Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

.

net

Scientific support for raising the age of


criminal responsibility
This report summarizes psychological and neuroscientific evidence from over 20 peer-reviewed studies. Compiled by the
Scientist Action and Advocacy Network. For questions or comments, contact info@scaan.net.

March 16, 2017

Summary

Developmental neuroscience research shows that brain areas (e.g., the prefrontal cortex) that
are involved in deliberative decision-making and the regulation of emotions and impulses are
among the last to develop, continuing to mature into young adulthood.
Because adolescents brains are under construction, they have a remarkable and unique ability
to learn new values and habits. However, this malleability is a crossroads: whether the brain
develops in prosocial or antisocial directions depends strongly on the adolescents environment
and experiences.
This malleability means that incarceration can leave a lasting and traumatic mark on adolescents
as they are separated from friends, parents, and loved ones, and deprived of the opportunities
to practice prosocial behavior. Conversely, this malleability means that the adolescent brain
is particularly amenable to rehabilitation. In adolescents demonstrating risk for criminal or
antisocial behavior, interventions that provide opportunities for prosocial interactions, both in
the community and with positive role models, can bolster healthy development.
Rather than hindering social and emotional development through incarceration, juvenile justice
policy should aim to reduce recidivism, promote rehabilitation, and implement interventions
during this formative window of brain plasticity.
The choice between these two approaches is particularly important because the psychological
and social skills that develop during adolescence tend to persist into adulthood. Therefore, how
we use this unique window of opportunity will likely have lasting impacts on the rest of their
adult life.
Scientific support for raising the age of criminal responsibility

1 Brain areas involved in negative images than adults aged 20-25, sug-
decision-making and regulation gesting less control over emotional reactions.3
Threats and rewards greatly compromise in-
of emotions are not fully hibitory control in adolescents compared to
developed in adolescents. adults.4 Adolescents are therefore particularly
susceptible to making risky decisions in emo-
Developmental neuroscience research shows that tional settings.2
brain areas (e.g., the prefrontal cortex) that are in-
volved in deliberative decision-making and the regu- Exploratory behavior. Adolescents have val-
lation of emotions and impulses are among the last to ues, attitudes, and beliefs which are ex-
develop, continuing to mature into young adulthood. ploratory and experimental but often not
enduring5 such views are often outgrown nat-
Response to threat (Figure 1). Subjects were
urally with age.
presented with a series of faces, and asked to
push a button for happy faces, and withhold Peer influence and risk-taking (Figure 2).
button presses for fearful faces. Adolescents Adolescents and adults played a traffic light
were less able to do the latter, suggesting that video game, in which they could stop in front
they impulsively react in threatening situations. of a yellow light, or try to pass before it turned
Correspondingly, these threatening situations red. Going for it would save time if successful
result in a stronger response in the adolescent but risk a crash. Subjects knew they were being
brain than in the adult brain.1 watched by their friends. The study found that
adolescents went for the risky decision much
Brain response to
more often, and this was reflected in higher ac-
0.5 fearful stimulus
tivity in reward-related areas of the brain.6,7,8

Percentage of risky decisions


0 in presence of peers
59%

-0.5
6-12 13-17 18+ 50%
Age 48%

Figure 1: Green area indicates the orbitofrontal cortex, a brain region


involved in decision-making and reward processing. The ado- 14-18 19-22 24-29
lescent brains orbitofrontal cortex response to fearful stimuli Age
is significantly higher than that of other age groups. Y-axis
shows beta weights for left orbitofrontal cortex response to Brain response in reward region
fearful faces, relative to calm faces. Adapted from Dreyfuss 0.3 in presence of peers
et al., 2014.1

Response to reward. The adolescent brain 0.2

evaluates rewards differently than the adult


brain, making adolescents more prone to im- 0.1
pulsive decision-making.2
Suppressing emotional information. Sub- 0

jects viewed a rapid stream of letters and


pressed a button to any letter besides X. Back- 14-18 19-22 24-29
Age
ground images had nothing to do with the
task but were emotionally negative, positive, Figure 2: Y-axis in bottom plot shows beta weights for left orbitofrontal
or neutral. Teenagers were less able to ignore cortex activity. Adapted from Chein et al., 2011.6

Page 2 of 5
Scientific support for raising the age of criminal responsibility

A related study used questionnaires to ask to Incarcerated adolescents experience a dis-


what extent young people of difference ages proportionate use of force by prison staff
can resist peer pressure. They found that resis- (Figure 4). The first report of the Nunez In-
tance to peer influence continues to increase dependent Monitor10 found that, each month,
until after the age of 17 (Figure 3). 27% of the incarcerated 16- and 17-year-old
population experienced the use of force by cor-
Resistance to peer inuence
3.4 rectional staff. This rate was 9 times that of
their adult counterparts.
3.2
Percentage of incarcerated people
who experience the use of force
3
by correctional staff per month
27%
2.8
12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 19%
Age

Figure 3: Higher scores on the Resistance to Peer Influence measure


indicate greater resistance to peer influence. Adapted from
Steinberg and Monahan, 2007.8 3%
16-17 18 19+
Future-oriented thinking. Adolescents are Age

highly sensitive to immediate reward and are


less future-oriented in their decision-making.5 Figure 4: Data collected from the New York City Department of Cor-
rections, November 1, 2015 to February 29, 2016. Adapted
from the First Report of the Nunez Independent Monitor.10

2 The adolescent brain is Exposure to the traumatic experiences endemic to


malleable and can be shaped jails and prisons may be particularly detrimental to
positively or negatively by teens long-term outcomes. Neuroscientific evidence
suggests that trauma experienced in adolescence has
environment and experiences. a lasting impact on behavior and brain development.
Because adolescents brains are under construction,9 Adolescence is a unique period of vulnera-
they have a remarkable and unique ability to learn bility. Plasticity in the brain during adoles-
new values and habits. However, this malleability is a cence makes them more vulnerable to social
crossroads: whether the brain develops in proso- stress, trauma, and drug use. Onset of psy-
cial or antisocial directions depends strongly on chiatric illness related to stressful events often
the adolescents environment and experiences. occurs during this time window.11,12,13,14,15

Incarceration and trauma leave lasting Adolescents are susceptible to lasting


marks on adolescents, and promote antiso- trauma (Figure 5). Subjects were trained
to associate a visual square with an unpleasant
cial behavior.
noise. Their fear response, as measured via skin
Incarceration places teens in an environment where conductance, increased upon seeing the square.
they lack the opportunities to practice prosocial be- Subjects were then trained to dissociate the
havior and to learn to regulate their emotions and square from the unpleasant noise. After this
impulses. Such facilities further stunt adolescents training, the fear response remained high in
emotional and social development, separating them adolescents but not in other groups, suggesting
from friends, parents, and loved ones, while employ- that adolescents have a uniquely low ability
ing forceful punitive tactics which themselves may to forget fearful memories. This suggests that
become lasting traumatic experiences. traumatic memories and associations acquired

Page 3 of 5
Scientific support for raising the age of criminal responsibility

during adolescence are more difficult to un- Personality development is sensitive to ex-
learn than those acquired during childhood periences during adolescence. The rapid de-
and adulthood.16 velopment of brain networks during adoles-
cence means that the adolescent brain is par-
Ability to forget fearful memory ticularly sensitive to both positive and negative
0.3
influences; the environment and experiences
of the individual have a much larger impact
0.2 on his or her personality during teenage years
than the same experiences would have on an
0.1 adult.18,19
Self-control develops during adolescence.
0 An important developmental milestone is the
5-11 12-17 18-28
Age
ability to make controlled decisions in emotion-
ally significant high-stakes situations. Brain
Figure 5: After being trained to learn a fearful association, children areas related to self-control are especially sen-
and adults, but not adolescents, were able to unlearn it. sitive during adolescence. Accordingly, cogni-
Adapted from Pattwell et al., 2012.16
tive training during adolescence can strongly
enhance self-control.20
Incarceration facilities stunt social develop- Positive rehabilitation should be most effec-
ment. Punishment-focused facilities stunt the tive during adolescence. Because the adoles-
development of psychosocial maturity in ado- cent brain is highly malleable, it is likely to be
lescents. Psychosocial maturity includes mea- extremely responsive to positive rehabilitation
sures of the ability to curb aggressive behavior, and reformative efforts.21,22
to consider others perspective, and to function
autonomously. This effect is especially strong
for adolescents who feel unsafe in their incar- 3 Conclusion
ceration facility.17
Experimental evidence suggests that, because the
adolescent brain is still developing, adolescents are
Conversely, the adolescent brain is particularly susceptible to lasting damage in trau-
particularly amenable to rehabilitation. matic situations. Conversely, they should be even
more receptive to behavior-reforming programs than
adults.
The heightened sensitivity and malleability of the
Rather than hindering social and emotional develop-
adolescent brain present a window of opportunity
ment through incarceration, juvenile justice policy
for behavioral change. In adolescents demonstrating
should aim to reduce recidivism, promote rehabilita-
risk for criminal or antisocial behavior, interventions
tion, and implement interventions during this forma-
that provide opportunities for prosocial interactions,
tive window of brain plasticity. The choice between
both in the community and with positive role models,
these two approaches is particularly important be-
can bolster healthy development.
cause the psychological and social skills that develop
Brain cell connections are drastically re- during adolescence tend to persist into adulthood.
shaped during adolescence. In the frontal Therefore, how we use this unique window of oppor-
cortex, adolescents can have up to twice as tunity will likely have lasting impacts on the rest of
many connections between brain cells com- their adult life.
pared to adults. As adolescents grow into
adults, these connections are severed in a
way that reflects their learned memories and
experiences.9

Page 4 of 5
Scientific support for raising the age of criminal responsibility

References [13] Ronald C Kessler et al. Age of onset of mental dis-


orders: a review of recent literature. In: Current
[1] Michael Dreyfuss et al. Teens impulsively react opinion in psychiatry 20.4 (2007), p. 359.
rather than retreat from threat. In: Developmental [14] Ronald C Kessler et al. Lifetime prevalence and
neuroscience 36.3-4 (2014), pp. 220227. age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in
[2] Leah H Somerville and BJ Casey. Developmental the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. In:
neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational Archives of general psychiatry 62.6 (2005), pp. 593
systems. In: Current opinion in neurobiology 20.2 602.
(2010), pp. 236241. [15] Elizabeth S Ver Hoeve et al. Short-term and long-
term effects of repeated social defeat during ado-
[3] Julia E Cohen-Gilbert and Kathleen M Thomas.
lescence or adulthood in female rats. In: Neuro-
Inhibitory control during emotional distraction
science 249 (2013), pp. 6373.
across adolescence and early adulthood. In: Child
development 84.6 (2013), pp. 19541966. [16] Siobhan S Pattwell et al. Altered fear learning
across development in both mouse and human.
[4] Alexandra O Cohen et al. When is an adolescent In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
an adult? Assessing cognitive control in emotional 109.40 (2012), pp. 1631816323.
and nonemotional contexts. In: Psychological sci-
ence (2016), p. 0956797615627625. [17] Julia Dmitrieva et al. Arrested development: The
effects of incarceration on the development of
[5] Laurence Steinberg and Elizabeth S Scott. Less psychosocial maturity. In: Development and psy-
guilty by reason of adolescence: developmental chopathology 24.03 (2012), pp. 10731090.
immaturity, diminished responsibility, and the ju-
[18] Suparna Choudhury, Tony Charman, and Sarah-
venile death penalty. In: American Psychologist
Jayne Blakemore. Development of the teenage
58.12 (2003), p. 1009.
brain. In: Mind, Brain, and Education 2.3 (2008),
[6] Jason Chein et al. Peers increase adolescent risk pp. 142147.
taking by enhancing activity in the brains reward [19] Ronald E Dahl. Adolescent brain development:
circuitry. In: Developmental science 14.2 (2011), a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities.
F1F10. Keynote address. In: Annals of the New York
[7] Margo Gardner and Laurence Steinberg. Peer in- Academy of Sciences 1021.1 (2004), pp. 122.
fluence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky [20] Philip David Zelazo and Stephanie M Carlson. Hot
decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an and cool executive function in childhood and ado-
experimental study. In: Developmental psychology lescence: Development and plasticity. In: Child
41.4 (2005), p. 625. Development Perspectives 6.4 (2012), pp. 354360.
[8] Laurence Steinberg and Kathryn C Monahan. Age [21] Adriana Galvn. Insights about adolescent be-
differences in resistance to peer influence. In: De- havior, plasticity, and policy from neuroscience
velopmental psychology 43.6 (2007), p. 1531. research. In: Neuron 83.2 (2014), pp. 262265.
[9] Zdravko Petanjek et al. Extraordinary neoteny of [22] Kenneth Laundra, Keyria Rodgers, and Heidi Zapp.
synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Transforming teens: Measuring the effects of
In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences restorative justice principles in a teen court set-
108.32 (2011), pp. 1328113286. ting. In: Juvenile and Family Court Journal 64.4
(2013), pp. 2134.
[10] Steven J Martin. First Report of the Nunez Inde-
pendent Monitor. In: (2016), pp. 1130.
[11] Susan L Andersen and Martin H Teicher. Stress,
sensitive periods and maturational events in ado-
lescent depression. In: Trends in neurosciences
31.4 (2008), pp. 183191.
[12] Delia Fuhrmann, Lisa J Knoll, and Sarah-Jayne
Blakemore. Adolescence as a sensitive period of
brain development. In: Trends in cognitive sciences
19.10 (2015), pp. 558566.

Page 5 of 5

Potrebbero piacerti anche