Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Neurocase

2004, Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 207214

Differential Impact of Parvocellular


and Magnocellular Pathways on Visual
Impairment in Apperceptive Agnosia?

Helmut Hildebrandt1,2, Cathleen Schutze1,3, Markus Ebke1 and Karoline Spang3


1
Municipal Hospital of Bremen, Neurology, Bremen, Germany, 2Institute for Psychology, University of Oldenburg,
Oldenburg, Germany and 3Institute for Human-Neurobiology, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany

Abstract

The term visual form agnosia describes a disorder characterized by problems recognizing objects, poor copying,
and distinguishing between simple geometric shapes despite normal intellectual abilities. Visual agnosia has
been interpreted as a disorder of the magnocellular visual system, caused by an inability to separate figure from ground
by sampling information from extended regions of space and to integrate it with fine-grain local information. However,
this interpretation has hardly been tested with neuropsychological or functional brain imaging methods, mainly
because the magnocellular and parvocellular structures are highly interconnected in the visual system.
We studied a patient (AM) who had suffered a sudden heart arrest, causing hypoxic brain damage. He
was/is severely agnosic, as apparent in both the Birmingham Object Recognition Battery and the Visual Object and
Space Battery. First- and especially second-order motion perception was also impaired, but AM experienced
no problems in grasping and navigating through space. The patient revealed a normal P100 in visual
evoked potentials both with colored and fine-grained achromatic checkerboards. But the amplitude of the P100 was
clearly decreased if a coarse achromatic checkerboard was presented.
The physiological and neuropsychological findings indicate that AM experienced problems integrating
information over extended regions of space and in detecting second-order motion. This may be interpreted as a
disorder of the magnocellular system, with intact parvocellular system and therefore preserved ability to detect both
local features and colors.

Introduction
Benson and Greenberg (1969) described a syndrome which produce different classes of symptoms. A subgroup of these
they named visual form agnosia. The case they described patients cannot distinguish between different shapes, sizes,
suffered from no serious intellectual impairment, but from and shadings of grey. One popular explanation for these
object recognition problems and poor copying abilities. As symptoms proposes a selective damage of the magnocellular
shown by Efron (1969) the patient was unable to distinguish visual pathway (Milner et al., 1991; Davidoff and Warrington,
between simple geometric shapes. Several other patients 1993).
with similar impairments have been described (Adler, The term magnocellular refers to a subdivision of the
1944, 1950; Abadi et al., 1981; Campion and Latto, 1985). visual system originating in the alpha ganglion cells of the
Extensive testing of these patients showed multiple dissocia- retina, projecting to layers 4B and 4C of the striate cortex
tions between figure-ground separation (Kartsounis and (V1), and thence to the thick stripes of the adjacent area
Warrington, 1991), shape discrimination (Davidoff and V2 (Livingstone and Hubel, 1988). The magnocellular path-
Warrington, 1993), and luminance and color discrimina- way is supposed to predominantly analyse depth and motion
tion (Heywood et al., 1994). These dissociations have been cues, while it is color blind. On the other hand, because of
taken as an argument for segregated visual pathways in the the larger receptive fields of its neurons, it has also been
human brain, which can be lesioned focally and therefore related to the perception of size and length differences

Correspondence to: Helmut Hildebrandt, University of Oldenburg, Institute of Psychology, 2611 Oldenburg, Germany. e-mail: helmut.hildebrandt@
uni-oldenburg.de

10.1080/13554790490495168$16.00 # Taylor & Francis Ltd.


208 H. Hildebrandt et al.

(Kosslyn et al., 1989, 1990), to perception of low spatial involves primarily the magnocellular pathway, whereas single
frequencies (i.e., coarse spatial information) and therefore of letter reading depends on parvocellular activity (Omtzigt
surfaces or entities in figure-ground separation (Milner et al., et al., 2002). The high incidence of reading impairments in
1991). visual form agnosia may therefore support the explanation
Though many of the problems of patients suffering from of their deficits in terms of damage to the magnocellular
visual form agnosia can be explained by an isolated lesion pathway.
of the magnocellular system, not all form agnosic patients Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) provide useful neuro-
have problems in recognizing motion and in localizing physiological information with respect to the functioning of
objects for action. Based on the importance of the magno- the two systems. Two predictions can be made in this
cellular pathway for motion processing, one would predict respect: (1) Selective damage of the magnocellular pathway,
that any lesion of this system should severely impair motion leaving the parvocellular pathway intact, should result in an
perception (for an extended discussion of this aspect, see intact P100 when using a checkerboard defined by color,
Goodale and Milner, 1995). Motion perception was tested independent of the square size since the parvocellular-blob
extensively only in the case of Milner et al. (1991), D.F. pathway processes color-defined stimuli for all visible spa-
and her motion perception was indeed impaired. Neither tial frequencies (Tobimatsu et al., 1995; Spinelli et al., 1996;
Bartolomeo et al. (1998) nor Davidoff and Warrington Arakawa et al., 1999). (2) If the magnocellular system
(1993), Ferreira et al. (1998) or Kartsounis and Warrington involves the sampling of low spatial frequencies, then
(1991) reported an impairment of motion perception in their achromatic checkerboards consisting of small squares should
cases. produce a normal VEP whereas a checkerboard with rela-
One possibility to reconcile these apparently contradictory tively large squares should evoke clearly smaller VEP ampli-
findings is that motion perception has not been investigated tudes (Livingstone et al., 1991). In particular, the amplitude
extensively in visual form agnosic patients because the of the N1/P1 complex reflects spatial frequency (Tobimatsu
impairment was so subtle that it did not produce obvious et al., 1995; Arakawa et al., 1999). In achromatic checker-
clinical deficits. For example, only higher-order motion de- boards this amplitude increases only with patterns of a
tection may be affected while first-order motion detection, spatial frequency well above 2 cycles per degree, whereas
relying on luminance changes, remains intact (Chubb and in chromatic checkerboards it decreases starting at 2 cycles
Sperling, 1988; Cavanagh and Mather, 1989; Smith et al., per degree.
1994; Braun et al., 1998). Recently, we studied a patient with visual form agnosia
Another possibility is to postulate different magnocellular due to sudden heart arrest using a series of neuropsycho-
subsystems of visual information processing responsible for logical tests and investigating first and second order motion
low spatial frequencies, for motion perception and for object perception. Moreover cortical sum potentials were evoked
localization. Low and high spatial frequencies are indeed by reversing achromatic and colored checkerboards of
processed predominantly in different parts of the brain (Fink different sizes.
et al., 1997, 1999) and may be disturbed differentially after
brain lesions (Robertson and Lamb, 1991), although the
Case description
hemispheric difference in global/local processing only be-
comes apparent at later stages of information processing AM, a technical drawer, suffered from sudden heart arrest
(Heinze et al., 1998). A strong argument for such a separation when he was 46 years-old. He was reanimated and awoke
of the magnocellular pathway comes from reading research. from coma three days after the incident. Five weeks after
Patients with developmental dyslexia show poor control of his acute medical treatment, AM was admitted to a neu-
saccades (Biscaldi et al., 1994, 1998) and impaired visual- rological rehabilitation unit where all the investigations
spatial attention (Facoetti et al., 2000). They show differences reported below took place. At this time, he had no obvious
in the structure of their magnocellular neurons at the lateral motor problems, was aware of having visual and memory
geniculate nucleus (Livingstone et al., 1991) and in the problems, but was disoriented in time and place. No
physiological activity of magno-recipient areas such as MT confusional state was evident, although he left the hospital
(Eden et al., 1996; Sperling et al., 2003; Stein, 2003). But several times without being allowed to do so. His mood
clearly dyslexic patients are not as impaired in motion per- state alternated between calm and anxious, but he never
ception and grasping (visuomotor integration) as they are in became really upset or aggressive. The patient was always
reading. motivated to participate in the investigations and in train-
Patients with visual form agnosia were also impaired in ing sessions, and became tired only after one hour of
reading (Kartsounis and Warrington, 1991; Milner et al., testing.
1991; Davidoff and Warrington, 1993; Bartolomeo et al., A neurological examination showed intact motor, sensori-
1998; Ferreira et al., 1998), but not necessarily in shape motor and cranial nerve functions. There was no visual field
perception (Kartsounis and Warrington, 1991), and in identi- defect as tested with a Goldman Perimeter. A 1.5 Tesla MRI
fying single letters. Recent investigations have shown that in scan, taken 2 months after the heart arrest, showed no focal
normal readers the separation of the letters in letter strings damage and no major brain atrophy.
Differential impact of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways 209

Neuropsychological investigation
Visual fields during visual extinction
We used the TAP-Neglect test (see Hildebrandt et al., 2002,
for an extensive description) to analyze the visual fields of
AM during visual extinction. The test was performed in a
dimmed room, with maximal screen contrast and a 21-inch
screen. AM had no visual field deficits, but response times
especially to targets on the left side were increased (725 ms for
left-sided targets and 602 ms for right-sided targets), and he
also missed one item there.

Clinical testing of visual perception


Illusionary contours
A set of high frequency gratings were shown to AM and he
had to decide whether or not a simple geometric figure was
hidden in these gratings, defined by small line breaks. AM had Fig. 1. Copy of the Rey-Osterrieth Figure. Notice the piecemeal fashion of the
no problems identifying circles and squares in the gratings, copy.
although his reactions were slow and he gave the impression
of not always feeling totally sure.

Kanisza figures
A set of Kanisza figures was presented and AMs task was to
recognize the figures. To enhance the sensitivity to impair-
ment we started with distorted figures (rotating one or two of
the Pac-men by about 90 degrees and then gradually back
rotating these circles). AM was able to discriminate between
the conditions, although his reactions were again slow and
appeared sometimes uncertain.

Color perception
A set of colored squares was shown on a computer monitor
and AM was asked to name the colors. This turned out to be a
very easy task for him.

Copying and visuoconstructional abilities


When copying the complex Rey-Osterrieth figure, AM started
in a piecemeal fashion and ended up with just a partial Fig. 2. Item of the Gailinger Abzeichentest.
solution, documenting some visuoconstructional problems
(see Fig. 1).
In the Gailinger Abzeichentest (Gailinger copy test) geo- probably because the letters in Arial lack serifs. Second, his
metrical figures have to be copied on a sheet of paper with reading ability depended on letter size: words written in small
dots, indicating specific parts of the original figure. Some of letters (letter size < 16 pt.) led to confusions between letters.
these figures require three-dimensional perception. AM had
no major problems perceiving the three-dimensional figures
Neuropsychological testing of visual
but in assessing the relative length of parts of the figures, and
abilities of AM
made four errors during copying (see Fig. 2).
In the Block Design test of the WAIS AMs score was 5, Space perception was investigated with the Visual Object
indicating a medium impairment. and Space Perception Battery (VOSP, see Table 1) and the
Birmingham Object Recognition Test (BORB, see Table 2).
Reading Because the control data used in BORB reflect a 65 age
AM could read single letters if the letters were spatially group, we additionally collected data from 11 control subjects
separated (independent of font type). The ability to read (mean age of 49 years, minimum age of 43, maximum age of
words depended on two aspects: First, he performed much 54 years). AM scored in all spatial tasks BORB below the
better in reading fonts like Arial than Times Roman, range of our control group, but in the normal range of the
210 H. Hildebrandt et al.

Table 1. Results in the visual object and space perception battery

Visual space perception Object recognition

VOSP Correct trials Cut-off score Correct trials Cut-off score

Shape detection (screening task) 18 15 Incomplete letters 8 17


Dot numerosity 10 8 Foreshortened Silhouettes 4 (only animal pictures) 16
Visual location 13 18 Object recognition 2 15
Spatial location 3 7

Performance in tests written in italics was impaired.

Table 2. Results in the Birmingham object recognition battery

Visual space perception Object recognition

BORB Correct trials Controlsa Range of controlsa Correct trials Controlsa Range of controlsa
(maximum) Mean (SD) & (cut-off score (maximum) Mean (SD) & (cut-off score
BORBb) BORBb)

Length match 18 (30) 26.6 (1.6) 2430 (<24) Minimal feature 16 (25) 24.8 (0.4) 2425 (< 19)
task match
Size match task 22 (30) 27.7 (2.3) 2430 (<25) Foreshortened 15 (25) 24.5 (0.5) 2425 (< 16)
match
Orientation 20 (30) 25.3 (1.4) 2328 (<20) Object decision 22 (32) 31.7 (0.6) 3032 (< 28)
match task (easy)
Position of gap 28 (40) 34.8 (2.4) 3039 (<27) Item match 25 (32) 32 (0) 3232 (< 26)
match task
Association match 25 (30) 29.6 (0.8) 2830 (< 22)
Picture naming 10 (15) 14.4 (0.8) 1315 (< 8)
(short version)
Animate drawings
Picture naming of 12 (20) 20 (0) 2020 (< 17)
inanimate
drawings from
Subtest 7
Picture naming 50 (76) 73.8 (2.4) 6976 (< 64)
(long version)

a
Eleven age-matched control subjects were tested with the BORB.
b
Scores of the BORB control group.
Performance in tests written in italics was below the cut-off scores of the BORB. But note that AM scored outside the range of our control group in all subtests.

original BORB control group for two tasks: the orientation fied only 8 out of 36 paired stimuli presented and 22 out of 54
match task and the position of gap task, both requiring a triplets presented). However, this performance is far below the
comparison of directions. Furthermore, AMs performance normal range.
was normal in the screening task and in dot counting of the Object recognition was also investigated with the VOSP
VOSP. and BORB (see Tables 1 and 2). AM scored in all of these
Identifying nonoverlapping figures, examined with the tasks below the range of our control group. He was also
subtest of the BORB, was possible for single letters (AM impaired on all these tasks taking the results of the original
misnamed 2 of 18 letters, but this may be due to his first BORB control group, except for association match and
language being Iranian) and almost perfect for geometrical partly for picture naming of the BORB. In the case of
shapes (3 misidentifications on 36 trials). Identification of picture naming his identification of animals was in the normal
single line drawings (14 errors in 40 trials), of paired letters range, whereas the naming of manmade objects was highly
and of triplets was impaired (13 errors on 36 presentations of impaired. His errors in the association match task involved
paired letters and 28 errors out of 36 presented triplets). The only manmade objects, but not animals. It is worth noticing
same held for triplets of shapes (10 mistakes on 36 trials) but that the only four correct answers AM gave in the Silhouette
less so for paired geometrical shapes (2 errors on 36 trials). task of the VOSP all concerned animals. We further tested this
Overlapping letters, geometrical shapes and line drawings difference by means of pictures of the Rivermead Behavioural
could hardly be identified. In general, AM replied correctly on Memory Test using 16 animals and 16 manmade objects.
less than half of the trials with the exception of geometrical AM identified all 16 animals correctly, but only 13 of the
shapes, on which he performed slightly better (he misidenti- manmade objects.
Differential impact of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways 211

Motion perception Table 3. Thresholds of AM and controls in detecting first-order (Fourier) and
second-order (Theta) motion
First-order and second-order motion perception was inves-
tigated using sets of moving random dots (Braun et al., Fourier motiona Theta motiona
1998). For each kind of motion the subject had to identify
Controls (n 21)
the direction of motion of a small rectangle, at 108 distance Mean 32.85 36.39
on the right or on the left of a central fixation point, moving Standard Deviation 5.82 6.77
upwards or downwards on the screen. The target was defined Minimum 25.20 26.6
Maximum 45.85 47.10
as a rectangle area with dots moving preferentially in one
direction, while dots in the remainder of the screen moved in AM: t1 45.85 74.05
AM: t2 63.6
random directions. The coherence of the dots in the target
was varied between 0% (random, no coherence) and 100% a
Numbers indicate the percentages of dots moving coherently in the target area,
(all dots in one direction, perfect coherence), and the mini- necessary for discrimination of motion direction. Thresholds of the left and right
mal amount of coherence required by the patient to identify hemifield are averaged.
the motion direction of the target was measured. In the first
type of motion condition, the dots within the target area
moved coherently in the same direction as the target itself the thresholds for the left and right sides (see Table 3).
( first-order motion). In the second condition, the dots AM was severely impaired in second order motion perception,
within the rectangle were moving in the opposite direction as he scored 4 standard deviations above the threshold of
as the target itself ( second-order motion). In this condi- the healthy controls. His first order motion perception was
tion, the elementary movement detectors signal the direction somewhat better and at least one of the control subjects
of the dots, not that of the target (cf. Braun et al., 1998), performed of the same level (see Table 3).
and thus higher-order detectors are required to give correct
responses to upward or downward motion of the target Visual evoked potentials
(Theta-motion). Cortical potentials (VEP) were evoked by presenting a
Stimuli were produced by a Silicon Graphics computer contrast-reversing checkerboard pattern, either achromatic
(O2) and presented on a LG Studioworks 221U Monitor white/black or else chromatic blue/red. In the control condition
(1152  870 pixel, monitor size 40  30.2 cm), viewed from the size of each square element was 1.5 cm. In the experimental
an observation distance of 50 cm with frame frequency of condition element size was 10 cm. AM was positioned in a
75 Hz. The screen contained overall 50% white and 50% darkened room at a distance of 1 m from the screen of a 14-
black pixels. The target for both conditions was 28 high and 48 inch VGA monitor. The elements of the checkerboard there-
wide, and moved for 2 s at a speed of 68/s from bottom fore comprised 0.868 in the control condition and 5.78 in the
upwards or from the top down. AM and 21 age-matched experimental condition. An additional stimulus consisted of a
control subjects (mean age of 43 years, minimum age of 38, black/white checkerboard with very small elements. The first
maximum age of 54 years) were tested. Subjects were seated set of VEPs (t1) started with these small elements (0.438),
in a dimly lt room and were instructed to fixate a central continued with the control condition (0.868), and ended with
small red arrow (1.38) that indicated whether the target would the experimental condition (5.78). In the achromatic series
appear on the left or right side of the screen. Subjects had
to indicate verbally whether the rectangle seemed to move
upwards or downwards (a binary forced choice task). The
examiner entered the responses on the keyboard without
seeing the stimulus. False answers were followed by acoustic
feedback. For both hemifields 50 trials were presented each
and coherence level was controlled using a staircase pro-
cedure (PEST; Taylor and Creelman, 1967). To determine
thresholds for motion perception, PEST varied the percentage
of dots within the stimulus area, by gradually decreasing the
percentages of dots within the rectangle that moved in the
defined direction, starting with a correlation of 100%. Hence,
with 0% correlation the movements of the dots within the
rectangle were absolutely identical to those in the surrounding
and the target was no longer present. Direction discrimination
thresholds, defined as percentage of signal dots that lead to
75% correct responses, were calculated using probit analysis
Fig. 3. VEP for different, chromatic and achromatic spatial layouts in AM.
(Finney, 1962). Left side: VEP for left eye stimulation. Right side: VEP for right eye
Motion detection did not differ significantly between hemi- stimulation. Each stimulation was replicated two times producing two graphs
fields both in AM and in the control group, hence we averaged per panel. Note the reduced or almost absent VEPs for both eyes in the
achromatic condition with low spatial frequency patterns (lower panels).
212 H. Hildebrandt et al.

Table 4. Results of the VEP investigation

Black/white Blue/red

High spatial frequency Low spatial frequency High spatial frequency Low spatial frequency

Left eye Right eye Left eye Right eye Left eye Right eye Left eye Right eye

Control (n 17)s
Mean 9.35 8.76 6.61 6.12 6.78 6.48 6.25 6.78
SD 3.68 4.24 2.59 3.06 2.32 2.29 2.28 2.90
Maximum 18.32 20.50 13.23 13.20 10.70 11.50 9.68 9.68
Minimum 3.52 3.08 3.71 2.90 2.10 3.86 1.96 3.17
AM: t1 3.26 1.54 0.91 1.89
AM: t2 6.81 7.08 2.60 2.45 6.02 4.87 6.27 4.60
AM: t3 5.16 6.23 4.30 2.20

All figures: peak to peak mV differences of N1/P100.

AM evoked a normal VEP in the small-sized and in the control was highly impaired and both had problems recognizing
conditions, but there was no clear P100 in the experimental objects in their environment, especially drawings and pic-
condition. Because of a possible order effect, we replicated tures without color information. On the other hand, while
the experimental condition in reverse order after two days (see SMK was able to trace overlapping figures, AM was un-
Fig. 3 and t2 in Table 4) and again seven weeks later (t3 in able to do so. In this respect, AM resembles FGP more
Table 4). At t1 there was no clear-cut N1/P100 complex. At t2 a (Kartsounis and Warrington, 1991), who also had problems
peak-to-peak amplitude (N1/P100) of 2.6 mV was measured recognizing both overlapping and embedded figures. But
for the left eye and 2.45 mV for the right eye. At t3 the FGP was able to discriminate between different object
corresponding amplitudes were 4.3 mV and 2.2 mV. With the sizes, while AM had problems with this task. The visual
exception of the amplitude for the left eye at the last measure- impairment of AM is an additional argument for the
ment, all amplitudes were completely outside the range of an existence of independent mechanisms for figure/ground
age-matched control group (n 17, mean age of 47, minimum discrimination on one hand and the perception of the re-
age of 38, maximum age of 60 years). During stimulation by lative size of surfaces on the other. FGP was able to give a
chromatic reversals the size of the checkerboard elements did correct size estimation, while SMK and AM could not.
not influence the results. In both conditions, AM showed a From a functional neuroanatomical viewpoint it can be
pronounced P100, within the range of the control group (see argued that V1 and V2 have to be intact in AM. In case of
Table 4). Therefore, AMs reduction of the peak-to-peak am- a lesion of V1 one would expect visual field deficits and an
plitude was specific for the large sized black/white checker- attenuation of the VEP potentials. This was not the case in
board. AM. V2 is concerned with higher order boundary percep-
The latencies of the P100 of AM were prolonged in each of tion. Peterhans and von der Heydt (1989, 1991) have shown
the black/white reversal conditions, but not in the color that neurons in this part of the visual system of monkeys
reversals. are activated by illusionary contours presented in their
receptive fields. Because AM was able to detect such
patterns and to discriminate between different Kanisza
Discussion
figures, V2 presumably was not impaired by the hypoxia.
Visual form agnosia has been documented in the past in This assumption can also be illustrated by the fact of
several case descriptions of patients suffering from hypoxia, AMs abilities to identify letters and short words if printed
carbon monoxide poisoning, or bilateral posterior infarcts. in an appropriate font type. Zeki et al. (1990) and Barbur
Our patient AM suffered from a sudden heart arrest. His et al. (1993) have shown that V4 is a major centre of
visual and memory impairment were therefore caused by conscious color perception. Again, at least some parts of
brain hypoxia although no morphological brain damage V4 must have been spared from the hypoxia, because AM
could be traced on an MRI two months after the event. was able to discriminate between colors easily and showed
The profile of his impairments was similar to that of the a normal checkerboard VEP based on blue/red reversals.
case SMK of Davidoff and Warrington (1993), while he The main functional problem for AM was to separate figure
was somewhat less impaired than the case of Milner et al. from ground, to separate individual letters in reading, and to
(1991). The similarity of our case with SMK is striking. estimate sizes. We would like to argue that his impaired
Like SMK, AM was able to perceive subjective contours, performance in the visual and spatial location tasks of the
colors and individual geometrical objects, but he could not VOSP can also be explained by the latter impairment, because
give exact size estimations. In both cases the reading ability both tasks rely on locating stimuli relative to a background
Differential impact of parvocellular and magnocellular pathways 213

figure (the squares). Kartsounis and Warrington (1991) and slower in responding to targets in the left hemifield compared
Milner et al. (1991) assumed that the impairment of FGP and to the right hemifield.
DF might be due to a lesion of the magnocellular pathway. If integration of different focal aspects into one global pic-
Such a hypothesis would be too general for AM because he ture relies on magnocellular structures, the investigation of
(like DF) was able to walk, to grasp and to localize objects in the visual evoked potential is also of relevance. AMs
the environment. All these performances rely on precise amplitudes in the black/white reversal VEPs for coarse
visual-spatial perception. Furthermore, AM was able to copy checker-boards were subnormal while normal for colored
reasonably well (although not in the normal range), which checker- boards of all element-sizes. Hence, the deficit seems
also argues for an intact perception of spatial coordinates. to be specific for large receptive fields, low spatial frequency
Motion perception is certainly one of the major functions of stimulus, i.e., a coarse checkerboard, evokes a normal VEP
the magnocellular system and recently Braddick et al. (2003) amplitude for a chromatic pattern while an impaired VEP for
proposed tests of global motion processing, i.e., the detection an achromatic pattern. This dissociation could be explained
of coherently moving dots, as an appropriate means to analyse by the involvement, in the first case, of parvocellular struc-
dorsal stream vulnerability. In their view the relative tures, whereas this is not the case primarily in the achromatic
threshold increase (how many dots have to move coherently condition. The investigations of Tobimatsu et al. (1995) and
in a specific area for a predominant direction being seen) Arakawa et al. (1999) demonstrate that achromatic stimula-
may be a sensitive measurement of the integrity of the tion of the parvocellular pathway can be dissociated from
magnocellular system. chromatic stimulation using different spatial frequencies.
We used two such tests one for first-order and one for Higher achromatic spatial frequencies (more than 2 cycles
second-order motion perception to investigate AMs motion per degree) seem to stimulate primarily the parvocellular
perception abilities. In both tests AM scored more than two interblob pathway, whereas low frequency spatial patterns
standard deviations above i.e., worse than the control have a higher impact on magnocellular structures, as demon-
group, and at least in the second-order motion task his strated in research on reading and neglect (Spinelli et al.,
performance was completely outside the range of the control 1996).
group. This result points to an impairment of the magnocel- The comparatively better ability of AM to recognize draw-
lular system, especially in the processing of higher-order ings of animals, compared to drawings of manmade objects,
motion signals, which presuppose a decoupling of local may also be explained by such a difference between the
directional selectivity at V1 from the direction of a globally parvocellular and the magnocellular system, while, of course,
moving object. other explanations are possible. Animal pictures, as in the
On the other hand, one could argue that the deficit in motion Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) drawings, often possess
perception of AM is not necessarily the result of a defect of typical features, which allow identification irrespectively of
magnocellular neurons but may also be a consequence of his their global shape. Manmade objects like tables, chairs, etc. are
visual form agnosia. We did not notice any clinical impairment characterized as drawings by symmetrical and partly over-
in motion perception and, although AM could identify surfaces lapping features. Recognition therefore presupposes a global
of degraded stimuli such as the X in the screening test of the integration of local features. In summary we suggest that the
VOSP, he was not perfect in this task. Detecting the predo- visual deficits of AM may be caused primarily by a defect of
minant motion direction of more or less coherently moving the magnocellular system as a result of the anoxic episode.
dots involves a similar perceptual function as in the screening
test of the VOSP. Therefore, a subclinical impairment Acknowledgement
obviously led to a significant deficit in the motion perception
task, with its more extended area to integrate and lower We would like to thank Paul Eling (Nijmegen) and Manfred
coherence level which make detection much more difficult. Fahle (Bremen) for their comments on an earlier draft of this
It has been argued that another function of the magnocel- article.
lular system is the selection of items within arrays of similar Supported by German Research Council, SFB 517.
items (Omtzigt et al., 2002). While the parvocellular system
identifies the feature in the focus of attention, the magnocel- References
lular system identifies, according to this hypothesis, the global
structure in which this feature is embedded. Such a view of the Abadi RV, Kulikowski JJ, Meudell P. Visual performance in a case of visual
agnosia. In: Hoff MW, Hohn E editors. Functional recovery from brain
magnocellular system has been used to explain dyslexia as an damage. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1981; 27586.
impairment of the magnocellular system (Livingstone et al., Adler A. Disintegration and restoration of optic recognition in visual agnosia:
1991; Sperling et al., 2003; Stein, 2003). AM was not only Analysis of a case. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 1944; 51: 24359.
Adler A. Course and outcome of visual agnosia. Journal of Nervous Mental
dyslexic, he also resembled dyslexic children in two other Diseases 1950; 111: 4151.
respects, which are considered as a sign for a magnocellular Arakawa K, Tobimatsu S, Tomoda H, Kira J, Kati M. The effect of spatial
deficit (Stein, 2003). First, he had problems in higher-order frequency on chromatic and achromatic steady-state visual evoked
potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology 1999; 110: 195964.
motion perception (see above) and second, he showed a left Barbur JL, Watson JDG, Frackowiak RSJ, Zeki S. Conscious visual perception
minineglect (Hari et al., 2001) as he was about 120 ms without V1. Brain 1993; 116: 1293302.
214 H. Hildebrandt et al.

Bartolomeo P, Bachoud-Levi AC, De Gelder B, Denes G, Dalla Barba G, Heywood CA, Cowey A, Newcombe F. On the role of parvocellar (P) and
Brugieres P, Degos JD. Multiple-domain dissociation between impaired magnocellular (M) pathways in cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 1994; 117:
visual perception and preserved mental imagery in a patient with bilateral 24554.
extrastriate lesions. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 23949. Hildebrandt H, Spang K, Ebke M. Case report: Visuo-spatial hemi-inattention
Benson DF, Greenberg JP. Visual form agnosia. Archives of Neurology 1969; following a cerebellar/brain stem bleeding. Neurocase 2002; 8: 32329.
20: 829. Kartsounis LD, Warrington EK. Failure of object recognition due to a
Biscaldi M, Fischer B, Aiple F. Saccadic eye movements of dyslexic and breakdown of figure-ground discrimination in a patient with normal acuity.
normal reading children. Perception 1994; 23: 4564. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29: 96980.
Biscaldi M, Gezeck St, Stuhr V. Poor saccadic control correlates with dyslexia. Kosslyn SM, Flynn RA, Amsterdam JB, Wang G. Components of high-level
Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 1189202. vision: A cognitive neuroscience analysis and accounts of neurological
Braddick O, Atkinson J, Wattam-Bell J. Normal and anomalous develop- syndromes. Cognition 1990; 34: 20377.
ment of visual motion processing: Motion coherence and dorsal-stream Kosslyn SM, Koenig O, Barrett A, Cave CB, Tang J, Gabrieli J. Evidence
vulnerability. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 176984. for two types of spatial representations: Hemispheric specialization for
Braun D, Petersen D, Scho nle P, Fahle M. Deficits and recovery of first- and categorical and coordinate relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
second-order motion perception in patiens with unilateral cortical lesions. Human Perception and Performance 1989; 15: 72335.
European Journal of Neuroscience 1998; 10: 211728. Livingstone MS, Hubel DH. Segregration of form, color, movement, and
Campion J, Latto R. Apperceptive agnosia due to carbon monoxide poisoning. depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 1988; 240: 7409.
An interpretation based on critical band masking from disseminated lesions. Livingstone MS, Rosen GD, Drislane FW, Galburda AM. Physiological and
Behavioral Brain Research 1985; 15: 22740. anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dys-
Cavanagh P, Mather G. Motion: The long and the short of it. Spatial Vision lexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 1991; 88:
1989; 4: 10329. 79437.
Chubb C, Sperling G. Drift balanced stimuli: A general basis for studying non- Milner AD, Perrett DI, Johnston RS, Benson PJ, Jordan TR, Heeley DW,
Fourier motgion perception. Journal of the Ophthamological Society of Bettucci D, Mortara F, Mutani R, Terazzi E, Davidson DLW. Perception and
America A. 1988; 5: 19862006. action in visual form agnosie. Brain 1991; 114: 40528.
Davidoff J, Warrington EK. A dissociation of shape discrimination and figure- Omtzigt D, Hendriks AW, Kolk HH. Evidence for magnocellular involvement in
ground perception in a patient with normal acuity. Neuropsychologia 1993; the identification of flanked letters. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40(12): 188190.
31: 8393. Peterhans E, Heydt Rvd. Mechanisms of contour perception in monkey
Eden GF, Van Meter JW, Rumsey JM, Maisog JM, Woods RP, Zeffiro TA. visual cortex. II. Contours bridging gaps. Journal of Neuroscience 1989; 9:
Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional 174963.
brain imaging. Nature 1996; 382: 669. Peterhans E, Heydt Rvd. Subjective contours bridging the gap between
Efron R. What is perception? Boston Studies of the Philosophy Sciences 1969; psychophysics and physiology. Trends in Neurosciences 1991; 14: 1129.
4: 13773. Robertson LC, Lamb MR. Neuropsychological contributions to theories of
Facoetti A, Paganoni P, Turatto M, Marzola V, Mascetti GG. Visual-spatial part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology 1991; 23: 299330.
attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex 2000; 36: 10923. Smith AT, Hess RF, Baker CL. Direction identification thresholds for second
Ferreira CT, Ceccaldi M, Giusiano B, Poncet M. Separate visual pathways order motion in central and peripheral vision. Journal of the Ophthalmo-
for perception of actions and objects: Evidence from a case of apperceptive logical Society of America A. 1994; 11: 50614.
agnosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1998; 65: Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for
3825. agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental
Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith ChD, Frackowiak RSJ. Neural Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 1980; 6: 174215.
mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of Sperling AJ, Lu Z, Manis FR, Seidenberg MS. Selective magnocellular deficits in
hierarchically organized visual stimuli. Brain 1997; 120: 177991. dyslexia: A phantom contour study. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 14229.
Fink GR, Marshall JC, Halligan PW, Dolan RJ. Hemispheric asymmetries in Spinelli D, Angelelli P, de Luca M, Burr DC. VEP in neglect patients have
global/local processing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsy- longer latencies for luminance but not for chromatic patterns. Neuroreport
chologia 1999; 37: 3140. 1996; 7: 8159.
Finney DJ. Probit Analysis. Cambridge University Press, 1962. Stein J. Visual motion sensitivity and reading. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:
Goodale MA, Milner AD. A. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford 178593.
University Press, 1995. Taylor MM, Creelman CD. PEST: Efficient estimates on probability functions.
Hari R, Renvall H, Tanskanen T. Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. Brain Journal of the Acoustical Society, Am. 1967; 41: 7827.
2001; 124: 137380. Tobimatsu S, Tomoda H, Kato M. Parvocellular and mognocellular contribu-
Heinze HJ, Hinrichs H, Scholz M, Burchert W, Mangun GR. Neural tions to visual evoked potentials: Stimulation with chromatic and achromatic
mechanisms of global and local processing: A combined PET and ERP gratings. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 1995; 134: 7382.
study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1998; 10: 48598. Zeki S. A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 1990; 113: 172177.

Potrebbero piacerti anche