Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/13812539

Distinguishing Optimism From Pessimism in


Older Adults: Is It More Important to Be
Optimistic or Not to Be Pessimistic?

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology January 1998


DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1345 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

225 157

4 authors, including:

Susan Robinson-Whelen Cheongtag Kim


Baylor College of Medicine Seoul National University
47 PUBLICATIONS 1,163 CITATIONS 18 PUBLICATIONS 982 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Cheongtag Kim on 02 January 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1997, Vol. 73, No. 6, 1345-1353 0022-3514/97/$3.00

Distinguishing Optimism From Pessimism in Older Adults:


Is It More Important to Be Optimistic or Not to Be Pessimistic?
Susan Robinson-Whelen, Cheongtag Kim, Robert C. MacCallum, and Janice K. Kiecolt-Glaser
Ohio State University

Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the Life Orientation Test (LOT) consisted of separate
Optimism and Pessimism factors among middle-aged and older adults. Although the two factors
were significantly negatively correlated among individuals facing a profound life challenge (i.e.,
caregiving), they were only weakly correlated among noncaregivers.Caregivers also expressed less
optimism than noncaregiversand showed a trend toward greater pessimism, suggestingthat life stress
may affect these dispositions. Pessimism,not optimism, uniquely predicted subsequentpsychological
and physical health; however, optimism and pessimism were equally predictive for stressed and
nonstressed samples. By exploring optimism and pessimism separately, researchers may better deter-
mine whether the beneficialeffects of optimismresult from thinking optimistically,avoidingpessimis-
tic thinking, or a combination of the two.

Research on dispositional optimism has increased rapidly in two factors probably reflect differences in item wording rather
the past decade because of evidence linking an optimistic out- than content.
look to psychological and physical well-being (see review in Although Scheier and Carver (1985) originally reported a
Scheier & Carver, 1992). Dispositional optimism has been re- strong negative correlation between the Optimism and Pessi-
lated to such diverse outcomes as success in an aftercare alcohol mism factors among their sample of undergraduates (r = - . 6 4 ) ,
treatment program (Strack, Carver, & Blaney, 1987), adjust- Plomin et al. (1992) found that optimism and pessimism were
ment to college (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992), resistance to post- uncorrelated in their sample of middle-aged and older Swedish
partum depression (Carver & Gaines, 1987), protection from adults (r = - . 0 2 ) . Mroczek, Spiro, Aldwin, Ozer, and Bosse
distress following a failed attempt at in vitro fertilization (Litt, (1993) reported a relatively modest correlation between the
Tennen, Affleck, & Klock, 1992), and adjustment following two subscales in their sample of older men (r = -.28). This
surgery for breast cancer (Carver et al., 1993). In a striking correlation, which was lower than typically reported in the liter-
demonstration of the benefits of optimism on health, Scheier et ature, led them to question whether optimism and pessimism
al. (1989) found that optimists recovered more quickly follow- may be more independent among older adults.
ing coronary artery bypass surgery and returned to a normal Scheier and Carver have only recently acknowledged that
life more rapidly than pessimists. there may be value in examining the two factors separately
All of the studies cited used the Life Orientation Test (LOT; (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994). However, they qualified this
Scheier & Carver, 1985), the most widely used measure of acknowledgment by adding that, although the negatively and
dispositional optimism, as a unidimensional measure of opti- positively worded scales might yield different results in some
mism. Scheier and Carver initially identified two factors on infrequent circumstances, explorations have generally failed to
the LOT (one containing positively worded items; the other demonstrate differential effects. Marshall, Wortman, Kusulas,
containing negatively worded items). Despite the two-factor Hervig, and Vickers (1992) were the first to argue that the two
structure of the measure, Scheier and Carver have preferred to factors on the LOT represented separate optimism and pessi-
treat the LOT as a unidimensional measure, suggesting that the mism constructs and that they correlated differently with other
measures. They reported that optimism was primarily associated
with extraversion and positive affect, pessimism with neuroti-
cism and negative affect.
Susan Robinson-Whelen and Janice K. Kiecolt-Glaser, Department Subsequent to this, other researchers have begun examining
of Psychiatry, Ohio State University; Cheongtag Kim and Robert C. the two subscales separately and have begun reporting differ-
MacCallum, Department of Psychology, Ohio State University. ences in their correlations with psychological and physical
A portion of these data was presented at the 103rdAnnual Convention health outcome variables. Mroczek et al. (1993) found that
of the American Psychological Association, New York, August 1995. optimism and pessimism each contributed unique variance to
This study was supported by Grants R37 MH42096 and RO1 MH50538
recent psychological well-being and physical illness among par-
from the National Institute of Mental Health. We thank the Central
Ohio Chapter of the Alzheimer's Disease Association and the Columbus ticipants of the Normative Aging Study. Lai (1994), examining
Chapter of the Parkinson's Disease Association for their support. the relation between optimism and pessimism and self-reported
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Susan physical symptoms among undergraduates in Hong Kong, re-
Robinson-Whelen, Department of Psychiatry, Ohio State University, ported that only the optimism items accounted for independent
1670 Upham Drive, Columbus, Ohio 43210. variance in symptom reports. In contrast, Schulz, Bookwala,
1345
1346 ROBINSON-WHELEN, KIM, MAcCALLUM, AND KIECOLT-GLASER

Knapp, Scheier, and Williamson (1996) recently reported that benefits from expecting good things to happen in the future or
pessimism, not optimism, predicted mortality among middle- from not expecting bad things.
aged cancer patients. Thus, the debate about whether or not Finally, Scheier and Carver (1993) have proposed that the
optimism represents one or two constructs has recently been LOT taps an enduring personality characteristic that "changes
replaced by a debate about the significance of optimism versus little with the vagaries of life" (p. 27). In this study we were
pessimism. Is it more important to be optimistic or to not be able to explore how a profound life stressor affects responses
pessimistic, or is it necessary to be both? to the LOT by comparing the factor means and factor structure
It has been suggested that optimism and pessimism may be of the LOT among a sample of individuals caring for a demented
particularly important when an individual is facing stressful family member with that of a sample of noncaregivers. We also
events (Adler & Matthews, 1994), threatening events (Taylor, examined the effect of recent negative life events on subsequent
1983), situations in which there is little personal control (Mar- optimism and pessimism in both samples.
shall & Lang, 1990), or situations that unfold or develop slowly To summarize, there were four primary goals of this study.
over time (Scheier, Weintraub, & Carver, 1986). Indeed, the First, the factor structure of the LOT was examined among
samples most commonly studied in optimism research have been middle-aged and older adults. Second, the influence of life stress
samples experiencing life-altering or life-threatening stressors on the LOT factor structure and factor means was examined.
(Carver et al., 1993; Curbow, Somerfield, Baker, Wingard, & This was accomplished by comparing family caregivers' re-
Legro, 1993; Fitzgerald, Tennen, Affleck, & Pransky, 1993; sponses to the LOT with those of noncaregiving control partici-
Friedman et al., 1992; Scheier et al., 1989; Schulz et al., 1996; pants and also by examining the effect of recent negative life
Stanton & Snider, 1993; Taylor et al., 1992; Zeidner & Ben- events on subsequent optimism and pessimism. Third, the ability
Zur, 1993). Despite the potential importance of the LOT for of optimism and pessimism to predict subsequent psychological
stressed populations, little research has examined whether indi- distress and self-reported physical health was examined. Finally,
viduals who are facing stressors and those who are not differ the ability of optimism and pessimism to predict psychological
on optimism or pessimism. Likewise, research has not addressed and physical health outcomes was compared across a sample of
the differential effect of optimism and pessimism on psychologi- chronically stressed individuals (i.e., caregivers) and a similar
cal or physical health outcomes among stressed and nonstressed sample of noncaregiving control participants.
populations.
Bromberger and Matthews (1996) recently examined the dif-
Method
ferential impact of optimism (unidimensional construct) on de-
pressive symptoms among a sample of middle-aged women. Participants
They reported that optimism was a better predictor of depressive
symptomatology among women who reported experiencing re- Participants were enlisted for a longitudinal study of caregiving stress
cent negative life events and ongoing problems than those who and health. Approximately half were providing care for a family member
did not. To examine this issue further, they conceptualized with a progressive, dementing illness; the other half were noncaregiving
community-residing adults. To be included in the caregiver sample, indi-
change in menopause status as a stressor and compared the
viduals had to be providing a minimum of 5 hr of care per week for
influence of unidimensional optimism on depressive symptoms a cognitively impaired relative. Caregiving participants were recruited
among individuals who were and those who were not experienc- through local diagnostic clinics, neurologists' referrals, local caregiver
ing a change in menopausal status, They later concluded that support programs, and the Alzheimer's Association newsletter. Noncare-
this was not an ideal test of the differential impact of optimism giving participants were recruited through local newspapers, senior citi-
on stressed versus nonstressed individuals because menopause zens' organizations, church groups, and university publications. Potential
did not, in fact, appear to increase women's vulnerability to comparison participants who reported any recent caregiving activities
stress. were excluded from the study.
In our study we had two samples differing greatly in life Each year, participants in the longitudinal study received a series of
stress (i.e., caregivers and noncaregivers), allowing us to ex- interviews and completed a battery of self-report measures. Only sub-
jects who completed the LOT during Years 1 and 3 of the longitudinal
plore whether optimism and pessimism are more important pre-
project were included in these analyses. The larger project includes
dictors of health and well-being among those experiencing a bereaved caregivers whose caregiving activities have ceased; however,
severe, chronic stressor and those who are not. Caring for a caregivers in this study were limited to those providing care for a relative
family member with a progressive dementia such as Alzheimer's both in Years 1 and 3. With these restrictions, the sample included 224
disease ( A D ) is an extraordinarily long-term, unpredictable, middle-aged and older adults (113 caregivers and 111 noncaregivers).
and uncontrollable life stressor (Light & Lebowitz, 1989). Opti- A majority of the caregivers (n = 80, 71%) and noncaregivers (n =
mism has been significantly related to mental health among 87, 78%) were women. Most were Caucasian (95%, caregivers; 92%,
caregivers for stroke patients (Tompkins, Schulz, & Rau, 1988) noncaregivers), and 68% had at least some college education. The mean
and cancer patients (Given et al., 1993) as well as AD patients annual family income was between $20,000 and $30,000. There were
(Hooker, Monahan, & Frazier, 1994; Hooker, Monahan, Shi- no significant differences between caregivers and noncaregivers in level
of education (t = 0.44) or income (t = 1.19), but the two groups
fren, & Hutchinson, 1992). Although providing evidence for
differed significantly in age (M = 56.00, SD = 13.84, caregivers; M =
the utility of the LOT in predicting caregivers' psychological 61.53, SD = 13.22, noncaregivers; t = -3.06).
well-being, they did not compare the predictive ability of the Caregivers were providing care for a spouse (n = 52), a parent (n
LOT in caregivers versus noncaregivers. These studies also used = 52), or some other relative (n = 9; e.g., mother-in-law). On average,
the LOT as a unidimensional measure of dispositional optimism, caregivers had been providing care for their relative for 53.33 months
making it unclear whether caregivers' psychological well-being (SD = 48.55) before joining the study, Caregivers were providing a
DISTINGUISHING OPTIMISM FROM PESSIMISM 1347

mean of 5.54 hr of care per day (SD = 5.36) for their impaired relative p < .001; and in Year 4: caregivers, M = 17.12, SD = 7.17; controls,
at Time 1 and 3.85 hr of care per day (SD = 4.35) at Time 3. The M = 12.30, SD = 6.19; t(181) = 12.59, p < .001.
caregiver sample included individuals caring for a relative in their own State anxiety. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y-1 ) (STAI;
home (Year 1, n = 62, 54.9%; Year 3, n = 38, 33.9%) as well as those Spieiberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) was used to
caring for relatives residing in nursing homes. Previous research has not assess state anxiety. This 20-item scale uses a 4-point response format
found a relationship between caregiver well-being and patient residence (1 = not at all to 4 = very much so) to assess how individuals feel at
(Dura, Stukenberg, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1990; Kiecolt-Glaser, Dura, the present time. Possible scores range from 20 to 80. Mean scores were
Speicher, Trask, & Glaser, 1991 ). 37.77 (SD = 12.79) and 29.52 (SD = 8.50) for caregivers and controls,
The previously described sample of 113 caregivers and 111 noncare- respectively, in Year 3, t( 181 ) = 17.18, p < .001, and were 35.17 (SD
givers was used to assess the LOT's factor structure and the influence = 10.80) and 30.54 (SD = 8.87) for caregivers and controls, respec-
of negative life events on subsequent levels of optimism and pessimism. tively, in Year 4, t( 181 ) = 9.98, p < .001. The internal consistency of
Data from 81 of the caregivers and 102 of the controls were used to the measure was .94 and .93 for Year 3 and Year 4, respectively.
explore the relation between Year 3 optimism, Year 3 pessimism, and Self-rated health. Self-reported health was assessed by using an
Year 4 measures of psychological distress and self-rated physical health. item from the OARS (Older Americans' Resources and Services ) Multi-
The sample size was reduced to 183 because of attrition between Year dimensional Functional Assessment Questionnaire ( F i l l e n b a u m &
3 and Year 4 (n = 24), missing outcome data at Time 4 (n = 3), and Smyer, 1981 ). Participants rated their health at each annual interview
the cessation of caregiving between Years 3 and 4 (n = 14) due to by using the following response options: excellent (3), good (2), fair
patient death. These two samples did not differ significantly in age, ( 1 ), orpoor (0). Both caregivers and controls rated their health as good
education, or income. in Year 3: caregivers, M = 1.96, SD = 0.52; controls, M = 2.30, SD =
0.61; t(181) = -3.14, p < .01 and Year 4: caregivers, M = 1.99, SD
= 0.67; controls, M = 2.27, SD = 0.66; t(181) = -2.29, p < .05.
Measures Negative affectivity. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PA-
NAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), a 20-item scale designed to
Optimism. The Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985) measure these two dimensions of emotional experience, was adminis-
was administered to participants in Years 1 and 3 of the longitudinal tered to participants as part of their annual interview beginning in Year
study. The measure was not administered in Year 2. It includes four 2. Participants were asked to rate the extent to which they experienced
positively worded items, four negatively worded items, and four filler each of the 20 mood states in the past week, using a 5-point scale. The
items. Participants indicate the extent to which they agreed with each Negative Affect subscale had adequate internal consistency (Cronbach's
statement, using a 5-point rating scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) coefficient alpha = .85 in Year 2 and .87 in Year 3 ) among the combined
to 4 (strongly agree). Cronbach's coefficient alpha for the optimism caregiver and control sample.
subscale was .80 and .74 for Years 1 and 3, respectively; internal consis- The Negative Affect subscale was used as a covariate in analyses
tency for the pessimism subscale was .82 and .81 for Years 1 and 3, predicting self-reported psychological and physical health to address
respectively. Mean optimism scores were 10.29 (SD = 3.29) and 10.47 recent criticism that optimism is indistinguishable from other measures
(SD = 3.17) for caregivers in Years 1 and 3, respectively; for controls, of neuroticism or negative affectivity (Smith, Pope, Rhodewalt, & Poul-
mean optimism scores were 11.50 (SD = 2.79) and 11.43 (SD = 2.68). ton, 1989). Because the version of the PANAS administered in this
Mean pessimism scores for caregivers were 5.39 (SD = 3.27) and 5.28 study is a measure of negative affect state rather than a trait measure
(SD = 3.28) in Years 1 and 3, respectively; for controls, they were 4.96 of negative affectivity, we decided to create a more traitlike measure by
(SD = 3.04) and 4.46 (SD = 3.04). allowing Year 2 and Year 3 negative affect to load on a single latent
Negative life events. Negative life event s, assessed every 3 months variable. By extracting that which is consistent and stable in the measure
by means of phone interviews, were summed across the 12-month period across 2 years of study, we were able to create a more suitable measure
preceding the Year 3 annual interview. The life event checklist used in of negative affectivity.
the study was a 28-item version of the Geriatric Social Readjustment For a summary of the measures used in the analyses and the year
Rating Scale (GSRRS; Amster & Krauss, 1974). The shortened version those measures were administered, see Table 1.
of the scale used in this study excluded items that might reflect changes
in mental or physical health (e.g., changes in sleeping or eating habits).
Results
Depression. The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS; Hamil-
ton, 1967) provides a 24-item interviewer-rated measure of depressive F a c t o r Structure o f the L O T A m o n g O l d e r A d u l t s
symptoms experienced in the previous week. The HDRS was adminis-
tered by advanced clinical psychology graduate students or clinical psy- Multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using
chology postdoctoral fellows as part of participants' annual assessments. L I S R E L 8 ( J S r e s k o g & S/Srbom, 1 9 9 4 ) w a s c o n d u c t e d o n the
Interrater reliability, calculated on 10% of all interviews in Year 4 by
using audiotapes, was .83. Cronbach's coefficient alpha was .82 and .85
for Year 3 and Year 4, respectively. Caregivers reported more depression
than controls both at Year 3: caregivers, M = 6.41, SD = 5.38; controls, Table 1
M = 3.22, SD = 3.57; t(181) = 10.26, p < .001; and at Year 4: Measures Administered, by Year of Study
caregivers, M = 6.23, SD = 5.57; controls, M = 2.55, SD = 3.52;
t(181) = 11.77, p < .001. Year 1 Year 2 Year 3 Year 4
Perceived stress. Beginning in Year 3, the 10-item Perceived Stress
Scale (PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) was used to mea- LOT Negative Life Events LOT Depression
sure the extent to which individuals perceived recent life circumstances PANAS PANAS Depression
Anxiety Anxiety
as stressful (i.e., unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overloading). Parti-
Stress Stress
cipants rated each of 10 statements on a 5-point scale (0 = never to 4 Health Health
= very often). The coefficient alpha was .91 for both Years 3 and 4.
Caregivers reported higher levels of stress both in Year 3: caregivers, M Note. LOT = Life Orientation Test; PANAS = Positive and Negative
= 16.96, SD = 6.45; controls, M = 12.28, SD = 6.96; t ( 1 8 1 ) = 12.14, Affect Schedule.
1348 ROBINSON-WHELEN, KIM, MAcCALLUM, AND KIECOLT-GLASER

caregiver and noncaregiver samples to assess the adequacy of a Table 3


one- versus two-factor model of the LOT. These analyses were Factor Loadings From the Multigroup Confirmatory Factor
conducted separately on data from Year 1 and Year 3. Analyses for the Two-Factor Model of the Life
The adequacy of fit of the one- and two-factor models was Orientation Test (N = 224)
assessed by using the following goodness-of-fit indexes: the
n o n n o r m e d fit index (NNFI; Bentler & Bonett, 1980); the com- Year 1 Year 3
parative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1988); and the root-mean- Item Optimism Pessimism Optimism Pessimism
squared error of approximation ( R M S E A ) , which was proposed
by Steiger and Lind ( 1 9 8 0 ) and has been further discussed by 1 .55 .49
Browne and Cudeck ( 1 9 9 3 ) . Values of .90 or higher on the 3 .68 .61
4 .83 .71
NNFI and CFI indicate good fit. In contrast, the R M S E A be-
5 .80 .84
comes smaller as fit improves. Values of less than .05 on the 8 .71 .74
R M S E A indicate very good fit, values less than .08 indicate 9 .67 .69
reasonable fit, and values greater than .10 indicate poor fit 11 .77 .77
( B r o w n e & Cudeck, 1993). Results of the conventional chi- 12" .52 .53
square test of the hypothesis that a model holds exactly in the Note. All factor loadings are significant at the p < .05 level.
population will also be reported. Less attention should be given
to this test of fit because models of this type are, at best, a close
approximation of the real world.
In the two-factor model tested, positively and negatively 130.47, p < .001. Thus, a two-factor solution provided a sig-
worded items loaded on different factors, and the correlation nificantly better fit to the data than a one-factor solution. The
between the two factors was freely estimated. Specifying a factor loadings for the two-factor model are presented in Ta-
model in which factor loadings were constrained to be equal ble 3.
between groups but factor intercorrelations were allowed to Although optimism and pessimism represent separate factors
differ by group provided a good fit for the data from Year 1, among both groups, the correlation between optimism and pessi-
X2(46, N = 224) = 64.82, p < .05; R M S E A = .04; NNFI = m i s m differed for caregivers and noncaregivers. Because of this
.96; CFI = .97. Analysis of Year 3 data produced a fairly similar difference, the fit of the two-factor model was significantly
pattern of results, X2(46, N = 224) = 99.24, p < .01; R M S E A worsened when the association between the factors was con-
= .07; NNFI = .89; CFI = .91. strained to be equivalent across groups: Year 1, ~(~2iff( 1, N =
A one-factor model was found to fit poorly at both Years 1 224) = 11.31, p < .01; Year 3, X,]iff(1, N = 224) = 9.98, p <
and 3 (see Table 2). Comparison of the one- and two-factor .01. Optimism and pessimism were more strongly correlated
models by chi-square using difference tests showed the latter to among caregivers than noncaregivers both at Year 1 and Year
fit significantly better at both Years 1 and 3: Year 1, X~ee(2, N 3. A fairly weak correlation was observed between optimism
= 224) = 196.09, p < .001; Year 3, Xd2iff(2, N = 224) = and pessimism among the noncaregiver sample in Years 1 and
3 (Year 1, r = - . 2 3 , z = - 2 . 1 4 ; Year 3, r = - . 2 6 , z = - 2 . 3 1 ).
In contrast, a strong negative correlation between the two factors
Table 2 was found among family caregivers (Year 1, r = - . 6 1 , z =
Comparison of One- and Two-Factor Multigroup - 7 . 9 7 ; Year 3, r = - . 6 3 , z = - 8 . 1 4 ) .
Confirmatory Factor Analyses of the Life Orientation Test
Fit indexes 1 factor 2 factors The Effect of Life Stressors on the LOT

Year 1 In addition to testing for significant differences between care-


givers and controls on factor loadings and factor intercorrela-
Xz 260.91 64.82 tions, we were also able to test group differences in mean levels
df 48 46
of latent optimism and pessimism. The two groups had signifi-
p .00 .04
RMSEA .14 .04 cantly different factor means, as evidenced by the decrease in
NNFI .61 .96 fit when factor means were constrained to be equivalent across
CFI .67 .97 groups: Year 1, Xa2iff(2, N = 224) = 7.37, p < .05; Year 3,
X,]iff(2, N = 224) = 7.00, p < .05. Family caregivers were
Year 3
significantly less optimistic than noncaregivers in Year 1 (z =
X2 229.71 99.24 2.65); in Year 3, they continued to be less optimistic (z = 2.36)
df 48 46 and also tended to be more pessimistic (z = - 1.91 ). These data
p .00 .00
RMSEA .13 .07 suggest that extreme negative life events, such as caring for a
NNFI .63 .89 relative with a progressive dementia, may affect dispositional
CFI .68 .91 optimism and pessimism.
To examine further the idea that negative life events may
Note. For Year 1, X2iff(2, N = 224) = 196.09, p < .001. For Year 3,
X,]~ff(2, N = 224) = 130.47, p < .001. RMSEA = root-mean-squared affect optimism and pessimism, multigroup structural equation
error of approximation; NNFI = nonnormed fit index; CFI = compara- modeling ( S E M ) was used to assess the stability of optimism
tive fit index. and pessimism over the 2-year period and to examine the contri-
DISTINGUISHING OPTIMISM FROM PESSIMISM 1349

bution of negative life events on subsequent optimism and pessi- these variables. Because of criticism that the LOT is merely
mism among caregivers and controls. A model was specified in another measure of neuroticism (Smith et al., 1989), negative
which Year 1 latent optimism influenced Year 3 optimism and affectivity was included in the models so that the contribution
Year 1 latent pessimism influenced Year 3 pessimism. Factor of Year 3 optimism and pessimism, independent of negative
means were included in the model so that differences between affectivity, could be assessed. This was judged to be particularly
Year 1 and Year 3 latent optimism and pessimism could be important, given our evaluation of pessimism as a separate
tested. The influence of negative life events occurring between construct.
Years 2 and 3 on subsequent Year 3 optimism and pessimism For each of the measured outcome variables (i.e., anxiety,
was also estimated in this model. Factor loadings were con- depression, perceived stress, and self-reported health), a series
strained to be equal across groups, but the intercorrelations of multisample analyses were conducted to assess the similarity
among optimism, pessimism, and negative life events and the between caregivers and noncaregivers in these models (see Fig-
path coefficients were free to vary for caregivers and noncaregiv- ures 1 - 4 ) . t On the basis of results of our multisample confir-
ers. This model provided a good fit to the data, X2(236, N = matory factor analyses, models were specified with equivalent
224) = 371.54, p < .01; R M S E A = .05; NNFI = .90; CFI optimism and pessimism factor loadings across groups. The
= .91. relationships among predictor variables (e.g., latent optimism,
In this model, Year 1 optimism accounted for 69% and 62% latent pessimism, negative affectivity, and Year 3 measured vari-
of the variance in Year 3 optimism for caregivers and noncare- ables) were each tested to determine whether they differed for
givers, respectively. In a similar vein, Year 1 pessimism ac- caregivers versus controls. Separate estimates for caregivers and
counted for 70% and 79% of the variance in Year 3 pessimism controls were retained only when the chi-square difference test
for caregivers and noncaregivers, respectively. Thus, optimism indicated a significant decrease in fit when equivalence con-
and pessimism appear to be fairly stable over a 2-year period straints were imposed. There were no group differences in the
in both samples. Stability was further supported by the similarity factor variances of Year 3 optimism, Year 3 pessimism, and
in factor means across the 2 years of study. Chi-square difference negative affectivity; however, the variances of Year 3 anxiety,
tests revealed that models constraining factor means to be equiv- depression, and perceived stress were different between groups.
alent across Years 1 and 3 did not result in a significant reduction The correlations in Figures 1 - 3 are based on the common metric
in fit: optimism, Xdiff
2 (2, N = 224) = 4.53, p > .05; pessimism, standardized solution.
X2iff(2, N = 224) = 5.53, p > .05. Models were initially specified with path coefficients esti-
Negative life events during the 12 months preceding the Year mated separately for caregivers and controls. When path coeffi-
3 assessment did, however, affect Year 3 optimism and pessi- cients were constrained to be equivalent across groups, there
mism after partialing out the influence of Year 1 optimism and was no significant reduction in the fit of the four outcome mod-
pessimism. Life events had a positive influence on subsequent els. Chi-square difference tests comparing constrained models
pessimism (gamma = .27, z = 3.04) and had a tendency to with models in which path coefficients were estimated indepen-
negatively influence subsequent optimism (gamma = - . 1 6 , z dently for the two samples were all nonsignificant: anxiety,
2
= - 1 . 7 1 ) among noncaregivers. Negative life events did not Xaiff(4, N = 183) = 2.06, p > .05; depression, X d i2f f (4, N =
significantly predict Year 3 pessimism (gamma = .01, z = 0.09) 183) = 4.17, p > .05; perceived stress, X2~rf(4, N = 183) =
or optimism (gamma = - . 0 5 , z = - 0 . 7 2 ) among caregivers. 2.61, p > .05; and self-rated health, X~iff(4, N = 183) = 1.01,
These data suggest that negative life events can, at times, influ- p > .05. The models with path coefficients constrained are
ence the otherwise stable dispositions of optimism and preferred because they result in a more parsimonious solution
pessimism. without a significant loss of fit. The fit of the models with
Although caregivers and noncaregivers appear to differ in the constrained path coefficients were generally adequate, with fit
extent to which recent negative life events influence their levels indices ranging from .05 to .06 on the RMSEA, .88 to .91 on
of optimism and pessimism, placing equality constraints on the NNFI, and .90 to .93 on the CFI (see Table 4).
these paths across groups did not result in a significant decrease Although these models included relatively few potential pre-
in fit. Thus, the differences between these groups are not statisti- dictors of outcome, they accounted for a substantial proportion
cally significant. In addition, when equality constraints were of the variance in Year 4 outcome variables. The variance ac-
imposed across groups, the influence of negative life events on counted for by the models ranged from .28 for anxiety among
subsequent optimism and pessimism was no longer significant noncaregivers to .56 for perceived stress among caregivers. The
(optimism, gamma = - . 0 6 , z = - 0 . 9 1 ; pessimism, gamma = amount of variance in Year 4 outcome accounted for by these
.01, z = 0.10). models was similar for caregivers and noncaregivers (see Fig-
ures 1 - 4 ) . This similarity, combined with the equivalence of
Optimism and Pessimism as Prospective Predictors of path coefficients across the two groups, suggests that the pre-
Outcome
SEM was used to examine the influence of Year 3 optimism
We attempted to analyze our data using one latent outcome variable
and pessimism on Year 4 psychological and physical health
consisting of all of our measures of well-being (i.e., depression, per-
among caregivers and noncaregivers. Year 3 psychological and ceived stress, anxiety, and self-reported health), but these variables were
physical health variables w~re included in the model to better not sufficiently correlated (rs ranged from .21 to .62). As a result, the
assess the ability of optimism and pessimism to prospectively single latent variable model provided an inadequate fit to the data,
predict subsequent outcomes independent of initial levels of X2(344, N = 183) = 728.34, RMSEA = .80, NNFI = .74, CFI = .77.
1350 ROBINSON-WHELEN, KIM, MAcCALLUM, AND KIECOLT-GLASER

Year 3 Year 4 Year 3 Year 4

~(04) (-.10) Perceived


(-.lr!/-.,
\
\
\1
"1
.3,~.so
" I ~(.,0).I~
l J
I
Anxiety

R==.331.35 \,q Stress

R==.501.56

Figure 1. Multisample structural equation model: Influence of Year 3 Figure 3. Multisample structural equation model: Influence of Year 3
optimism, pessimism, anxiety, and negative affectivity on Year 4 anxiety. optimism, pessimism, perceived stress, and negative affectivity on Year
q~vo parameter estimates are presented when caregivers and controls 4 perceived stress. Two parameter estimates are presented when caregiv-
differed significantly. When two estimates are presented, values on the ers and controls differed significantly. When two estimates are presented,
left represent estimates for controls; values on the right, caregivers. values on the left represent estimates for controls; values on the right,
Factor variances for Year 3 anxiety, which are standardized by a common caregivers. Factor variances for Year 3 perceived stress, which are stan-
metric, are 1.36 and 0.72 for caregivers and controls, respectively. Values dardized by a common metric, are 0.91 and 1.07 for caregivers and
in parentheses are nonsignificant; all other values are significant at or controls, respectively. Values in parentheses are nonsignificant; all other
beyond the p < .05 level. Circles represent latent variables with multiple values are significant at or beyond the p < .05 level. Circles represent
indicators; rectangles represent measured variables. latent variables with multiple indicators; rectangles represent measured
variables.

dictive utility o f o p t i m i s m and p e s s i m i s m does not differ sub-


stantially for caregivers and noncaregivers.
Year 3 Year 4 O f p r i m a r y interest in the m o d e l s was the contribution o f
latent o p t i m i s m and p e s s i m i s m on Year 4 o u t c o m e measures.
Year 3 p e s s i m i s m significantly influenced Year 4 anxiety

~(,10)
( g a m m a = .17, z = 2 . 0 5 ) , perceived stress ( g a m m a = .20, z
= 2 . 5 7 ) , and self-rated health ( g a m m a = - . 1 6 , z = - 2 . 1 0 )
i n d e p e n d e n t o f the other variables in the m o d e l The only Year
4 o u t c o m e variable that p e s s i m i s m failed to significantly predict
was d e p r e s s i o n ( g a m m a = .05, z = 0 . 6 8 ) . In contrast, Year 3
latent o p t i m i s m did not make an i n d e p e n d e n t contribution to
any o f the Year 4 o u t c o m e m e a s u r e s ( a b s o l u t e g a m m a s ranged
Depression
f r o m .04 to .10). 2 The p e s s i m i s m path coefficients were not,
/~:.411.28 however, significantly stronger than the o p t i m i s m path coeffi-
~ -- -, ' ( . ~ 3 ~ cients. W h e n equality constraints were i m p o s e d on these path
coefficients, there was not a statistically significant reduction in
fit in any o f the m o d e l s , Xd2iffs(1, N = 183) = 2.46, 0.45, 0.16,
and 0.04, all p s > .05, for anxiety, d e p r e s s i o n , perceived stress,
and health, respectively.
W h e n w e e x a m i n e d the relationship b e t w e e n Year 3 o p t i m i s m
Figure 2. Multisample structural equation model: Influence of Year 3
optimism, pessimism, depression, and negative affectivity on Year 4
depression. Two parameter estimates are presented when caregivers and 2 When Year 3 optimism was the sole predictor in the model, optimism
controls differed significantly. When two estimates are presented, values significantly predicted Year 4 depression, perceived stress, anxiety, and
on the left represent estimates for controls; values on the right, caregiv- self-reported health. Optimism remained a significant predictor of Year
ers. Factor variances for Year 3 depression, which are standardized by 4 perceived stress (z = -2.13 ) and health (z = 2.05 ) (but not depression
a common metric, are 0.74 and 1.32 for caregivers and controls, respec- and anxiety) after negative affectivity and Year 3 premeasures were
tively. Values in parentheses are nonsignificant; all other values are sig- added to the model. Optimism's ability to predict subsequent perceived
nificant at or beyond the p < .05 level. Circles represent latent variables stress and self-reported health dropped out only after pessimism was
with multiple indicators; rectangles represent measured variables. added to the model.
DISTINGUISHING OPTIMISM FROM PESSIMISM 1351

and pessimism and Year 3 measures of psychological and physi- Table 4


cal health (see Figures 1 - 4 ) , optimism and pessimism were Fit Indexes f o r Models Predicting Year 4 Anxiety, Depression,
both correlated with these measures in Year 3. These relation- Perceived Stress, and Self-Reported Health (N = 183)
ships were based on simple correlations; the relationships be-
tween optimism and pessimism and subsequent outcomes in our Fit index Anxiety Depression Stress Health
SEM analyses were based on regression weights that partial out X2 157.13 171.16 167.86 177.04
the effects of other predictors in the model. Although we did df 107 109 110 111
not find group differences in the influence of optimism and p .00 .00 .00 .00
pessimism on subsequent psychological and physical health in RMSEA .05 .06 .05 .06
NNFI .91 .89 .91 .88
our SEM analyses, differences between caregivers and noncare- CFI .93 .91 .93 .90
givers in the relationship between optimism and outcome vari-
ables did emerge when simple correlations were examined. Spe- Note. RMSEA = root-mean-squarederror of approximation;NNFI =
cifically, Year 3 optimism was more highly correlated with Year nonnormed fit index; CFI = comparative fit index.
3 anxiety, Year 3 depression, and negative affectivity among
caregivers than noncaregivers.
Concerns about the bidimensionality of the LOT apply to the
Discussion newly developed LOT-R as well. Although Scheier et al. (1994)
reported a cleaner one-factor solution on the LOT-R, their con-
Among caregiving as well as noncaregiving middle-aged and firro~atory factor analysis on data from young college students
older adults, the LOT consisted of two separate Optimism and still revealed that a two-factor solution provided a better fit to
Pessimism factors. Results of multigroup confirmatory factor their data than a one-factor model.
analyses revealed that a two-factor model provided a signifi- Caregiving and noncaregiving participants did not differ in
cantly better fit than a single-factor model. In fact, the two factor structure on the LOT; however, other important differ-
factors were only weakly correlated among noncaregiving mid- ences did emerge. Although optimism and pessimism were
die-aged and older adults. This is consistent with the results of largely independent among control participants, optimism was
Plomin et al. (1992) and Mroczek et al. (1993), who suggested significantly negatively correlated with pessimism among indi-
that optimism and pessimism may be more independent among viduals experiencing a profound life stressor. Mirroring the rela-
older adults. It is also consistent with literature on adult cogni- tionship with pessimism, optimism also correlated more
tive development that suggests that youthful thinkers often ap- strongly with negative affectivity and concurrent levels of anxi-
proach problems more dualistically and accept a more black- ety and depression among caregivers than among controls.
versus-white view of reality than older adults (Labouvie-Vief, These differences suggest that optimism may not be as pure
1992). and independent a measure of positive expectations about the
future among stressed individuals. Individuals experiencing an
extreme life stressor appear to be less able to perceive that both
Year 3 Year 4 good and bad things can be expected for the future. Individuals
experiencing a severe stressor may respond to the LOT to some
extent with their specific stressor in mind. Thus, even though
optimism and pessimism may generally be independent, these
individuals may be more likely to report either positive or nega-
tive expectations for the future (i.e., they may be more likely
to expect that their current circumstances will either improve
or decline).

(-.13)1.49~ - . 1 6 ~ I
Sell-mt~l
Health I
Caregivers and noncaregivers also differed in mean levels of
optimism and pessimism. Chronically stressed participants were
- less optimistic than controls and showed trends toward greater
/~.44/.44 pessimism. Although optimism and pessimism were quite stable
(-.03) over 2 years among caregivers and noncaregivers, optimism and
pessimism appeared to be influenced by environmental circum-
stances and life experiences. This was evidenced not only by
the differences in factor means among caregivers and noncare-
givers but also by the finding that negative life events signifi-
cantly affected subsequent optimism and pessimism among non-
Figure 4. Multisample structural equation model: Influence of Year 3
caregiving participants. Negative life events did not, however,
optimism, pessimism, self-rated health, and negative affectivityon Year
4 self-rated health. Two parameter estimates are presented when caregiv- influence subsequent levels of optimism and pessimism among
ers and controls differed significantly.When two estimates are presented, caregivers. It may be that caregivers' outlook for the future had
values on the left represent estimates for controls; values on the right, already been altered by the challenge of caring for a demented
caregivers. Values in parentheses are nonsignificant;all other values are family member. Thus, additional life events, many relatively
significant at or beyond the p < .05 level. Circles represent latent vari- minor in comparison to the ongoing stress of caregiving, did
ables with multiple indicators; rectangles represent measured variables. not modify their expectations for the future. The differences
1352 ROBINSON-WHELEN, KIM, MAcCALLUM, AND KIECOLT-GLASER

between caregivers and controls in the influence of negative stressed individuals. Optimism among stressed adults is also
life events on optimism and pessimism were not statistically more highly correlated with negative affectivity, anxiety, and
significant, but this may have been the result of our less than depression, variables that Marshall et al. (1992) reported to be
optimal sample size. more highly correlated with pessimism than optimism. Of great-
Examining group differences in the ability of optimism and est interest, we found that pessimism, not optimism, was a
pessimism to predict subsequent outcomes, we did not find evi- prospective predictor of psychological and physical health out-
dence that optimism and pessimism were stronger predictors comes a year later. In addition, we found that optimism and
among stressed than nonstressed samples. There were no differ- pessimism were equally predictive among individuals experienc-
ences between the two groups in the ability of optimism and ing an extreme stressor and those who were not.
pessimism to prospectively predict anxiety, depression, per- These data suggest that there are benefits to exploring opti-
ceived stress, or self-reported health over a 1-year period. mism and pessimism separately among middle-aged and older
Among caregivers as well as controls, pessimism significantly adults. The differentiation of optimism and pessimism is not
predicted anxiety, perceived stress, and self-rated health a year purely academic and should no longer be relegated to footnotes
later. The influence of pessimism was independent of initial in investigations of the LOZ. We believe it would be valuable
anxiety, stress, and health and was independent of the influence to know whether the beneficial effects of optimism are best
of negative affectivity. attributed to thinking optimistically about the future, avoiding
In contrast to pessimism, optimism failed to predict any out- pessimistic expectations for the future, or a combination of the
come variable a year later. Although we found that only pessi- two. Results of this study tentatively suggest that pessimism
mism predicted outcome above and beyond the influence of may be the more crucial factor.
the other variables in the model, the differences between path
coefficients for optimism and pessimism were not statistically
significant. This may have been the result of our less than opti- References
mal sample size, given the multigroup analyses conducted. With
Adler, N., & Matthews, K. (1994). Health psychology: Why do some
larger samples, pessimism may prove to be a more salient pro-
people get sick and some stay well? Annual Review of Psychology,
spective predictor of psychological and physical health outcomes 45, 229-259.
than optimism. This speculation is consistent with the recent Amster, L. E., & Krauss, H. H. (1974). The relation between life crises
results of Schulz et al. (1996), who found that pessimism, and mental deterioration in old age. International Journal of Aging
not optimism, predicted mortality among middle-aged cancer and Human Development, 5, 51-55.
patients. The beneficial effects of optimism found in the past Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1992). Modeling cognitive adaptation:
may have been a function of pessimism rather than optimism, A longitudinal investigation of the impact of individual differences
and the widespread use of the LOT as a unidimensional measure and coping on college adjustment and performance. Journal of Per-
might have camouflaged such effects. sonality and Social Psychology, 63, 989-1003.
Bentler, P.M. (1988). Comparative fit indexes in structural models.
It is possible that optimism was not a significant predictor of
Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246.
subsequent outcomes in our study because of our choice of
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance test and goodness
outcome measures. Marshall et al. (1992), who reported that of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin,
pessimism correlated highly with neuroticism and negative af- 88, 588-696.
fect, also reported that optimism correlated highly with extraver- Bromberger, J. T., & Matthews, K. A. (1996). A longitudinal study of
sion and positive affect. We did not have measures of extraver- the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive
sion in our study, nor did we have positively laden outcome symptoms in middle-aged women. Psychology and Aging, 11, 207-
measures that could be used in our models. Thus, we cannot be 213.
certain that optimism might not have been a more significant Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing
predictor, had we examined positive outcomes. model fit. In K.A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), 7~sting structural
equation models (pp. 136-162). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
It is also possible that the LOT Optimism scale is less concep-
Carver, C. S., & Gaines, J. G. (1987). Optimism, pessimism, and post-
tually cohesive than the Pessimism scale. Scheier et al. (1994)
partum depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 449-462.
recently indicated that the LOT Optimism scale contains two Carver, C. S., Pozo, C., Harris, S. D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M. E, Rob-
items that reflect coping strategies rather than expectations. inson, D. S., Ketcham, A. S, Moffat, E L., Jr., & Clark, K. C. ( 1993 ).
They offered a revised measure that replaces these items with How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A study of
more expectancy-based items. Although a scale with less consis- women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and
tent item content might explain the differences in predictive Social Psychology, 65, 375-390.
ability of the two factors, this does not seem likely in light Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure
of the fact that optimism and pessimism were both related to of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-
psychological and physical health when simple correlations were 396.
Curbow, B., Somerfield, M. R., Baker, E, Wingard, J. R., & Legro, M. W.
examined. It is also inconsistent with our examination of the
(1993). Personal changes, dispositional optimism, and psychological
internal consistency of the subscales.
adjustment to bone marrow transplantation. Journal of Behavioral
To summarize, results of this study suggest that optimism and Medicine, 16, 423-443.
pessimism are separate constructs that are largely independent Dura, J. R., Stukenberg, K. W~, & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1990). Chronic
among nonstressed middle-aged and older adults. Although opti- stress and depressive disorders in adult children caring for demented
mism and pessimism are separate factors among stressed adults parents. Psychology and Aging, 6, 467-473.
as well, they are less independent among stressed than non- Fillenbaum, G. G., & Smyer, M. A. (1981). The development, validity,
DISTINGUISHING OPTIMISM FROM PESSIMISM 1353

and reliability of the OARS Multidimensional Functional Assessment Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancy on
Questionnaire. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 418-434. health. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.
Fitzgerald, T. E., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Pransky, G. S. (1993). The Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psycho-
relative importance of dispositional optimism and control appraisals logical and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical
in quality of life after coronary artery bypass surgery. Journal of update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201-228.
Behavioral Medicine, 16, 25-43. Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power of positive think-
Friedman, L. C., Nelson, D. V., Baer, P. E., Lane, M., Smith, E E., & ing: The benefits of being optimistic. Current Directions in Psycholog-
Dworkin, R.J. (1992). The relationship of dispositional optimism, ical Science, 2, 26-30.
daily life stress, and domestic environment to coping methods used Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing
by cancer patients. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15, 127-141. optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-
Given, C. W., Stommel, M., Given, B., Osuch, J., Kurtz, M. E., & Kurtz, esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Per-
J. C. (1993). The influence of cancer patients' symptoms and func- sonality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-1078.
tional states on patients' depression and family caregivers' reaction Scheier, M. E, Matthews, K. A., Owens, J. E, Magovern, G. J., Sr., Le-
and depression. Health Psychology, 12, 277-285. febvre, R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Dispositional
Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary de- optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The
pressive illness. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. Journal
278-296. of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1024-1040.
Hooker, K., Monahan, D. J., & Frazier, L. D. ( 1994, November). Person- Scheier, M. F., Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with
stress: Divergent strategies of optimists and pessimists. Journal of
ality of spouse caregivers: Relationships to mental health in two
disease groups. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1257-1264.
Schulz, R., Bookwala, J., Knapp, J. E., Scheier, M., & Williamson, G. M.
Gerontological Society of America, Atlanta, GA.
(1996). Pessimism, age, and cancer mortality. Psychology and Aging,
Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K., & Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental
11, 304-309.
and physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality.
Smith, T. W., Pope, M. K., Rhodewalt, F., & Poulton, J. L. (1989). Opti-
Psychology and Aging, 7, 367-375.
mism, neuroticism, coping, and symptom reports: An alternative inter-
J6reskog, K. G., & S6rbom, D. (1994). LISREL 8 user's guide. Chicago: pretation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and
Scientific Software. Social Psychology, 56, 640-648.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Dura, J. R., Speicher, C. E., Trask, O. J., & Glaser, Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs,
R. ( 1991 ). Spousal caregivers of dementia victims: Longitudinal G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form
changes in immunity and health. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 345- Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
362. Stanton, A. L., & Snider, P.R. (1993). Coping with a breast cancer
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1992). A neo-Piagetian perspective on adult cogni- diagnosis: A prospective study. Health Psychology, 12, 16-23.
tive development. In R. J. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically based tests for
development (pp. 197-228). New York: Cambridge University Press. the number of common factors. Paper presented at the annual meeting
Lai, J. C. L. (1994). Differential predictive power of the positively ver- of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.
sus the negatively worded items of the Life Orientation Test. Psycho- Strack, S., Carver, C. S., & Blaney, P. H. (1987). Predicting successful
logical Reports, 75, 1507-1515. completion of an aftercare program following treatment for alcohol-
Light, E., & Lebowitz, B.D. (1989). Alzheimer's disease treatment ism: The role of dispositional optimism. Journal of Personality and
and family stress: Directions for research. Rockville, MD: National Social Psychology, 53, 579-584.
Institute of Mental Health. Taylor, S.E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of
Litt, M. D., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Klock, S. (1992). Coping and cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173.
cognitive factors in adaptation to in vitro fertilization failure. Journal Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Aspinwall, L. G., Schneider, S. G., Rodri-
of Behavioral Medicine, 15, 171-187. guez, R., & Herbert, M. (1992). Optimism, coping, psychological
Marshall, G.N., & Lang, E.L. (1990). Optimism, self-mastery, and distress, and high-risk sexual behavior among men at risk for acquired
symptoms of depression in women professionals. Journal of Person- immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). Journal of Personality and
ality and Social Psychology, 59, 132-1.39. Social Psychology, 63, 460-473.
Tompkins, C. A., Schulz, R., & Ran, M. T. (1988). Post-stroke depres-
Marshall, G. N., Wortman, C. B., Kusulas, J. W., Hervig, L. K., & Vick-
sion in primary support persons: Predicting those at risk. Journal of
ers, R. R., Jr. (1992). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism: Rela-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 502-508.
tions to fundamental dimensions of mood and personality. Journal of
Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1067-1074.
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PA-
Mroczek, D. K., Spiro, A., III, Aldwin, C. M., Ozer, D. J., & Bosse, R.
NAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-
(1993). Construct validation of optimism and pessimism in older
1070.
men: Findings from the Normative Aging Study. Health Psychology,
Zeidner, M., & Ben-Zur, H. (1993). Coping with a national crisis: The
12, 406-409. Israeli experience with the threat of missile attacks. Personality and
Plomin, R., Scheier, M. E, Bergeman, C. S., Pedersen, N.L., Nessel- Individual Differences, 14, 209-224.
roade, J. R., & McClearn, G. E. (1992). Optimism, pessimism, and
mental health: A twin/adoption analysis. Personality and Individual Received April 7, 1995
Differences, 13, 921-930. Revision received December 11, 1996
Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health: Accepted D e c e m b e r 16, 1996

View publication stats

Potrebbero piacerti anche