Sei sulla pagina 1di 16

Linking Family Hardship to Children's Lives

Glen H. Elder, Jr.


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Tri Van Nguyen and Avshalom Caspi


Cornell University

ELDER, GLEN H., JR.; NGUYEN, TRI VAN; and GASPI, AVSHALOM. Linking Family Hardship to Chil-
dren's Lives. GHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1985, 56, 361-375. The impact of drastic income loss on chil-
dren is mediated by a number of family adaptations, including the shift toward more labor-intensive
households and altered relationships. Using newly developed codes on parenting behavior during
the Great Depression, this study investigates the role of parental behavior (rejecting, nonsupportive)
in linking economic hardship to children's lives in the Oakland Growth Study. The results extend
beyond those reported in Children of the Great Depression by showing that economic hardship
adversely influenced the psychosocial well-being of girls, but not boys, by increasing the rejecting
behavior of fathers. The parenting behavior of mothers did not vary significantly by income loss. In
addition, the rejecting influence of hard-pressed fathers was more pronounced in relation to less
attractive daughters, as judged by physical features. Attractive daughters were not likely to be
maltreated by their fathers, no matter how severe the economic pressure. These outcomes on family
mediation and conditional effects underscore the importance of viewing economic decline in rela-
tion to both the child's characteristics and parenting behavior. An understanding of the effects of
economic decline in children's lives requires knowledge of parent and child behavior within the
family and life course.

This study exemplifies two approaches to hard-pressed city of Oakland during the worst
understanding the relation between eco- years of the 1930s. Building on this earlier
nomic hardship and children's lives. The first analysis, the present study examines the role
approach specifies appropriate causal paths of parenting behavior in the process whereby
that mediate the relationship between eco- drastic economic change infiuenced chil-
nomic change and children's fimctioning. In dren's lives. Using the data archive of the
this study, we examine the role of parenting Oakland Growth Study, we focus on the pe-
behavior, especially fathers', in linking eco- riod from late childhood through early adoles-
nomic hardship to the social-affective behav- cence (1929-1936).
ior of children. The second approach iden- rp, i .i J Ac i.u l-,.
..n j-i- iU i J J. S.. i_ r Three hypotheses derived trom the liter-
tines conditions that moderate the nature of . * r -T 4. ^U t
.1 . J. i- 1 J -i,- I . ature on stress in lamifies orient the present
this mediational process, a conditional or in- . j f/-,ii j i . . i j j j . r . i .
f rr 4. T 4.t,- t, J uij study ol Oakland children and their families
teraction efi:ect. In this study, w e view child . iu i^ ^ TA T'U C ,. T
^. ., . i 4.. 1 J \ TiL- m the Great Depression. T h e first involves
attributes as potential moderators ol this pro- r -i j - ,. u j-u i
. f, 1 . , .. ^ family mediation. Here w e examine the role
cess, especially physical attractiveness. r u v. i, n r ^u '
' t- ' '^ ' ot parenting behavior, especially fathers, m
The structure of the present study ex- linking economic stress to children's lives,
tends the analytic fi-amework of Children of The second hypothesis concerns the process
the Great Depression (Elder, 1974), a longitu- of conditional variation, with emphasis on the
dinal study of 167 California children who role of children in shaping parental behavior
were bom in 1920-1921 and grew up in the in stressful times. The third hypothesis fo-

This study is based on a program of research on social change in the family and life course.
Support from the National Institute of Mental Health (grant MH-34172) is gratefully acknowledged
(Glen H. Elder, Jr., principal investigator). The most important support has come from Dr. Marjorie
Honzik, who is directing the parental behavior project at the Institute of Human Development,
Berkeley. Her pioneering conceptualization and measurement of parenting behavior in the institute
samples provided the opportunity for the present study. We are deeply grateful for her generosity
and guidance. We are also indebted to the Institute of Human Development at the University of
Galifomia, Berkeley, for permission to use the Oakland Growth data, and to Urie Bronfonbrenner for
helpful commentary on initial drafts of this manuscript. Requestsforreprints may be sent to the first
author at Garolina Population Genter, University of North Garolina at Ghapel Hill, University
Square East, Ghapel Hill, NG 27514-3997.
[Child Development, 1985, 56,361-375. 1985 by the Society for Research in Child-Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/85/5602-0008$01.00]
362 Child Development
cuses on gender differences in both pro- been "shaken for nearly six years by a catas-
cesses. trophe involving not only people's values but,
in the case of many, their very existence . . .
1. Family mediation.Economic set- the great knife of the Depression had cut
backs clearly make a difference in psychologi- down impartially through the entire popula-
cal functioning (e.g., Pearlin, Lieberman, tion, cleaving open the lives and hopes of rich
Menagham, & Mullan, 1981), but how is this as well as poor" (Lynd & Lynd, 1937, p. 295).
difference manifested in children's lives? To This observation applies also to families and
understand the impact of economic hcii"dship children in the San Francisco Bay region of
on children's lives requires knowledge of the California during the Great Depression (El-
adaptations chosen and played out by their der, 1974, 1979). But while some families ex-
parents. The adverse effects of stressful eco- perienced very heavy losses through unem-
nomic times are not necessarily exercised di- ployment, others were spared hard times
rectly. They may be produced indirectly altogether, and some even managed to benefit
through their disorganizing effects on family economically from the lower living costs of
relations. / Specifically, economic hardship the 1930s. This historical veiriation (deprived
may increase children's socioemotional dis- vs. nondeprived families) serves as a point of
tress and the risk of developmental impair- departure for examining the process by which
ment by increasing the rejecting behavior of children were infiuenced by economic hard-
parents, and especially that of fathers. ship. Indeed, the Great Depressi^m can be
2. Conditional effects.It is widely rec- viewed as a naturd^_field__experiinent that
ognized that characteristics of children elicit created an exQgenous^hange in the social and
modes of parental behavior (e.g.. Bell &c Har- economic situations of^inilies and altered
per, 1977). Such behavior is likely to be espe- the developmental context of children.
cially dependent on the attributes of children Using data on the Oakland Grovi^ Study
in stressful times. A child's aversive qualities (children bom between 1920 and 1921), El-
may heighten parental hostility under such der (1974, 1979) assessed the effects of in-
pressure, and conversely, the positive qual- come loss according to a conceptual model
ities of a child may elicit greater concern and that viewed the family and its socioeconomic
nurturance fi-om parents in hard-pressed and psychological adaptations to hardship as
households. In the present study, we examine the primary link between economic change,
the physical attractiveness of children (see El- on the one hand, and the life experience and
der, 1969) as one determinant of parenting be- personality of children on the other. Sudden
havior in hard economic times.f Specifically, income loss called for new forms of economic
adolescents who are less physically attractive maintenance among Oakland families that
may be at greatej risk from family hardship and altered the domestic and economic roles of
parental maltreatment than attractive adolescents. family members, shifiing responsibilities to
3. Sex variation.The final hypothesis mother and older children. Girls assumed
derives from a growing literature (e.g., Wer- greater responsibility within the household,
ner & Smith, 1982) that identifies early ado- and a good many worked at paid jobs, as did
lescence as a period of greater vulnerability boysfi-omhard-pressed families.
for girls than for boysiConsequently, both hy-
potheses, mediational and conditional, should aptations Eathers' loss of earnings and resulting ad-
in family maintenance (e.g., wom-
apply more to adolescent girls than to boys. en's entry into the labor market) increased the
To provide a rationale for these hypoth- relative power of mother, reduced the level
eses, we tum to the analytic model and re- and effectiveness of parental control (particu-
search task. Three issues are discussed. The larly in relation to boys), and diminished the
first involves the nature of the Depression attractiveness of father as a role model for
crisis for families and children; the second, children. Income loss also fostered social un-
the postulated role of father behavior in link- certainty and ambiguity conceming family
ing hard times to children's lives; and the standing and the status of members. This ac-
third, the protective infiuence of physical at- count (Elder, 1974), however, did not give ex-
tractiveness in family interaction and its rele- plicit attention to parenting behavior as a link
vance especially to adolescent girls. between hard economic times and children's
social and emotiond behavior. In this study,
Hard Times in Children's Lives: we are concerned primarily with testing this
The Analytie Model link.
When Robert Lynd returned to Mid- Some insights conceming the mediating
dletown in 1935, he found a city that had role of pEirenting behavior in hard-pressed
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, and Caspi 363
families have emerged from cognate studies Earlier analyses (Elder, 1974, chap. 6)
of families and children in the Depression era found that the Oakland girls, but not boys,
using the rich family and individual data were judged "less well-groomed" during
archive of the Berkeley Guidance Study early adolescence if they came from hard-
(Eichorn, Clausen, Haan, Honzik, & Mussen, pressed families in the 1930s, whether mid-
1981; Elder, Caspi, & Downey, in press; El- dle or working class. Some implications of
der, Liker, & Cross, 1984; Elder, Liker, & this handicap are manifested in junior high
Jaworski, 1984; Liker & Elder, 1983). Heavy school. According to self-reports, girls from
income loss between 1929 and 1933 in- deprived homes, middle and working class,
creased the emotional instability, tenseness, scored higher than the nondeprived on social
and explosiveness of fathers. This change en- unhappiness and on feelings of being socially
hanced the tendency for fathers to be punitive excluded from peer activities. Mothers also
and arbitrary in the discipline of their chil- perceived differences of this sort. Mothers
dren. By comparison, mothers did not be- from deprived families ranked their daughters
come more unstable under economic stress, higher on hurt feelings, worries, and self-
and income loss did not directly increase consciousness than mothers from nonde-
their punitive or arbitrary behavior. What ac- priyed families.
counts for this difference? One possible ex-
planation centers on the more personal nature To the extent that adolescent girls were
of income and job loss among men than especially vulnerable to family tension in the
among wives and mothers. Eamily misfortune Great Depression, this should be most appar-
was typically a result of men's losses. ent among the least attractive girls in the Oak-
land cohort. Unattractive girls generally think
The overall pattem of men's reactions to poorly of themselves (Sorell &c Nowack,
sudden economic loss conforms to a theory of 1981), and they rank low on self-confidence
force in regaining control over life circum- and assertiveness, qualities often characteris-
stances (Goode, 1971). Loss of control over tic of the victimized (Scherer & Shepherd,
one's life situation prompts efforts to regain 1982). In addition, physically unattractive
control. Force is one means to this end. All children are more often the recipients of
findingsfi-omthe Berkeley sample (e.g., Liker negative attributions from others. For ex-
& Elder, 1983) lend support to the mediating ample, adults assign more blame to physically
role of father's behavior in hard times (Hy- unattractive children, regardless of the facts
pothesis 1): heavy income loss adversely in- (Dion, 1972). In the Oakland cohort, adoles-
fiuenced young children (between 3 and 7 cent girls in the middle and working class
years old) by increasing the rejecting, nonsup- who ranked high on physical attractiveness
portive behavior of fathers. In the present (staff assessmentsee Elder, 1974, chap. 8)
study, we attempt to replicate this finding were rated higher than the less attractive on
with an older cohort of adolescents (from 11 to self-confidence, peer acceptance, and leader-
14 years old) from the Oakland Growth Study. ship. In the working class, these girls were
"less likely to form steady relationships with
In the OsJsland sample, hard economic boys in high school, and were generally more
times generated by the Great Depression guarded and selective in social friendships."
caught children in transition to the social
world of adolescence. The transition into ado- Overall, we assumed that the least attrac-
lescence is increasingly identified as a time of tive adolescents in the Oakland cohort were
psychological vulnerability for girls (e.g., more vulnerable to the rejecting behavior of
Wemer & Smith, 1982; see also Eme, 1979). father than attractive children (Hypothesis 2),
In their longitudinal research, Simmons and and that this risk should be greatest among
her colleagues (Simmons, Blyth, Van Cleave, adolescent girls. Several studies suggest that
& Bush, 1979) found self-image disturbances physical attractiveness is more salient for the
to be more common among girls than boys in adjustment of females than males (see Han-
early adolescence. These observations sug- sell, Spaaracino, & Ronchi, 1982). Evaluations
gest that early adolescent girls would be espe- along social dimensions are affected not only
cially vulnerable to parental maltreatment by attractiveness level, but also by sex, with
during hard economic times. Some evidence more differences appearing between attrac-
fi-om previous analyses of the Oakland study tive and unattractive females than between
supports this hypothesis. The psychological similarly grouped males (e.g., Bar-Tal & Saxe,
costs of the transition into adolescence during 1976). Hence, we have good reason to believe
the period of economic deprivation were es- that attractiveness was a powerful conditional
pecially strong for girls who lacked appropriate factor in the Depression family experience of
dress and material resources for social dating. adolescent girls (Hypothesis 3).
364 Child Development
Methods prevailing concepts of child care and develop-
ment in the 1930s. Mothers were considered
Sample the major figures of child rearing, and they
The Oakland Growth Study began in were interviewed on two occasions during
1931 when fifth-grade children from five the period of economic hardship: 1932 and
schools in the Northeastern sector of Oakland 1934. The fathers were not interviewed, al-
were selected for a projected longitudinal though interviews with mothers included
analysis of mental, social, and physical devel- questions about fathers' behavior. Another,
opment in a normal sample of boys and girls. less systematic source of information on par-
Selection was based on two criteria: willing- enting behavior was derived from staff obser-
ness to participate, and anticipated residential vations of parents during their visits to the
permanence in the area. This procedure pro- institute. Through the generosity of Marjorie
duced a sample of 167 children, 84 boys and Honzik at the Institute of Human Develop-
83 girls, who were studied continuously from ment, we borrowed selected parenting ratings
1932 to 1939. Bom in 1920-1921, the Oak- from her large parenting project with Mary
land children were well beyond the depen- Main for the years 1931-1934. The Oakland
dent stages of early childhood at the time of children were between 11 and 14.5 years old
maximum economic hardship in the early at this time.
1930s. They graduated from high school just
before World War 11, reaching the age of Building on more than 40 years of re-
majority after opportunities had improved search on parental behavior, Honzik and
through nationwide mobilization for war. Main (1982) developed 54 parenting scales;
22 capture stressful, negative behaviors, and
Measurement 32 tap supportive parental behaviors and at-
Economic deprivation.The median titudes. This approach is based on the prem-
family income for households in the sample ise that different parents may exhibit mixes of
was $3,179 in 1929, as reported in the first positive and negative behaviors. Each behav-
parent interviews, and all but a few of the ior is rated on a seven-point scale. The pres-
fathers were fully employed. Some 4 years ent analysis is based on only a single rating
later, in the trough of the Great Depression per item and case, although the completed
(the year 1933), the median family income hit coding on the Honzik-Main project will have
a low of $1,911. To characterize Depression at least two ratings per item. Two methods
hardships, we used a binary measure of eco- were used to calculate reliabilities from the
nomic loss or deprivation between 1929 and scores of two raters on a small subset of
1933 (0 = nondeprived; 1 = deprived). Tak- families during the initial stage of the Honzik-
ing into account the sharp decline in the cost Main project: (1) correlations between raters 1
of living in the San Francisco Bay area (about and 2 for the total protocol and for each item
25% in 1933) as well as the correlation be- of the overall scale, and (2) the average of the
tween income and asset loss, all families that difference between ratings assigned by the
lost at least 35% of their income (between two raters without regard for sign. Acceptable
1929 and 1933) were classified as econom- items in terms of interjudge agreement had an
ically deprived. Less deprived families were r of .45 or greater, since the Spearman-Brown
classified as nondeprived. Deprived working- correction of this coefficient is .62 with two
(max. N = 46) and middle-class families (max. judges. On the absolute difference analysis,
N = 49) lost 58% and 64% of their income, the acceptable difference was 1.50 for the
respectively. The corresponding figures for seven-point scales.
nondeprived families were 15% in the work-
ing class (max. ZV = 21) and 20% in the mid- Considering these data, we selected four
dle class (max. N = 40). Income loss repre- parent behavior ratings for the analysis, three
sents a general index of the loss of material on negative behavior (rejecting, exploiting,
resources. More specific measures of asset indifferent) and one on positive behavior
change are not available. However, prior re- (emotional support). The last rating was the
search indicates that loss of income generally only index of emotional support that was
coincided with the loss of family assets, from available for a substantive number of the
life insurance to furniture and other residen- mothers and fathers in the Oakland sample.
tial property. An extended discussion of the As broader measures of the family environ-
deprivation measure is provided in Children ment, we also selected four seven-point rat-
of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974, chap. ings, entitled marital compatibility, emotional
3). tone of home, family coordination, and child-
centered home. The parent ratings within
Parenting behavior.The data archive of each set of negative behaviors were highly
the Oakland Growth Study generally refiects
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, atid Caspi 365
correlated, thus permitting construction of the 1. Goal-oriented: "High aspirations," "produc-
following scales: tive," "gets things done," "self-defeating in relation
to goals," and "lacks personal meaning." Scores on
Rejecting (mother, father): "Negatively re- the latter two items were reflected. Average r: boys
sponsive to child," "rejecting," "neglecting," "not = .49; girls = .31. Reliability coefiScient (a): boys
dependable"; average r: mothers = .59; fathers = = .80; girls = .51.
.77. 2. Self-inadequacy: "Satisfied with self"
Exploitive (mother, father): "Overly demand- (reflected), "thin-skinned," "feels victimized,"
"brittle," and "fearful." Average r: boys = .58;
ing," "exploitive"; r: mothers = .66; fathers = .45.
girls = .62. Reliability coefficient (a): boys = .87;
Indifferent (mother, father): "Not responsivegirls = .88.
to child," "indifferent"; r: mothers = .68; fathers =
.73. 3. Social competence: "Arouses liking and ac-
ceptance," "gregarious," "social poise," "socially
These measures index parent attitudes toward perceptive," and "aloof (reflected). Average r:
the study child that became apparent in the laoys = .45; girls = .44. Reliability coefficient (a):
boys = .81; girls = .80.
course of interviews and observations. Thus,
for example, the rejecting index captures a
negative attitude of the parent toward the Self-other relations.A second ()-sort
child. (Block, 1971) for adolescents focused on inter-
personal behavior during the junior high
f The use of wife reports on father behav- school period (1933-1936). Differential asso-
ior could entail some bias in the measure- ciation with peers and adults, attitudes toward
V ments. For example, mothers may have pre- parents, and roles in the peer group are the
sented a consistently biased (favorable or concerns of this set of descriptive variables.
unfavorable) view of their husbands, thereby At least two judges rated each case, with a
infiating correlations among all family and mean interrater reliability of .75. We used two
parenting indicators. However, the correla- items that refer to attitudes toward father:
tions reported in Table 1 suggest that this was "perceives father as an attractive man," and
not the case in this sample. Had reliance on "feels father is a respected man as judged by
mothers' reports introduced this systematic social standards." Three additional items bear
bias, we would expect between-parent corre- on peer experience: "emphasizes being with
lations to be of the same magnitude as within- peers," "seeks reassurance from peers," and
parent correlations. But the former set of rela- "is dependent on peers." Each scale ranges
tionships is generally lower than within- from a score of 1 (extremely uncharacteristic)
parent correlations. to 9 (extremely characteristic).
Child Behavior Maternal ratings.A third source of in-
Aspects of a competent self.Utilizing formation on children's behavior comes from
the California Q-sort for the junior high school maternal reports in the 1936 interview.
period (1933-1936),^ we sought to charac- Mothers were asked to rate their children on a
terize adolescent functioning in terms of at set of behavioral attributes using a three-point
least three prominent elements of a compe- Likert scale to indicate frequency. Building
tent self (Coan, 1974; Smith, 1968): (1) goal upon prior analysis (Elder, 1974), we selected
directednessconcerted and satisfying ef- four scales: moody, easily hurt, calm, and
forts toward the achievement of goals; (2) a angry.
sense of self-worth and initiative, including
effective coping with life's problems; and (3) Attractiveness.Infonnation on chil-
social competencesocial skills, rewarding dren's appearance was obtained from obser-
associations, consideration of the needs of vational ratings made by staff members in a
others. Three item clusters bearing on these playground setting during semiannual visits
dimensions were identified from correlational to the Institute of Human Development
and factor analyses of ratings on boys and girls (1932-1936). The two or more staff members
(Elder, 1979): attending these sessions were well known to

' In the 100-item Galifomia ^-sort (Block, 1971), clinical judgments take into account the
relative salience of each item within aforcednormal distribution (nine categories) for each person.
The position of an item on the distribution indicates its salience as a descriptor of the individual
when compared to other items. AU ratingsforthe 100-item Galifomia Q-sort were made by profos-
sionally trained clinicians (at least two judges per case). The average composite interjudge reliability
for the Q-sort as a whole is .72.
366 Child Development
the children and related to them as interested, to the stronger connection between income
friendly adults. Observations made during the loss and father's behavior when compared to
free-play sessions were recorded afterward on mother's behavior and its correlates. Eco-
comment sheets, and children were rated on a nomic loss is moderately linked to father's re-
number of seven-point scales (the ratings are jecting (r = .24), supportive (r = .23), and
known as Free-Play Ratings). An overall in- indifferent (r = .17) behavior. By comparison,
dex of physical attractiveness was derived by none of these modes of parenting among
averaging scores on the following interrelated mothers is related to income loss (r's between
scales: attractive coloring, good features, good .01 and .02). The findings are consistent
physique, thin-to-fat, femininity/masculinity with previous analyses of the Berkeley Guid-
of behavior and physique, pleasing expres- ance cohort (birth years, 1928-1929; see El-
sion, and sex appeal. Interrater reliabilities for der, Liker, & Cross, 1984), and suggest that
the junior high items were above .80. This economic loss adversely infiuenced family
composite index has been used in prior re- frmctioning through its direct infiuence on
search (Elder, 1969, 1974) in the adolescent fathers' behavior. Economic hardship did not
experience and marital success of Oakland fe- generally affect mothers' parenting behavior.
males. Hence, in the analyses that follow we focus
on fathers as the primary link between De-
Data Analysis pression hardship and children's personal and
To investigate the infiuence of economic social functioning in adolescence.
deprivation on family relations and parenting
behavior, we begin with an examination of The correlations in Table 2 relate eco-
correlations between these variables. The nomic deprivation and father's rejecting be-
next step focuses on the relationship of eco- havior to all measures of adolescent function-
nomic hardship and parenting behavior to ing among boys and girls. Father's rejecting
measures of adolescent functioning during behavior receives primary attention through-
the junior high school years. With regard to out this analysis in order to permit compari-
the temporal sequence, measures of parenting sons with previous analyses of the younger
behavior (1931-1934) precede both the Q-sort Berkeley cohort that employed similar mea-
ratings (1933-1936) and maternal reports sures (Elder, Caspi, & Downey, in press; El-
(1936). Because we expected sex differences der, Liker, & Cross, 1984). Moreover, consis-
to emerge in these data, separate correlations tent with findings from the Berkeley sample,
cire reported for boys and girls. To test the preliminary analyses suggested that rejecting
mediation hypothesis, path-analytic models behavior is a critical variable linking eco-
are used to examine the role of father behav- nomic hardship to adolescent behavior. The
ior in linking economic hardship to adoles- social change and child behavior correlates of
cent fimctioning. Finally, to examine the con- father's rejecting behavior are consistently
ditional hypothesis of physical attractiveness, higher than those of father's indifferent, sup-
we compare the effect of^ economic hardship portive, and exploitive behavior.
on father's behavior in subgroups of attractive
and unattractive children and test for sig- Different pattems of association emerge
nificant differences in the regression slopes. among boys and girls (Table 2). In the case of
adolescent boys, economic hardship is linked
Before getting into the results, we stress to negative perceptions of father and to peer
again our primary objective, that of investigat- dependence, as well as to modes of psycho-
ing parental behavior in the Great Depression logical distress. Depression losses diminished
as a link between economic hardship and the perceived attractiveness of fathers (r =
adolescent functioning. This perspective .40 and .49), while strengthening the ap-
clearly rules out a number of potentially re- peal of peers. Boys from deprived homes
warding analyses from the behavioral conse- were more likely to emphasize being with
quences of parental behavior to the effects of peers (r = .27), to seek reassurance from
parental behavior in both deprived and non- friends (r = .27), and to express dependence
deprived families. In time we shall explore on the peer group (r = .26). According to
these domains as well. mother's reports, boys from deprived families
were also moodier (r = .28), more likely to
Results have their feelings easily hurt (r = .33), to
Economic Hardship, Parenting, and anger easily (r = .19), and to be less calm (r =
Adolescent Behavior .27). In theory, the rejecting behavior of
Correlations between economic hardship fathers should laear on each of these out-
and measures of parental behavior are sum- comes, from psychological distress and the
marized in Table 1. These correlations point devaluation of father to hurt feelings, anger.
2.74 1.10
.49
.76
00 l> : 00 cq rf in
Q r-iro( CO I1 oq CO.

2.05
1.92

2.00
1.78
00 in CO to ro pi 1
1

p 05 to ^ to

4.
4.
I-H 11
^ C O "*' - ^

cq ( > t- ( cq t- cq CO 00 in to
00 < 1 cq 1
>n o in m ro CO in

-.22 ]
-.20"
.10"
-.23
.17
.82

.28
iO CO

17" .02"
.52
00
^ ^ 1
1

to ro oq cq
^ 00 CO OJ oq
.37

.19
.36

ro in CO
CO to oq
td 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
r
.08"
,01"

m
in
ro -H
CO OJ
11 1 1 1
r\
.21"

.35

00 to 1> CO in 0 oq m
in to CO in to CD in ^
r 1' r 1" 1 1
.13"

,17"

0 cq
co_ cq cq
1' 1 r1 i
.13"
.21"

.11"
.41

ro cq 0 CO >* ro 1> CO
cq -^
CO M* in -*
1' r i 1 1
rr
.15"
.31
.36

cq CO to ro
in in in cq ^ to

1 1 1
r1 r1
z

o
U
368 Chad Development
TABLE 2
SOME BEHAVIORAL CORRELATES OF ECONOMIC DEPRIVATION AND FATHER'S REJECTING BEHAVIOR BY
SEX OF CHILD

CORRELATES OF:

Economic
MEANS BY Deprivation Father's Rejection
SEX OF CHILD (1929-1933) (1931-1934)
INDICATORS OF BEHAVIOR, Boys Cirls Boys Cirls Boys Cirls
JUNIOR HIGH PERIOD X X r r r r
Aspects of a competent self
(1933-1936) (range = 1-9):
Goal oriented 5.53 5.78 .14 -.26 .00 .41**
Self-inadequacy 5.50 5.12 .02 .23 .08 .38**
Social competence 5.27 6.27 .06 - .17 .06 .26 +
Self-other relations (1933-1936)
(1 = lo-w, 9 = high):
Father is attractive 5.78 5.64 - .40** - .06 .46** .36**
Father has social prestige 5.79 5.55 - .49** - .08 .50** .46**
Emphasizes being with peers 6.40 7.21 .27* .08 .14 .07
Seeks peer assurance 5.36 5.35 .27* .17 .20 .00
Dependent on peers 6.03 6.06 .26+ .18 .40* .01
Maternal reports (1936)
(1 = low, 3 = high):
Moody 1.53 1.54 .28* -.04 .01 .48**
Easily hurt 2.15 2.10 .33* .32* .13 ,30*
Calm 2.45 2.50 -.27+ -.03 ,03 .38*
Angry 1.89 1.75 .19+ .04 .15 .19
NOTE.The IV for boys ranges from 37 to 57; for girls from 38 to 55,
+ p < ,10,
*p< .05,
** p < .01,

and moodiness. However, with the exception ward boys during the early 1930s, t(119) =
of boys' negative perception of fathers, we 1.65, p = .10. In addition, the correlation be-
find little evidence of a relationship between tween economic hardship and father's reject-
father rejection and the behavior of adoles- ing behavior is higher for girls than boys (.25
cent boys in the hard-pressed 1930s. vs. .11, with social class partialed out). Judg-
ing from these differences, one might con-
In the case of adolescent girls, economic clude that the gender contrast stemmed from
hardship made little difference in how they adolescent girls who became more likely
felt about themselves or in how others de- targets of father's rejecting behavior and/or
scribed them. However, girls' behavior was from adolescent boys who were less accept-
strongly influenced by the rejecting behavior ing of such treatment than adolescent girls.
of father. The overall pattem is just the re- We know that hard times were conducive to
verse of findings on adolescent boys. The the prominence of wives and mothers in
daughters of rejecting fathers were less apt to household matters during the 1930s, but we
aspire toward high goals (r = .41), and they have no evidence from which to conclude
held a low opinion of themselves (r = .38, that such change increased the risk of daugh-
index of self-inadequacy). In addition, they ters from the frustrations and anger of a de-
were described by mothers as moodier (r = prived father.
.48), more easily slighted (r = .30), and less
calm (r = - .38). Whatever the causal mechanism, the
One possible explanation for this contrast findings reported in Table 2 are suggestive
between adolescent boys and girls is that of the hypothesized sex difference in family
father's rejecting behavior increased under mediation. Most of the correlations are consis-
hardship conditions only toward daughters. tent with a model in which family hardship
Indeed, mean scores suggest that fathers were increased the psychological distress of girls
slightly more rejecting toward girls than to- by increasing the rejecting behavior of
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, and Caspi 369
TABLE 3
THE MEDIATING ROLE OF FATHERS: DIRECT AND INDIRECT EFFECTS OF ECONOMIC
HARDSHIP ON CIRLS' SOCIAL-AFFECTIVE BEHAVIOR WITH SOCIAL CLASS, 1929,
CONTROLLED: REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS IN STANDARDIZED FORM

INDEPENDENT VARIABLES

Economic Father
Social Class Deprivation Rejecting
DEPENDENT VARIABLES

Mediating variable (1933-1934):


Father rejecting - .22 .25*
Child outcomes (1933-1936; 1936):
Father is attractive 16 .03 - .33**
Father has social prestige 56** .19 - .47**
Coal oriented 18 - .14 - .32*
Self-inadequacy 01 .03 .42*
Social competence .07 .00 .35*
Moody 14 - .18 .48**
Easily hurt -.08 .26+ .26 +
Calm 23 -.23 -.47**
Angry -.05 -.10 .23
NOTE,The coefficients presented in this table are the direct effects of the independent
variables on measures of girls' functioning. Indirect effects are computed by the product of the
coefEcients in the flrst row and the coefficients in the third column (see Fig, 1),
+ p < .10.
* p < ,05.
** p < .01.

fathers. However, more evidence on this rect effects of economic hardship on adoles-
causal sequence and mediated influence is cent girls apart from its influence on fathers?
needed in order to claim support for Hypoth-
eses 1 and 3. The standardized regression coefficients
from the path models are presented in Table
Father's Influence on Daughters: A 3. The findings suggest that economic hard-
Mediating Factor ship adversely affected the behavior of girls
We have discussed two causal paths be- by influencing the rejecting behavior of
tween family hardships and the behavior of fathers. Drastic income loss heightened the
girls. One involves a direct connection, while prospect of difficulties for girls in social and
the other operates through the rejecting be- psychological functioning, though only by en-
havior of fathers. For each outcome measure hancing the negative, rejecting tendencies of
on the total sample of adolescent girls, we fathers. By comparison, economic deprivation
constructed a path-analytic model in which had no significant effects on girls' functioning.
economic deprivation and socied class in 1929
represent exogenous variables; they are not For illustrative purposes. Figure 1 pre-
explained by any other factor in the model. sents one of the path models with a focus on
The measure of father's rejecting behavior moody behavior. Coefficients above the path
represents a mediating variable, linking eco- arrow are derived from the model tested for
nomic misfortune and its pressures to girls' adolescent girls; coefficients below the path
attitudes and behaviors.^ Each model ad- arrow refer to those derived firom the compa-
dressed two questions about the process by rable model tested for boys. These results, as
which girls were influenced by family hard- do the remaining path models, suggest that
ship: Does father's rejecting behavior actually family mediation (indirect effect hypothesis)
mediate (indirect effect) the influence of hard holds only for adolescent girls. The behavior
times in the family? And were there also di- of boys in the Great Depression was directly

^ Because we found no e-vidence for the influence of economic deprivation and fathers' behav-
ior on girls' peer group experiences, these measures were excluded from the analysis. This set of
path models was also tested on the sample of adolescent boys. However, as indicated by the zero-
order correlations in Table 2, no indirect efiFect could be detected because father's rejection does
not, in general, relate strongly to measures of boys' psychological functioning (see, e.g.. Fig. 1).
370 Child Development
R - . 1 3 / . 11 R - . 27/. 09
Economic
Oeprlvation

.16
Moody, 1936
.37

Social CIa88,
1929

p * . 10
p * .05
p * .01
FIG. 1.The mediating role of fathers: path analysis predicting adolescents' moody behavior. Regres-
sion coefficients are in standardized form. Coefficients above the path arrow are for girls; below the path
Eirrow for boys.

affected by economic hardship; this relation- hard times than were attractive girls (Hy-
ship was not mediated by the rejecting behav- pothesis 2).
ior of father. Were deprived fathers most
likely to hold rejecting attitudes toward rela- To investigate this conditional hy-
tively unattractive daughters? pothesis, we stratified the sample of Oakland
girls by a general index of physical attrac-
Daughters' Attractiveness: A tiveness for the junior high school years
Conditional Effect (1933-1936). In lieu of a pre-Depression mea-
The Depression experience was in large sure, we used an index that corresponds with
measure a family experience for the Oakland the worst years of the 1930s. We find no evi-
girls, but it may not have been the same, dence that income loss diminished girls' rank-
whether negative or not, for all girls who grew ing on physical attractiveness (p = .06, with
up in deprived families. Indeed, we have hy- social class controlled). For purposes of analy-
pothesized that relatively unattractive daugh- sis, girls were classified as "unattractive" if
ters were at greater risk of the rejecting be- their scores fell below the median; all other
havior of deprived fathers than were attractive girls were placed in the "attractive" category.
girls. We know that physical attractiveness For each group, attractive and unattractive,
made a difference in how the Oakland girls we estimated the effects of economic hard-
felt about themselves and in how they in- ship on father's behavior, with adjustments
teracted with others. An early study of the for initial social class. Three measxires of
Oakland cohort (Elder, 1969) found that rela- father behavior were included in the analysis:
tively attractive girls were more likely to hold rejecting, exploitive, and supportive. Paternal
a positive image of self and to marry well as indifference was excluded because it is so
defined by their husbands' social status at
mid-life. They were more popular among age- highly correlated with rejection. The results,
mates in secondary school and were per- along with t tests of the differences between
ceived as relatively feminine. Considering unstandardized regression coefficients in the
these and other observations in the literature two groups (Cohen & Cohen, 1975), are pre-
(e.g., Sorell & Nowack, 1981), we assumed sented in Table 4.^
that less attractive daughters would be more As stated in Hypothesis 2, economic de-
likely to experience paternal rejection during privation is more predictive of the nonsuppor-

^ These equations were EJSO estimated for the entire sample of girls by entering an interaction
term into each model. Estimates of the interaction effects in these models were similar to those
obtained in our stratifying procedure.
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, and Caspi 371
TABLE 4
DEPRIVATION EFFECTS ON FATHER'S BEHAVIOR TOWARD DAUGHTER BY HER PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS

ATTRACTIVE UNATTRACTIVE TESTS OF


{N = 29) {N = 26) INTERACTION
INDICATORS OF FATHER'S EFFECTS
BEHAVIOR, 1931-1934 r b p r b (t Ratios)
Rejecting behavior 07 .06 .06 .30 1.24 .33 1.81
Exploitive behavior^ - .30 - .67 - .46 .28 .37 .31 2.20*
Emotional support 10 .11 .08 - .43 -1.04 -.45 2.32*
" This model was estimated for unattractive girls with a subgroup N of 16.
* p < .05.

tive, rejecting, and exploitive behavior of fathers, when compared to other boys, but we
fathers when daughters were unattractive cannot explain this association by reference to
than when they were attractive. With adjust- the paternal behavior under study in this re-
ments for class origin, family hardship in- search. However, it is conceivable that the
creased fathers' rejecting behavior only when frustrations and harshness of deprived men
daughters ranked low on physical attrac- may have been focused on their less attractive
tiveness. The difference (p = .33 vs. .05 for sons, as measured by physical attributes. Us-
"attractive") is not statistically significant, al- ing the same measure of physical attrac-
though it conforms with the initial hypothesis. tiveness in junior high, we divided the boys at
More powerful support comes from the rat- the median into relatively attractive and un-
ings of exploitive and supportive behavior. If attractive groups. The results of this design
girls were unattractive, family hardship ac- showed no reliable group differences. We
centuated fathers' overly demanding, exploi- also explored the implications of differential
tive behavior (p = .31 vs. - .46, t = 2.20, p < height and rate of physical maturation among
.05). In addition, such hardship diminished boys for their relation with hard-pressed
father support only when daughters were fathers in the Depression. These results did
rated as unattractive (p = .45 vs. .08, t = not alter the picture. The conditional effect is
2.32, p < .05). Note that in two of the models restricted to adolescent girls.
(for exploitive behavior and emotional sup-
port. Table 4) the signs of the coefficients are Discussion
reversed. Attractive girls were not only in-
sulated from the psychological costs of eco- The three hypotheses of this study re-
nomic hcirdship; in some cases, economic ceive modest support from the analysis of
deprivation actually increased the suppor- father's behavior in hard times and especially
tive and benign parenting qualities of their in the lives of daughters.
fathers.*
1. The first issue concerns the role of par-
If we applied this approach to Oakland ent behavior in linking economic hardship
boys, would we find adverse paternal in- to children's lives. Economic loss among
fluences linked to their attractiveness? families in the Oakland cohort increased the
Whether rejecting, exploitive, or supportive, psychological distress and self-inadequacy of
father-son relations in the Oakland sample adolescents by increasing the risk of father's
have little to do with whether these men were rejecting behavior (Hypothesis 1). Such hard-
economically deprived. The sons of deprived ship did not influence matemal behavior. As
men were less likely to think well of their discussed below, the sequence from eco-

'' The strength of this conditional outcome is especially noteworthy when we consider data
limitations, in particular, the heavy reliance on wives' reports for information regarding the behavior
of fathers. The Oakland fathers were not interviewed, whereas their wives were interviewed twice
during the period of economic hardship, 1932 and 1934. Whatever the full implications of this data
source, they are likely to work against the attractiveness effect. According to a variety of studies of
neglect and abuse in the family (e.g., Kadushin & Martin, 1981), the evidence suggests that wives
and modiers are apt to underreport the paternal abuse of children. If so, the link between Income
loss and father maltreatment among relatively unattractive daughters would represent an underesti-
mate of the actual causal relation.
372 Child Development
nomic hardship to paternal rejection and ado- Cross, 1984), 7-8 years younger than their
lescent functioning varied according to gen- Oakland counterparts. The Berkeley children
der and physical attractiveness. were less than 2 years old when the economy
collapsed, and they remained exclusively
2. The second issue concerns the differ- within the family through the worst years of
ential role of father's rejecting behavior dur- that decade. In this sample, boys in early
ing hard times among adolescent boys and childhood were more likely than girls to be
girls. The causal sequence from economic affected by family turmoil and punitive par-
hardship to the rejecting behavior of fafhers enting in deprived families (Elder, 1979; see
and then to the social-affective behavior of also Elder, Caspi, & Van Nguyen, in press).
children emerged primarily among girls.
Family hardship heightened the peer orienta- 3. A third issue examined in this research
tion of boys and their devaluation of father, concerns child effects on parent behavior in
but the behavior of deprived fathers generally stressful times. Even with aversive families
had limited influence on boys' functioning. In and the most stressful experiences, some ado-
this adolescent sample, father mediation of lescents came through unscathed. What is dif-
economic hardship is restricted to the psycho- ferent about these children? What protective
logical functioning of girls (Hypothesis 3). In- factors or circumstances shielded fhem from
deed, income loss had no significant effects the adverse consequences of economic hard-
on girls apart from such family mediation. ship (Carmezy, 1981)? One factor involves
the observed difference between boys and
Several factors may account for gender girls. Overall, adolescent girls appeared to be
variations in the family-mediation hypothesis. more subject to rejection by deprived fathers
Perhaps because of their less imposing physi- during the Creat Depression.
cal size and strength, adolescent girls may
have been a more likely target for father's hos- A second relevant source of vulnerability
tility in hard times. Indeed, findings from the or resourcefulness is the child's physical ap-
present study suggest that fathers were pearance. Following the literature on child
slighdy more rejecting toward girls than to- maltreatment and the social meaning of phys-
ward boys in hard-pressed families. This sen- ical attributes, we expected the least attractive
timent may pardy reflect a displaced resent- adolescents to be at greater risk of patemal
ment conceming females over loss of male maltreatment than attractive boys and girls
status within the family. (Hypothesis 2). The data support this hy-
pothesis for girls. The physical attractiveness
Another explanation centers on the gen- of adolescent girls conditioned father's behav-
der roles of adolescents in economically de- ior toward them in hard times. Deprived
prived times. These may have had implica- fathers were more likely to reject unattractive
tions for girls' vulnerability within the family. than attractive daughters, and, in fact, they
For example, adolescent girls in the Oakland were actually more likely to be nurturant to-
sample were called upon to assume major re- ward attractive girls than nondeprived fathers.
sponsibilities within the household as their
mothers sought work. Adolescent boys, on Consistent with a social interactional per-
the other hand, were more likely to assume spective on family process (e.g., Patterson,
paid jobs outside the home. Family change of 1983), these findings suggest that some char-
this sort enhanced the social and family inde- acteristics of children actually strengthened
pendence of boys, but for girls greater house- the aversive behaviors of fathers in stressful
hold involvement may have meant greater ex- times. These findings underscore the impor-
posure to family discord and tension. Perhaps tance of viewing the family's changing socio-
because of their increased involvement in economic situation in relation to both the
household affairs during hard economic child's characteristics and parent behavior
times, as well as their greater interpersonal (Belsky, 1984; Ricciuti & Dorman, 1983).
sensitivity, adolescent girls appear more
strongly influenced by father's negative be- Conclusion
havior during hard times than boys. For boys, The developmental implications of social
however, the effects of economic deprivation change can only be studied within a theoret-
were seldom mediated by the behavior of ical framework that relates individual and
fathers. family change. Individuals are changed by
These gender differences in adolescence changing families, and families are changed
may be contrasted with findings on the family by changing the developmental course of
mediation model tested with boys and girls in members (Elder, 1984, 1985). Both of these
the Berkeley cohort (e.g.. Elder, Liker, & processes are likely to emerge when the life-
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, and Caspi 373
course dynamics of families and individuals demonstration of a simple association be-
are studied over time. A model of family tween two variables that are assumed to be
mediation typified the approach of Children ordered in a causal sequence. Literally hun-
of the Great Depression, and the present dreds of studies have correlated measures of
study builds on this work in at least two re- socioeconomic status and change with indi-
spects. First, it does so by examining the be- cators of child behavior, leaving the analysis
havior of fathers in linking economic stress to at that point without the barest feature of an
children's experience. interpretation or empirical test of linkages
Second, the present study explores (Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1983). In the
sources of variation in this causal process. The present research, the concept of" linkage
initial study of Oakland children in the De- serves as a reminder of the connection be-
pression era could only achieve a skeletal im- tween family income loss, on the one hand,
age of Depression fathers, owing to data limi- and children's social and emotional behavior
tations. Conceptual blinders at the time also on the other.
resulted in an understatement of children as As a whole, the results of this research
producers of their own socialization. In addi- support a perspective on families and chil-
tion to their role in the family economy, dren that locates them within the life course
which the first study covered in detail, these and its age-graded tasks and experiences. The
children also fared better or worse in the De- ever-changing environment of developing in-
pression regime of fathers for reasons linked dividuals implies that risk factors should vary
to gender and physical attractiveness.^ in type and relative influence along the life
The rejecting behavior of fathers repre- line. Indeed, family stresses appear to be
sents the connection between economic hard- more pathogenic for boys than for girls in the
ship and children's lives more than the corre- preschool years (Rutter & Madge, 1976),
sponding behavior of mothers. Fathers are whereas a contrasting sex difference is emerg-
prominent in our analysis of Depression ing in studies of early adolescence (Wemer &
families, in part because economic misfortune Smith, 1982). Sensitivity to this complexity of
was typically the first-hand experience of interactions among social, psychological, and
men. Their response to this loss intensified biological factors is consistent with Kagan's
the social consequences of economic loss. But (1979, p. 886) criticism of the search for "abso-
the full significance of father's behavior lute principles which declare that a particular
emerges only when we consider the adoles- set of external conditions is inevitably associ-
cent roles of" girls and boys in the Oakland ated with a fixed set of consequences for all
cohort, as well as physical attractiveness. The children." Extemal events, such as the Great
rejecting behavior of father is most strongly Depression, can affect older and younger chil-
linked to both economic deprivation and the dren, as well as parents, in different ways. An
emotional disturbance of girls and especially understanding of these ways requires knowl-
to those girls who were relatively unattrac- edge of the life course.
tive. We believe this causal sequence has
much to do with the psychological vulnerabil- References
ity of early adolescent girls to patemal
maltreatment and to the self-esteem costs of Bar-Tal, D., & Saxe, L. (1976). Physical attrac-
social pressures in the larger social world of tiveness and its relation to sex-role stereotyp-
peers and school (Simmons et al., 1979). ing. Sex Roles, 2, 123-133.
Bell, R. Q., & Harper, V. L. (1977). Child effects on
This study is responsive to a long- adults. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
standing criticism of purely descriptive re- Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A
search on socioeconomic factors in the lives of process model. Child Development, 55,83-96.
adults and children (Kohn, 1977). All too of- Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA:
ten, research proceeds no further than the Bancroft.

As in Children of the Great Depression, the most satisfactory analysis would trace causal
paths and linkages from the 1930s into the adult years and view all of this in relation to variable life
circumstances or conditions. Do the interacting effects of family deprivation and father's rejecting
behavior have consequences for psychosocial well-being in the pre-adult and adult years? Is such
treatment by father more likely to have adverse psychological effects among the least attractive
girls? Using a combined sample of Oakland and Berkeley Cuidance cases, preliminary research
indicates that such effects are concentrated in the relatively unattractive group. The Berkeley analy-
sis generally produced results that closely resemble the Oakland findings on the Interaction of
economic hardship and physical attractiveness.
374 Child Development
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The America. In K. H. McCluskey & H. W, Reese
evolution of environmental models in develop- (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology:
mental research. In W. Kessen (Ed.), P. H. Historical and cohort effects (pp. 161-201).
Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psy- New York: Academic Press.
chology: Vol. 1. History, theory, and methods Eme, R. F. (1979). Sex differences in childhood
(pp. 357-414). New York: Wiley. psychopathology: A review. Psychological Bul-
Coan, R. W. (1974). The optimal personality: An letin, 86, 574-595.
empirical and theoretical analysis. New York: Carmezy, N. (1981). Children under stress: Per-
Columbia University Press. spectives on antecedents and correlates of vul-
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple nerability and resistance to psychopathology.
regression/correlation analysis for behavioral In A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff, A. M. Barclay, &
sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. R. A. Zucker (Eds.), Further explorations in
Dion, K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evalua- personality (pp. 196-269). New York: Wiley
tions of children's transgressions. Journal of Interscience.
Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 207- Coode, W. J. (1971). Force and violence in the fam-
213. ily. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 33,
Eichorn, D. H., Clausen, J. A., Haan, N., Honzik, 624-636.
Marjorie M. P., & Mussen, P. H. (Eds.). (1981). Hansell, S., Sparacino, J., & Ronchi, D. (1982).
Present and past in middle life. New York: Ac- Physical attractiveness and blood pressure: Sex
ademic Press. and age differences. Personality and Social
Elder, C. H., Jr. (1969). Appeeirance and education Psychology Bulletin, 8, 113-121.
in marriage mobility. American Sociological Honzik, M. D., & Main, M. (1982). A fifty-year
Review, 34, 519-533. study of stressful and supportive parenting.
Elder, C. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the Great Crant proposal BNS-8342014, National Science
Depression. Chicago: University of Chicago Foundation.
Press. Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Child abuse:
Elder, C. H., Jr. (1979). Historical change in life An interactional event. New York: Columbia
pattems and personality. In P. B. Baltes & University Press.
O. C. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life span development Kagan, J. (1979). Family experience and the child's
and behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 117-159). New York: development. American Psychologist, 34, 886-
Academic Press. 891.
Elder, C. H., Jr. (1984). Families, kin, and the life Kohn, M. L. (1977). Class and conformity. Chicago:
course: A sociological perspective. In R. Parke University of Chicago Press.
(Ed.), Advances in child development research Liker, J. K., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1983). Economic
(Vol. 7). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. hardship and marital relations in the 1930s.
Elder, C. H., Jr. (Ed.). (1985). Life course dynam- American Sociological Review, 48, 343-359.
ics: Trajectories and transitions, 1968-1980. Lynd, R. S., & Lynd, H. M. (1937). Middletown in
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. transition: A study in cultural conflicts. New
Elder, C. H., Jr., Caspi, A., & Downey, C. (in York: Harcourt, Brace.
press). Problem behavior and family relation- Patterson, C. R. (1983). Stress: A change agent for
ships: Life course and intergenerational family process. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter
themes. In A. Sorensen, F. Weinert, & L. Sher- (Eds.), Stress, coping, and development in chil-
rod (Eds.), Human development: Interdisci- dren (pp. 235-264). New York: McCraw-Hill.
plinary perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Pearlin, L. I., Lieberman, M. A., Menagham, E. G.,
Elder, G. H., Jr., Caspi, A., & Van Nguyen, T. (in & Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress process./oMr-
press). Resourceful and vulnerable children: nal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337
Family influences in stressful times. In R. K. 356.
Silbereisen & K. Eyferth (Eds.), Development Ricciuti, H. N., & Dorman, R. (1983). Interaction of
in context: Integrative perspectives on youth multiple factors contributing to high-risk par-
development. New York: Springer. enting. In R. A. Hoekelman (Ed.), Minimizing
Elder, C. H., Jr., Liker, J. K., & Cross, C. E. (1984). high-risk parenting (pp. 187-210). Media, PA:
Parent-child behavior in the Creat Depression: Harwal.
Life course and intergenerational influences. Rutter, M., & Madge, N. (1976). Cycles of disadvan-
In P. B. Baltes & O. C. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life tage: A review of research. London: Hein-
span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. emann.
109-158). New York: Academic Press. Scherer, J., & Shepherd, G. (1982). Victimization of
Elder, C. H., Jr., Liker, J. K., & Jaworski, B. J. the weak: Contemporary social reactions.
(1984). Htird times in lives: Historical in- Springfield, IL: Thomas.
fluences from the 1930s to old age in postwar Simmons, R. G., Blyth, D. A., Van Cleave, E. F., &
Elder, Jr., Nguyen, and Caspi 375
Bush, D. M. (1979). Entry into adolescence: physical attractiveness as a contributor to indi-
T'he impact of school structure, puberty, and vidual development. In R. M. Lemer & N. A.
early dating on self-esteem. American Socio- Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Individuals as pro-
logical Review, 44, 948-967. ducers of their development: A life span per-
Smith, M. B. (1968). Competence and socializa- spective (pp. 389446). New York: Academic
tion. In J. A. Clausen (Ed.), Socialization Press.
and society (pp. 270-320). Boston: Little, Wemer, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable
Brown. but invincible: A study of resilient children.
Sorell, G. T., & Nowack, C. A. (1981). The role of New York: McGraw-Hill.

Potrebbero piacerti anche