Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2015

Vol. 10, No. 6, 499506, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1004554

Intellectual humility and forgiveness of religious leaders


Joshua N. Hook *, Don E. Davisb, Daryl R. Van Tongerenc, Peter C. Hilld, Everett L. Worthington Jr.e,
a

Jennifer E. Farrella and Phillip Diekef


a
Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA; bDepartment of Counseling and Psychological Services,
Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA; cDepartment of Psychology, Hope College, Holland, MI, USA; dDepartment of
Psychology, Biola University, La Mirada, CA, USA; eDepartment of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA,
USA; fHighland Park United Methodist Church, Dallas, TX, USA
(Received 16 July 2014; accepted 2 December 2014)

This article presents two studies that examined how perceptions of intellectual humility affect response to a transgression
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

by a religious leader. In Study 1, participants (N = 105) rated the religious leader on intellectual humility regarding
different religious beliefs and values, as well as general humility and forgiveness of the leader for a transgression.
Perceived intellectual humility was positively associated with forgiveness, even when controlling for perceived general
humility. In Study 2, we replicated the ndings from Study 1 on an independent sample (N = 299). Also, the type of
offense moderated the association between perceived intellectual humility and forgiveness. For participants, who reported
an offense in the area of religious beliefs, values, or convictions, the association between perceived intellectual humility
and forgiveness was stronger than for participants, who reported a different type of offense. We conclude by discussing
limitations and areas for future research.
Keywords: intellectual humility; forgiveness; religious leaders

With the rise of the positive psychology movement in the dimension, humble individuals have an accurate view of
last decade (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000), the self and an awareness of their limitations; on the
study of virtues such as forgiveness (Fehr, Gelfand, & interpersonal dimension, humble individuals have an
Nag, 2010) and gratitude (Emmons & McCullough, 2003) interpersonal stance that is other-oriented, rather than
has increased. Recently, philosophers and theologians self-focused; characterized by respect for others, rather
have become interested in epistemic virtue (DePaul & than superiority (Davis et al., 2011). McElroy et al.
Zagzebski, 2003), which addresses how to develop and (2014) suggested that, like constructs such as intelligence
hold certain beliefs well, including the concept of intellec- or self-efcacy, humility might involve subdomains
tual humility (IH). Whereas there has been some work in based on contexts that make humility difcult to prac-
the eld of psychology on humility in general (e.g. Davis tice. For example, Hook, Davis, Owen, Worthington, and
et al., 2011; Davis, Worthington, & Hook, 2010), there has Utsey (2013) recently focused on the subdomain involv-
been almost no work on IH humility about ones intel- ing engagement of cultural differences in psychotherapy.
lectual strengths and weaknesses (Samuelson, Church, In the present article, we focus on the subdomain of
Jarvinen, & Paulus, 2012). Theorizing within the eld of intellectual humility (IH) while engaging different
philosophy has suggested that IH may be especially religious beliefs, values, and convictions.
important for those having high levels of intellectual inu- The subdomain of IH pertains to how someone han-
ence such as religious leaders (Roberts & Wood, 2003). dles situations that make it difcult for most people to
This work also aligns with theorizing within psychology negotiate ideas fairly. Accordingly, IH involves having
that (a) perceptions of humility regulate the formation and an accurate view of ones intellectual strengths and
repair of social bonds and (b) are especially important in weaknesses, as well as the ability to negotiate different
contexts where humility is most difcult to practice (Davis ideas in an interpersonally respectful manner. Thus, gen-
et al., 2013). Thus, in the present study, we empirically eral humility (GH) refers to ones behavior across situa-
examine the importance of perceptions of IH under the tions and relationships, whereas IH refers to a particular
strain of a transgression by a religious leader. subset of behaviors involving ones thinking and behav-
ior while negotiating with different ideas in intellectual
Denitions discourse. The intellectually humble person is able to
Humility has been dened as including both intraper- regulate ones concern for being right and is open to
sonal and interpersonal dimensions. On the intrapersonal new information and pursuing and incorporating

*Corresponding author. Email: joshua.hook@unt.edu

2015 Taylor & Francis


500 J.N. Hook et al.

knowledge and truth from other sources, even when it is teaching information about the Sacred (Woodruff,
discrepant from ones original position. IH may be espe- Van Tongeren, McElroy, Davis, & Hook, 2014).
cially important for individuals that are viewed by their Religious leaders may be considered by followers to be
communities to have high levels of intellectual inuence especially wise and in tune with the Sacred. Religious
(Roberts & Wood, 2003). leaders hold positions of power in the area of religious
discourse, sometimes with thousands of followers, who
listen to their sermons and read their books. Religious
Function of humility in relationships individuals often model their thoughts and behaviors
Davis et al. (2013) described a model that focused on after their religious leaders, such as pastors, priests, or
humility in the context of social relationships. They other clergy.
argued that humility is perceived within a social context Third, holding religious convictions often makes IH
in order to help people predict how others will treat difcult to practice. Religious convictions involve ulti-
them. They posited that perceptions of humility help reg- mate questions about purpose, meaning, and the afterlife,
ulate social bonds. Namely, social bonds cause people to and religious individuals often have a lot invested in
react to the needs of others as if they were their own, their particular world view. Adhering to cultural world
but they can make someone vulnerable to exploitation if views (including religion) may also alleviate existential
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

someone draws on but does not reciprocate a strong anxiety by providing followers with both literal immor-
bond. Thus, when people perceive someone as high in tality (e.g. eternal life) and symbolic immortality (e.g. be
humility, this indicates that the person is unselsh and a part of something greater than the self; Vail et al.,
capable of a mutually benecial relationship, and thus 2010). Finally, certain religious teachings connect ones
the social bond is likely to grow stronger. However, if faith with being certain that what one believes is true
one sees a relationship partner acting arrogantly, then (Boyd, 2013). Given the important role of conviction in
one may perceive the target person as less humble, religious beliefs, it seems that IH may be a particularly
which leads one to view the relationship as a riskier difcult virtue to cultivate in this area.
investment and the social bond is likely to weaken. Ini- Fourth, a betrayal by a religious leader ought to espe-
tial empirical evidence is consistent with this idea. For cially strain IH. Indeed, this may be a perfect storm in
example, Davis et al. (2011, 2013) found that the more terms of challenges to ones ability to perceive a leader
victims perceived an offender as humble, the higher their as high in IH. Religious leaders hold a highly trusted
level of forgiveness. In a study of dating couples, per- role of governing religious discourse within a commu-
ceptions of humility were linked with higher levels of nity, so a major betrayal ought to make the victim feel
commitment and relationship satisfaction (Farrell et al., particularly vulnerable and likely to adjust their view of
in press). the leaders degree of IH. Indeed, religious individuals
often associate religious leaders with the Sacred, and
thus an offense by a clergy member may negatively
Intellectual humility and leadership impact a congregants view of the Sacred (Thomas,
Davis et al. (2013) also theorized that humility is espe- White, & Sutton, 2008). Congregants may also be likely
cially important in situations that make it most difcult to appraise such offenses as desecrations (Pargament,
to practice. Based on this logic, we describe four factors Magyar, Benore, & Mahoney, 2005), which intensify
that we posit will make IH especially difcult to prac- negative moral emotions such as anger, contempt, or
tice, especially when they occur simultaneously. disgust.
First, the role of leadership makes IH difcult to Prior research on humility and leadership has found
express. Namely, a leaders role usually involves having that humility may be viewed as more important in reli-
greater inuence than subordinates over the exchange of gious leaders than other types of leaders (e.g. business
ideas. The leaders opinion counts more than others. In leader, military leader; Exline & Geyer, 2004). Also,
addition, leaders are charged with moderating the ow of prior work has linked perceptions of humility by vic-
ideas fairly. Thus, they have greater opportunity to tims to forgiveness of general offenses (Davis et al.,
reward or punish ideas based on their alignment with the 2013). But this hypothesis has not been examined in
leaders agenda. In fact, a key aspect of leadership style the context of severe strain, such as when multiple fac-
may involve the degree to which leaders are viewed as tors align (e.g. betrayals by religious leaders involving
fairly incorporating ideas of team members (e.g. transfor- disagreements about convictions). This may be a per-
mational leadership, Judge & Piccolo, 2004; servant fect storm in terms of straining IH, and ought to lead
leadership, Van Dierendonck, 2011). to considerable variability in peoples ability to express
Second, the role of religious leadership may make IH IH. We expect that victims who view their religious
even more difcult to express. Religious leaders are leader as more intellectually humble will report higher
usually entrusted with the role of interpreting and forgiveness.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 501

The present studies items that assess the extent to which a target person is
The purpose of the present studies was to examine humble regarding an aspect of their cultural identity (e.g.
whether perceptions of IH were positively related to for- gender, race/ethnicity, religion/spirituality, sexual orienta-
giveness of a religious leader after a major rupture in tion). The original version of the CHS allowed people to
trust. In Study 1, we examined the association between choose a highly salient domain, whereas in this study,
perceptions of leader IH and forgiveness. We hypothe- the CHS was modied, so that all participants focused
sized that perceptions of leader IH would be positively on the domain of religious beliefs and values. Thus, in
related to forgiveness of the religious leader, even when the present study, participants rated the degree to which
controlling for perceptions of leader GH. In Study 2, we they perceived the religious leader to be intellectually
aimed to replicate the ndings from Study 1, as well as humble regarding different types of religious beliefs and
test whether the type of offense moderated the associa- values. Participants rate items on a 5-point scale from
tion between perceptions of leader IH and forgiveness. 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. There are
Specically, we hypothesized that, controlling for hurt- two subscales on the CHS: the positive subscale includes
fulness and GH, the association between perceptions of positive other-oriented characteristics (seven items; e.g.
leader IH and forgiveness would be stronger for offenses Is open to explore) and the negative subscale reects
negative characteristics involving superiority and making
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

that dealt with religious beliefs, values, or convictions


than for other types of offenses. assumptions (ve items; e.g. Makes assumptions).
Scores on this measure have shown evidence for internal
consistency and construct validity (Hook et al., 2013).
Study 1 For the present study, we took the mean of all items for
a total IH score. For the current sample, the Cronbachs
Method
alpha was 0.91 (95% CI = 0.880.93).
Participants and procedure
Participants were 105 undergraduate students (40 M, 65
F) from a large public university in the Southeastern Perceptions of general humility
United States. Participants ranged in age from 19 to Perceptions of GH of the religious leader were measured
55 years (M = 25.10, SD = 7.25). Participants reported a with the Relational Humility Scale (RHS; Davis et al.,
variety of racial backgrounds (24.0% White, 51.9% 2011). The RHS consists of 16 items that assess percep-
Black, 6.7% Latino, 9.6% Asian, 5.8% Multiracial, 1.9% tions of GH. The RHS consists of three subscales: global
Other). Participants were mostly Christian (72.4%, Mus- humility (ve items; e.g. He/she has a humble charac-
lim 5.7%, Buddhist 1.9%, Hindu 2.9%, Other 5.7%, ter), superiority (seven items; e.g. He/she thinks of
None 11.5%) and heterosexual (94.8%, gay/lesbian him/herself too highly), and accurate view of self (four
2.1%, bisexual 2.1%, other 1.0%). Participants were items; e.g. He/she knows him/herself well). Participants
recruited from undergraduate courses and participated in rate each item on a 5-point rating scale from 1 = strongly
exchange for a small amount of course credit. Partici- disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Scores on this measure
pants completed the study online. Participants rst gave have shown evidence for internal consistency and con-
consent to participate and then completed the question- struct validity (Davis et al., 2011). For the present study,
naires. Participants completed the questionnaires in rela- we took the mean of all items for a total GH score. For
tion to a religious leader who had committed an actual the current sample, the Cronbachs alpha was 0.89 (95%
transgression in a church. Participants were asked to CI = 0.86.92).
describe the offense, and ll out a series of question-
naires in relation to the offense and the religious/spiritual
leader. Participants were debriefed and given the contact Forgiveness
information of the researcher should they have questions. Forgiveness toward the religious leader was measured
with the Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivation
Inventory (TRIM; McCullough & Hoyt, 2002;
Measures McCullough et al., 1998). The TRIM consists of 18
Hurtfulness of the offense. Participants rated the hurtful- items that assess interpersonal forgiveness toward an
ness of the offense to them personally on a scale from offender. The TRIM consists of three subscales: benevo-
1 = very little hurt to 5 = large amount of hurt. lence (six items; e.g. Despite what he/she did, I want us
to have a positive relationship again), avoidance (seven
Perceptions of intellectual humility. Participants items; e.g. I keep as much distance between us as
completed the Cultural Humility Scale (CHS; Hook possible), and revenge (ve items; e.g. Ill make him
et al., 2013), which we adapted for the present study to or her pay). Participants rate each item on a 5-point
measure IH (see Appendix 1). The CHS consists of 12 rating scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly
502 J.N. Hook et al.

agree. Scores on this measure have shown evidence for Southwestern United States. Participants ranged in age
internal consistency and construct validity (McCullough from 18 to 53 years (M = 20.77, SD = 4.13). Participants
& Hoyt, 2002; McCullough et al., 1998). For the present reported a variety of racial backgrounds (60.2% White,
study, we took the mean of all items for a total forgive- 18.7% Black, 11.0% Latino, 4.3% Asian, .3% Native
ness score. For the current sample, the Cronbachs alpha American, 5.0% Multiracial, .3% Other). Participants
was 0.91 (95% CI = 0.88-.93). were mostly Christian (80.6%, Muslim 1.3%, Jewish
.7%, Other 1.3%, None 16.1%) and heterosexual
(90.3%, gay/lesbian 5.7%, bisexual 3.3%, other .7%).
Results and discussion
Participants were recruited from undergraduate courses
Before conducting the primary analyses, we checked the and participated in exchange for a small amount of
data for assumptions. There were no problems with outli- course credit. Participants rst gave consent to participate
ers or normality. Means, standard deviations, and inter- and then completed questionnaires online. Participants
correlations for all scales are in Table 1. completed the questionnaires in relation to a religious
Our primary hypothesis was that perceptions of leader leader who had committed an actual transgression in a
IH about religious beliefs and values would be positively church. Participants were asked to describe the offense,
associated with forgiveness of the religious leader, con- and ll out a series of questionnaires in relation to the
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

trolling for the hurtfulness of the offense and perceptions offense and the religious/spiritual leader. Participants
of GH. We tested this hypothesis using a hierarchical were debriefed and given the contact information of the
regression analysis with forgiveness as the dependent vari- researcher should they have questions.
able, hurtfulness and GH entered in Step 1, and IH entered
in Step 2. This hypothesis was supported. In Step 1,
hurtfulness and GH signicantly predicted forgiveness, Measures
R2 = 0.27, p < 0.001. In Step 2, IH was a signicant pre- Hurtfulness of the offense. As in Study 1, participants
dictor of forgiveness over and above the effects of hurtful- rated the hurtfulness of the offense to them personally
ness and GH, R2 = 0.07, p = 0.001. In this nal model, on a scale from 1 = very little hurt to 5 = large amount
both GH ( = 0.27, p = 0.011) and IH ( = 0.35, of hurt.
p = 0.001) positively predicted forgiveness. The nal
model predicted 33.6% of the variance in forgiveness.
This rst study provided evidence that perceptions of Type of offense
leader IH regarding religious beliefs and values were Two independent coders evaluated whether or not the
positively related to forgiveness of a religious leader. IH offense was related to a conict in the area of religious
was a signicant predictor of forgiveness, even when beliefs, values, or convictions. The two coders agreed on
controlling for hurtfulness and GH. In the second study, 94.0% of cases. In cases of disagreement, the two coders
we explored the extent to which the type of offense discussed and reached consensus. 8.7% of offenses were
moderated the association between IH and forgiveness. related to a conict in the area of religious beliefs,
values, or convictions. An example of this type of
offense was: I was told I would go to Hell for not being
Study 2
Christian and for [sic] friending people who are homo-
Method sexual. Examples of other types of transgressions (i.e.
Participants and procedure not related to conict in the area of religious beliefs, val-
Participants were 299 undergraduate students (97 M, 201 ues, or convictions) included nancial and sexual
F, 1 other) from a large public university in the offenses.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study Perceptions of intellectual humility. As in Study 1,
variables (Study 1). participants completed the Cultural Humility Scale
(CHS; Hook et al., 2013), which we adapted for the
M SD 1 2 3 4
present study to measure perceptions of IH of the reli-
1. Hurtfulness 2.98 1.32 gious leader in relation to religious beliefs and values
2. CHS Total 2.86 0.82 0.16 (see Appendix 1). For the current sample, the Cronbachs
3. RHS Total 3.06 0.60 0.02 0.62* alpha was 0.90 (95% CI = 0.88.91).
5. TRIM 3.41 0.75 0.18 0.53* 0.48*
Note: CHS = Cultural Humility Scale, which reects perceptions of lea-
der IH; RHS = Relational Humility Scale, which reects perceptions of Perceptions of general humility
leader GH; TRIM = Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivation
Inventory; all scales range from 1 to 5. As in Study 1, participants completed the Relational
*p < .01. Humility Scale (RHS; Davis et al., 2011) to measure
The Journal of Positive Psychology 503

perceptions of GH of the religious leader. For the current a hierarchical regression analysis with forgiveness as the
sample, the Cronbachs alpha was 0.92 (95% CI = 0.91 dependent variable. We entered hurtfulness, GH, IH, and
0.93). offense type in Step 1, and the interaction between IH
and offense type in Step 2. This hypothesis was sup-
ported. In Step 1, hurtfulness, GH, IH, and offense type
Forgiveness predicted a signicant amount of variance in forgiveness,
As in Study 1, forgiveness toward the religious leader was R2 = 0.30, p < 0.001. In Step 2, controlling for the afore-
measured with the Transgression-Related Interpersonal mentioned variables, the interaction between offense type
Motivations Inventory (TRIM; McCullough & Hoyt, and IH was a signicant predictor of forgiveness,
2002; McCullough et al., 1998). For the current sample, R2 = 0.02, p = 0.009 (see Figure 1).
the Cronbachs alpha was 0.88 (95% CI = 0.85.89). To explore this signicant interaction, we conducted
follow-up hierarchical regression analyses, examining the
effect of IH on forgiveness separated by offense type,
Results and discussion controlling for hurtfulness and GH. We entered hurtful-
Before conducting the primary analyses, we checked the ness and GH in Step 1, and IH in Step 2. For offenses
data for assumptions. The TRIM had one low outlier, that were in the area of religious beliefs, values, or
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

which we recoded to three standard deviations below the convictions, in Step 1, hurtfulness and GH predicted a
mean. There were no problems with normality. Means, signicant amount of variance in forgiveness, R2 = 0.49,
standard deviations, and intercorrelations for all scales p < 0.001. In Step 2, controlling for hurtfulness and GH,
are in Table 2. IH was a signicant predictor of forgiveness over and
Our rst hypothesis was that the ndings from Study above the effect of hurtfulness and relational humility
1 would replicate on an independent sample. We tested R2 = 0.16, p = 0.005. In the nal model, IH was a sig-
this hypothesis using a hierarchical regression analysis nicant predictor of forgiveness ( = 0.67, p = 0.005),
with forgiveness as the dependent variable. We entered but GH was not ( = 0.08, p = 0.707).
hurtfulness and GH in Step 1, and IH in Step 2. This For other types of offenses, in Step 1, hurtfulness
hypothesis was supported. In Step 1, hurtfulness and GH and GH predicted a signicant amount of variance in
predicted a signicant amount of variance in forgiveness, forgiveness, R2 = 0.26, p < 0.001. In Step 2, controlling
R2 = 0.27, p < 0.001. In Step 2, controlling for hurtful- for hurtfulness and GH, IH was a signicant predictor of
ness and GH, IH was a signicant predictor of forgive- forgiveness over and above the effect of hurtfulness and
ness over and above hurtfulness and GH, R2 = 0.03, GH, R2 = 0.02, p = 0.012. In the nal model, both GH
p = 0.001. In this nal model, both GH ( = 0.30, ( = 0.34, p < 0.001) and IH ( = 0.21, p = 0.012) were
p < 0.001) and IH ( = 0.27, p = 0.001) were signicant signicant predictors of forgiveness. Thus, IH was a
predictors of forgiveness. The nal model predicted stronger predictor of forgiveness for offenses in the area
29.7% of the variance in forgiveness. of religious beliefs, values, or convictions.
Our second hypothesis was that the type of offense This second study replicated the results from Study
would moderate the association between perceptions of 1, and also explored whether the type of offense moder-
leader IH and forgiveness. Specically, we expected that ated the association between perceptions of leader IH
if the offense was in the area of religious beliefs, values, and forgiveness. Namely, when the offense focused on a
or convictions, the association between perceptions of
leader IH and forgiveness would be stronger than for a
different type of offense. We tested this hypothesis using 5

4.5
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study
variables (Study 2).
4 Conviction
M SD 1 2 3 4 Other

1. Hurtfulness 2.84 1.31 3.5


2. CHS Total 2.98 0.84 0.24*
3. RHS Total 3.11 0.82 0.23* 0.79*
4. TRIM 3.36 0.71 0.14 0.51* 0.52* 3
IHLow IHHigh
Note: CHS = Cultural Humility Scale, which reects perceptions of lea-
der IH; RHS = Relational Humility Scale, which reects perceptions of
leader GH; TRIM = Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivation Figure 1. Moderator effects of the type of offense on the
Inventory; all scales range from 1 to 5. association between perceptions of leader IH and forgiveness
*p < .01. ( = 0.14, p = 0.009).
504 J.N. Hook et al.

conict in the area of religious beliefs, values, or convic- to be important to the development and repair of social
tions, the association between perceptions of leader IH bonds (Davis et al., 2013; Farrell et al., in press). The
and forgiveness was stronger than for different types of present study expanded on this body of literature by spe-
offenses. Thus, IH might be especially important for cically measuring perceptions of IH toward religious
religious leaders who commit offenses in the area of leaders. Furthermore, the present study supports prior
religious beliefs, values, or convictions. theorizing by Davis et al. (2013) that identies certain
difcult situations or contexts as being important to
study humility. The present study examined a context
General discussion that involved (a) conict and (b) a hierarchical relation-
Prior work has provided evidence for the social bond ship (religious leader and member).
hypothesis that perceptions of humility help regulate
the formation, maintenance, and repair of social bonds
(Davis et al., 2011; Farrell et al., in press). Prior work Limitations and suggestions for future research
examined this hypothesis in the context of general There were several limitations to the present set of stud-
offenses. The present set of studies examined this ies. First, both studies used a sample of undergraduate
hypothesis in a situation theorized to be a perfect storm students. The religious experience of undergraduate stu-
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

in terms of making IH difcult to practice (i.e. an dents may be unique, as some evidence suggests that
offense committed by a religious leader). We focused on religious participation declines and the nature of reli-
this context for two reasons. First, IH may be an espe- gious/spiritual beliefs may change during the college
cially important virtue in individuals who are viewed by years (Mayrl & Oeur, 2009). Generalizing the ndings
their communities as having a high level of intellectual from these studies to adolescents or older adults is not
inuence (Roberts & Wood, 2003). The role of a reli- recommended. Future research should examine the
gious leader certainly falls within this denition. Second, perceptions of IH in these populations.
it may be especially difcult to engage with IH around Second, both studies used cross-sectional, correla-
ones religious beliefs, values, and convictions. Religious tional designs. Thus, causal conclusions should not be
beliefs, values, and convictions are often strongly made. Although the data were consistent with our theo-
defended because they answer ultimate questions, such retical model (i.e. perceptions of IH leading to increased
as (a) ones meaning and life and (b) the afterlife (Vail forgiveness), there may be other theoretical models that
et al., 2010), and IH is theorized to play an important are consistent with the data as well. Longitudinal or
role in religious tolerance (see Woodruff et al., 2014). experimental research is necessary to further explicate
Overall, our hypotheses were supported. The more the nature of these relationships.
victims perceived the religious leader to have IH, the Third, the present study adapted an existing measure
more they reported being able to forgive him or her. This of cultural humility (Hook et al., 2013) to measure IH.
was true even after controlling for perceptions of leader This strategy focused on assessing a subdomain of rele-
GH. Furthermore, the association between perceptions of vant behaviors by adjusting the focus of the directions of
IH and forgiveness was especially strong for offenses the scale. The original prompt had people focus on a
that involved religious beliefs, values, or convictions. particular cultural identity, which is somewhat broader in
These ndings provide initial evidence that domain- focus. The present study focused on humility regarding
specic constructs such as IH may predict unique vari- different kinds of religious beliefs and values. Presum-
ance relative to measures of GH. Moreover, they provide ably, such domain-specic assessments ought to predict
additional evidence for the social bond hypothesis and domain-relevant behaviors more strongly than more gen-
the role of humility perceptions in regulating victims eral measures of humility, but this prediction has not
relationships with offenders. been thoroughly examined, and it is important to see if
The present study adapted the Cultural Humility the ndings in our study continue to replicate across a
Scale (CHS; Hook et al., 2013) to assess IH of religious variety of samples.
beliefs and values. In general, scores on the CHS Fourth, although the present study offered some evi-
showed evidence for internal consistency and construct dence for the unique predictive ability of IH relative to
validity, although more research is needed on this mea- GH, other variables that may be related to IH or GH,
sure as applied to religious beliefs and values. Future such as perceived agreeableness, trustworthiness, or nar-
research could (a) explore both other-ratings and self- cissism of the leader, were not assessed. Future research
reports of IH, as well as (b) explicitly measure the level could explore the relationship between IH and forgive-
of agreement (or not) between the religious leader and ness controlling for other personality and relationship
the person judging their level of IH. variables.
More generally, the present study provides support Fifth, the present study explored the links between
for a growing body of literature that has shown humility IH and forgiveness in one type of leadership context (i.e.
The Journal of Positive Psychology 505

religious leaders). It is important to explore whether Farrell, J. E., Hook, J. N., Ramos, M., Davis, D. E., Van
these ndings will extend to other leadership contexts Tongeren, D. R., & Ruiz, J. M. (in press). Humility and
relationship outcomes in couples: The mediating role of
(e.g. politics, business). Some research has found that
commitment. Couple and Family Psychology: Research
humility in religious leaders may be viewed as more of a and Practice.
strength than other types of leaders (Exline & Geyer, Fehr, R., Gelfand, M. J., & Nag, M. (2010). The road to for-
2004). Future research should investigate the relationship giveness: A meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and
between intellectual humility and forgiveness in other dispositional correlates. Psychological Bulletin, 136,
894914.
leadership contexts.
Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Owen, J., Worthington, E. L., Jr, &
Utsey, S. O. (2013). Cultural humility: Measuring openness
to culturally diverse clients. Journal of Counseling
Conclusion Psychology, 60, 353366.
We encourage researchers to continue to explore the role Judge, T. A., & Piccolo, R. F. (2004). Transformational and
transactional leadership: A meta-analytic test of their rela-
of IH in regard to different types of religious beliefs, val-
tive validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 3651.
ues, and convictions. It may be especially difcult to act Mayrl, D., & Oeur, F. (2009). Religion and higher education:
humbly about different types of religious beliefs, values, Current knowledge and directions for future research. Jour-
and convictions, especially for individuals who have nal for the Scientic Study of Religion, 48, 260275.
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

high levels of religious commitment (Woodruff et al., McCullough, M. E., & Hoyt, W. T. (2002). Transgression-
related motivational dispositions: Personality substrates of
2014). However, conict in religious contexts, as seen in
forgiveness and their links to the big ve. Personality and
the present study, is common. IH toward religion may be Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 15561573.
an effective way to help repair damaged relationships McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington,
when such conict inevitably occurs. E. L., Jr, Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal
forgiving in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration
and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social
Funding Psychology, 75, 15861603.
McElroy, S. E., Rice, K. G., Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., Hill, P.
We would like to acknowledge the generous nancial support C., Worthington, E. L., Jr, & Van Tongeren, D. R. (2014).
of Fuller Theological Seminary/Thrive Center in concert with Intellectual humility: Scale development and theoretical
the John Templeton Foundation, [grant number 108] (Intellec- elaborations in the context of religious leadership. Journal
tual Humility in Religious Leaders), as well as the John Tem- of Psychology and Theology, 42, 1930.
pleton Foundation [grant number 29630], The Development, Pargament, K. I., Magyar, G. M., Benore, E., & Mahoney, A.
Validation, and Dissemination of Measures of Intellectual (2005). Sacrilege: A study of sacred loss and desecration
Humility and Humility; [grant number 14979], Relational and their implications for health and well-being in a com-
Humility: An Interdisciplinary Approach to the Study of munity sample. Journal for the Scientic Study of Religion,
Humility). 44, 5978.
Roberts, R. C., & Wood, W. J. (2003). Humility and epistemic
goods. In M. DePaul, & L. Zagzebski (Eds.), Intellectual
References virtue: Perspectives from ethics and epistemology
Boyd, G. A. (2013). Benet of the doubt: Breaking the idol of (pp. 203226). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
certainty. Grand Rapids, MI: Baker Books. Samuelson, P. L., Church, I. M., Jarvinen, M., & Paulus, T.
Davis, D. E., Hook, J. N., Worthington, E. L., Jr, Van (2012). The science of intellectual humility white paper.
Tongeren, D. R., Gartner, A. L., Jennings, D. J., II, & Unpublished manuscript, Fuller Theological Seminary
Emmons, R. A. (2011). Relational humility: Conceptualiz- School of Psychology.
ing and measuring humility as a personality judgment. Seligman, M. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive
Journal of Personality Assessment, 93, 225234. psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55,
Davis, D. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr, & Hook, J. N. (2010). 514.
Humility: Review of measurement strategies and conceptu- Thomas, E. K., White, K., & Sutton, G. W. (2008). Clergy
alization as personality judgment. The Journal of Positive apologies following abuse: What makes a difference?
Psychology, 5, 243252. Exploring forgiveness, apology, responsibility-taking,
Davis, D. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr, Hook, J. N., Emmons, R. A., gender, and restoration. Journal of Psychology and
Hill, P. C., Bollinger, R. A., & Van Tongeren, D. R. (2013). Christianity, 27, 1629.
Humility and the development and repair of social bonds: Vail, K. E. III., Rothschild, Z. K., Weise, D. R., Solomon, S.,
Two longitudinal studies. Self and Identity, 12, 5877. Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2010). A terror manage-
DePaul, M., & Zagzebski, L. (2003). Intellectual virtue: Per- ment analysis of the psychological function of religion.
spectives from ethics and epistemology. Oxford: Clarendon Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 8494.
Press. Van Dierendonck, D. (2011). Servant leadership: A review and
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting bless- synthesis. Journal of Management, 37, 12281261.
ings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of grati- Woodruff, E., Van Tongeren, D. R., McElroy, S., Davis, D. E.,
tude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of & Hook, J. N. (2014). Humility and religion: Benets, dif-
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377389. culties, and a model of religious tolerance. In C. Kim-
Exline, J. J., & Geyer, A. L. (2004). Perceptions of humility: A Prieto (Ed.), Positive psychology of religion and spirituality
preliminary study. Self and Identity, 3, 95114. across cultures (pp. 271285). New York, NY: Springer.
506 J.N. Hook et al.

Appendix 1. Cultural Humility Scale (CHS) which you agree or disagree with the following statements
religious beliefs and values about your clergy/minister, regarding different types of reli-
gious beliefs/values.
Directions: Please think about the clergy/minister you
identied. Using the scale below, please indicate the extent to

Regarding different types of religious beliefs and values, the Strongly Mildly Neutral Mildly Strongly
clergy/minister disagree (1) disagree (2) (3) agree (4) agree (5)
1. Is respectful 1 2 3 4 5
2. Is open to explore 1 2 3 4 5
3. Assumes he/she already knows a lot 1 2 3 4 5
4. Is considerate 1 2 3 4 5
5. Is genuinely interested in learning more 1 2 3 4 5
6. Acts superior 1 2 3 4 5
7. Is open to seeing things from other perspectives 1 2 3 4 5
8. Makes assumptions 1 2 3 4 5
Downloaded by [Ohio State University Libraries] at 04:56 16 June 2016

9. Is open-minded 1 2 3 4 5
10. Is a know-it-all 1 2 3 4 5
11. Thinks he/she understands more than he/she actually does 1 2 3 4 5
12. Asks questions when he/she is uncertain 1 2 3 4 5
Note: Positive subscale items: 1, 2, 4, 5, 7, 9, 12.
Negative subscale items: 3, 6, 8, 10, 11.

Potrebbero piacerti anche