories, (2) the individuals inter- memories in PTSD (Michael et al., 2005b). Furthermore,
pretations of their trauma memories, and (3) their cognitive people with PTSD may also show affect without recollection
and behavioral responses to trauma memories. (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), that is, emotions and behavior from
the trauma without having a conscious memory of the trauma
(e.g., collapsing in a fetal position when seeing someone who
resembles the assailant). Thus, some of the reexperiencing
Characteristics of Reexperiencing symptoms lack the autonoetic awareness which is a dening
in PTSD feature of episodic memories (Tulving, 2002).
(2) Lack of context: Intrusive memories in PTSD lack
Clues about the memory processes underlying reexperienc- contextual information and appear disjointed from other rel-
ing symptoms in PTSD can be drawn from their phenome- evant autobiographical information (Ehlers et al., 2004;
nological characteristics. Note that unlike the convention in Michael et al., 2005b). People with PTSD keep reexperienc-
DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), which ing moments of the trauma and the corresponding emotions
classies ruminative thoughts about the trauma such as (e.g., when they believed they would never see their children
Why did it happen to me or If only I had as a part again), even if they know the predicted outcome did not
of reexperiencing, the author distinguishes between intrusive occur. Thus, during intrusive trauma memories, people with
memories and rumination about the trauma (Ehlers & Clark, PTSD have difculties retrieving other information that cor-
2000; Ehlers & Steil, 1995). Evidence is accumulating that rected their original impressions and predictions during
these cognitions are phenomenologically and functionally these moments or their meanings.
distinct (e.g., Evans, Ehlers, Mezey, & Clark, 2007b; (3) Ease of triggering by matching cues: In PTSD, a wide
Speckens, Ehlers, Hackmann, Ruths, & Clark, 2007). range of situations can trigger intrusive memories, including
Systematic comparisons of intrusive trauma memories in those that do not have an obvious meaningful connection
people with and without PTSD pointed to many important with the trauma and those that the individual does not recog-
similarities. The intrusions most commonly take the form nize as triggers. This has the effect that intrusions may appear
of relatively brief, vivid sensory impressions such as images, to come out of the blue. Closer analysis of these situations
sounds, body sensations, tastes, or smells (e.g., Ehlers et al., shows that triggers often have sensory similarities with stim-
2002; Michael, Ehlers, & Halligan, 2005a). Yet, there are uli present shortly before or during the trauma (e.g., similar
also a number of differences between the intrusions in peo- color, shape, smell, or body sensation; Ehlers et al., 2002).
ple with and without PTSD. These have been shown to pre- (4) Distress: Trauma survivors with PTSD describe their
dict chronic PTSD, over and above what can be predicted intrusive memories as more distressing than those without
from the frequency of early intrusions, and shown to change PTSD (e.g., Michael et al., 2005b).

2010 Hogrefe Publishing. Distributed under the Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology 2010; Vol. 218(2):141145
Hogrefe OpenMind License [http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/a000001] DOI: 10.1027/0044-3409/a000021
142 Opinion

onset of the trauma or of one of the distressing. Furthermore, it is important to note that in pro-
worst moments of the trauma
longed trauma, there are usually several moments when the
meaning of the event changed for the worse, and each of
A
them may be represented in reexperiencing.
B

C Hypothesized Memory Mechanisms


D Underlying Reexperiencing
Sensory impression predicting onset of trauma or
worst moment that is later reexperienced Ehlers and Clark (2000) suggested that reexperiencing and
Time
the ease with which it is triggered in PTSD can be explained
by a combination of three memory processes, namely strong
Figure 1. Warning signal hypothesis of intrusive memo- perceptual priming, strong associative learning, and poor
ries (Ehlers et al., 2002). Possible temporal relationships memory elaboration (binding with other information in auto-
between content of intrusion and worst moments of the biographical memory). Experimental evidence for the role of
trauma. each of these processes in reexperiencing is emerging (e.g.,
priming: Ehlers, Michael, Chen, Payne, & Shan, 2006;
Michael & Ehlers, 2007; Michael et al., 2005a; associative
learning: Sundermann, Ehlers, Bollinghaus, Gamer, &
What Is Reexperienced? Glucksman, 2010; Wessa & Flor, 2007; poor binding with
other autobiographical information: Kleim, Wallott, &
Many things happen during a traumatic event. What deter- Ehlers, 2008).
mines what is later reexperienced? Patients with PTSD The description of phenomenological characteristics of
reported in systematic interviews that they reexperienced reexperiencing and intentional trauma recall in PTSD by
brief moments from the trauma such as hearing footsteps Ehlers and Clark (2000) has been interpreted by some theo-
behind me or seeing the perpetrator stand before me with rists (Berntsen, 2009) as an argument for special mecha-
a knife (Ehlers et al., 2002). Further analysis of these intru- nisms that are unique to trauma memories. This is
sions indicated that they represented sensory impressions incorrect. The Ehlers and Clark (2000) model proposes that
that signaled the onset of the trauma or the onset of its worst the phenomenology of reexperiencing can be explained by
moments. Ehlers et al. (2002) therefore suggested that the the above three basic memory processes. One important dif-
intrusions had functional signicance in that they repre- ference between the Ehlers and Clark (2000) model and
sented stimuli that predicted the worst moments of trauma other approaches that aim to explain involuntary memories
and had thus acquired the status of warning signals, consis- of trauma by general memory mechanisms (Berntsen,
tent with modern associative learning models that highlight 2009; Rubin, Boals, & Berntsen, 2008) is the range of mem-
the information value of conditioned stimuli (CS) in predict- ory phenomena under consideration. Whereas Ehlers and
ing the unconditioned stimuli (UCS) (Rescorla, 1988). Thus, Clarks (2000) model was developed to explain the full
in Pavlovian conditioning terms, it appears that people with range of intrusive reexperiencing in PTSD, including those
PTSD reexperience the CS rather than the UCS after trauma. that lack autonoetic awareness, Berntsen (2009) and Rubin
Evidence for this hypothesis comes from studies of et al. (2008) only include the involuntary retrieval of mem-
PTSD patients and violent offenders who had intrusive ory content that is recognized by the individual as a trauma
memories of their crime (Evans, Ehlers, Mezey, & Clark, memory. Berntsen (2009) explicitly excludes involuntary
2007a; Hackmann et al., 2004). auditory and visual imagery that is not deemed autobio-
Ehlers et al.s (2002) emphasis that the sensory impres- graphical and phenomena in which the involuntary mental
sions that are later reexperienced preceded the trauma or contents overrule reality more or less completely (p. 15).
its worst moments has been interpreted by some authors Thus, many of the phenomena that the Ehlers and Clark
(e.g., Berntsen & Rubin, 2008) as meaning that they needed (2000) theory aims to explain (such as ashbacks where
to be nished before trauma onset (Possibility A in Figure 1). patients lose all contact with current reality, other intrusions
However, consistent with what is known about CS in that are not recognized by the individual as a memory, reex-
Pavlovian conditioning (Rescorla, 1988), the hypothesis periencing strong emotions, physiological reactions, or
concerns sensory impressions that started just before the behavior from the trauma without recollection of the
worst moment and thus had predictive information value. trauma), are excluded from consideration. However, these
They may well have still been present during the worst phenomena are important for understanding PTSD and for
moment (this includes possibilities B to D in Figure 1). Fur- developing effective treatments. The wider range of reexpe-
thermore, it is important to note that the worst moments of riencing symptoms under consideration explains why the
the trauma are dened subjectively in Ehlers et al. (2002), Ehlers and Clark (2000) model emphasizes memory pro-
from the perspective of the individual with PTSD, not cesses that facilitate cue-driven reexperiencing, namely
from the perspective of what other people may nd most priming and associative learning.

Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology 2010; Vol. 218(2):141145 2010 Hogrefe Publishing. Distributed under the
Hogrefe OpenMind License [http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/a000001]
Opinion 143

Are There Deficits in the Intentional narrative that were not; furthermore in 23% of the cases these
moments were not included in the narrative at all.
Recall of Trauma Memories? Further clarity is needed about the exact nature of the
problems in voluntary recall of parts of the trauma memory
Several theorists suggest that compromised cognitive pro- in PTSD and the pathways by which they contribute to the
cessing during the trauma leads to decits in intentional persistence of PTSD. Ehlers et al. (2004) suggested that the
recall of the trauma. Different hypotheses about the nature disjointedness of the memories of the worst moments during
of this decit have been suggested in the literature, for exam- the trauma from other relevant autobiographical information
ple, a decit in memory representations that facilitate inten- is particularly relevant in PTSD. Kleim et al. (2008) found
tional recall (Brewin, Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996) or highly experimental support for this hypothesis. Disjointedness
fragmented memories (e.g., Foa & Rothbaum, 1998; van has the effect that when the individual remembers the worst
der Kolk & Fisler, 1995). Building on the work on trans- moments, they have difculty accessing other information
fer-appropriate processing (for a review see Roediger, that puts the meaning of these moments into perspective.
1990), Ehlers and Clark (2000) suggested that data-driven This maintains a sense of current threat (Ehlers & Clark,
processing during trauma (i.e., the predominant processing 2000). Furthermore, poor memory elaboration is thought
the sensory impressions) will facilitate strong perceptual to contribute to reexperiencing as it leads to poor inhibition
priming for accompanying stimuli and (in combination with of cue-driven retrieval. Note that this would only be
a lack of self-referent processing) will lead to relatively expected to lead to distressing reexperiencing symptoms if
weaker intentional recall of the worst moments of the trauma. there is also strong priming for potential triggers and strong
Other theorists (Berntsen, 2009; Rubin et al., 2008) pre- associations between the triggers and emotional responses as
dict the opposite and hypothesize that trauma memories are (Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Ehlers et al., 2004).
highly accessible so that intentional and unintentional retrie-
val is enhanced. Empirical tests of these hypotheses in clin-
ical populations are sparse (for reviews see Ehlers et al.,
2004; McNally, 2003). There is very little evidence for com- Nontrauma Autobiographical
plete psychogenic amnesia (Evans et al., 2009; McNally,
2003, but also see Markowitsch et al., 1998). Unless there Memories in PTSD
are organic causes for poor memory (e.g., head injury or
drugs), trauma survivors usually remember most of what People with and without PTSD also differ in how they
happened. There is evidence, however, for more subtle prob- remember other events in their lives. Like people with
lems with intentional recall. People with PTSD may be con- depression, those with PTSD have difculty remembering
fused about the order of events, have small gaps in memory, specic autobiographical events (see Moore & Zoellner,
or may fail to remember details that are important for the 2007, for a review). This effect is not due to differences
meaning of especially distressing parts of the event (e.g., a in verbal intelligence (Schonfeld & Ehlers, 2006). Low auto-
rape survivor who blamed herself for not ghting the perpe- biographical memory specicity at 2 weeks after trauma pre-
trator did not remember that he had threatened her with a dicts chronic PTSD (Kleim & Ehlers, 2008).
knife beforehand). Trauma survivors with PTSD give more
disorganized narratives of traumatic events than those with-
out PTSD (e.g., Halligan, Michael, Clark, & Ehlers, 2003;
Harvey & Bryant, 1999). Relationship Between Memory
In the authors view, the controversy about decient ver- and Appraisals in PTSD
sus enhanced intentional recall of trauma in PTSD can be
resolved by considering the trauma as a series of events An important predictor of chronic PTSD is how people inter-
rather than one event (Ehlers et al., 2004). Earlier work pret their memories of the trauma. Ehlers and Steil (1995)
denes the traumatic event from an external point of view suggested that people with PTSD show negative idiosyn-
and refers to the memory for this externally dened event cratic interpretations of intrusive trauma memories such as
as the trauma memory. The author believes that this is mis- I am going crazy, which lead to a sense of ongoing threat
leading. From the trauma survivors perspective, the trau- and distress, and motivate dysfunctional control strategies
matic experience may have several distinct parts that are such as rumination, memory suppression, and excessive
not necessarily remembered as one integrated event. For safety-seeking behaviors that maintain PTSD. Prospective
example, Ehlers and Clark (2000) described a patient with longitudinal studies of trauma survivors supported this
PTSD who had intrusive memories that contradicted each suggestion (e.g., Dunmore, Clark, & Ehlers, 2001; Ehlers,
other and stemmed from different parts of the (prolonged) Mayou, & Bryant, 1998; Ehring, Ehlers, & Glucksman,
traumatic event. Furthermore, it is important to bear in mind 2008; Halligan, Michael, Clark, & Ehlers, 2003; Michael,
that only some moments from the traumatic event are later Halligan, Clark, & Ehlers, 2007; Murray, Ehlers, & Mayou,
reexperienced. Thus, the proposed problems in intentional 2002; Steil & Ehlers, 2000).
retrieval may mainly apply to the memory for these Similarly, Ehlers and Clark (2000) suggested that negative
moments. In line with this argument, Evans et al. (2007a) interpretations of problems in intentional recall such as mem-
found that narratives of the moments that were reexperienced ory gaps or difculties in remembering the exact order of
were more disorganized than other segments of the trauma events (e.g., I must have a brain damage and Something

2010 Hogrefe Publishing. Distributed under the Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology 2010; Vol. 218(2):141145
Hogrefe OpenMind License [http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/a000001]

Potrebbero piacerti anche