Sei sulla pagina 1di 20

2013 3 97116

1 2

Implicit Association Test, IAT IAT 36

IAT
IAT IAT
IAT 30

Galvanic Skin Response, GSR IAT


IAT GSR r
.48 .53 GSR r = -.24 n.s
GSR r = -.32IAT
IAT

1.
2.
3.
2011.10.072012.01.10
Emailfachung1014@gmail.com
70

60 1

Psychological Testing

March 2013, Vol. 60 No. 1, pp. 97-116

Development of Implicit Association


Test for Flight Stress toward Pilots

Abstract
This study aims to develop the flight stress Implicit Association TestIATby
taking the general stress IAT as reference. In Experiment 1, 36 pilots were randomly assigned to the stress group and the non-stress group. Ten aircraft accident pictures were
shown to the pilots in the stress group to induce their stress state, and ten neutral pictures were shown to the pilots in the non-stress group. Afterward, the flight stress IAT and
the general stress IAT were performed. The results showed that the score of IAT for the
pilots in the stress group was higher than those in the non-stress group. In Experiment
2, 30 pilots were required to watch the same stress pictures as those in Experiment 1,
and their Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) were measured at the same time. In addition,
the flight stress IAT and the general stress IAT were performed, and the explicit self-repot flight stress and general stress statuses were measured. The results revealed that two
IATs and GSR were significantly related (r = .48 and .53, accordingly), the correlation
between the explicit general stress measurement and GRS was not significant (r = .24),
and the correlation between the explicit flight stress measurement and GRS was not significant either (r = -.32). The results indicated that the flight stress IAT has criterion-related validity. In sum, the flight stress IAT edited in this study is proven to be a valid instrument which can measure pilots implicit stress sate.
Keywords: flight stress, Galvanic Skin Response, Implicit Association Test, stress
1. Fa-Chung Chiu, Associate Professor, Department of Psychology and Social Work, National Defense University
2. Chih-Chun Hsu, Postdoctoral Fellow, Center for Teaching and Learning Development,
National Taiwan Normal University
3. Hsueh-Chih Chen, Professor, Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling,
National Taiwan Normal University
Received: 2011.10.07; Accepted: 2012.01.10
Corresponding Author: Fa-Chung Chiu; Email: fachung1014@gmail.com
Address: 70, Section 2, Zhongyang N. Road, Taipei City 112, Taiwan
Department of Psychology and Social Work, National Defense University

stress
burnoutDoyle & Hind, 1998Beehr, 1995
Theorell & Karasek, 1996
employee withdrawalBeehr, 1995

60%42.5%
Pflanz, 2001

Farmer & Mclntyre, 2000

Ahmadi Alireza2007 89
33.7%48.5%
13.5%
life stressorganizational stress
flight environmental stresstask-based stress
Ahmadi Alireza
temperatureaccelerationnoise communication decompression sickness
vibration hypoxia exhaust fumes
Ohrui 2008F-15
Carlisle, 2001; Farmer &
Mclntyre, 2000; Lee & Liu, 2003; McGrath, 1984; Sheriden & Laurence, 1996; Veltman & Gaillard, 1996; Wiggins & OHare, 2003

AlkovGaynor Borowsky1985
Bowles
Ursin Picano2000

60 1

self-report Cohen,
Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983; French, Lenton, Walters, & Eyles, 2000; Johnson &
Cooper, 2003
introspective limitssocial desirability
Perugini & Banse, 2007
awareness
Kihlstrom, 1987; Mandler, 1975Wicker1969 31

10%
evaluation apprehensionRosenberg, 1969faking
Cronbach, 1990conscious distortionViswesvaran & Ones, 1999
Holden, Wood, & Tomashewski, 2001

Implicit Association Test


IATGreenwaldMcGhee Schwartz1998 IAT

Clerkin & Teachman, 2010; Stieger,


Gritz, & Burger, 2010Frost et al., 2010Goethem,
Scholte, & Wiers, 2010Ames, Grenard, Sussman, & Wiers, 2007;
Houben, Nosek, & Wiers, 2010; McCarthy & Thompsen, 2006Turner & Crisp, 2010 Roccato & Zogmaister, 2010
Grumm, Hein, & Fingerle, 2011
IAT computer-administered
categorization tasksIAT
combined discrimination tasksconcepts
strengths of associations
WHITEBLACK IAT

initial target-con

cept discrimination

associated attribute discrimination task

initial combined task vs.


vs.

reversed target-concept discrimination task


reversed combined task

Greenwald 1998

300 3,000
300 3,000
20%
IAT D D
Greenwald 1998 IAT 2003
IAT
IAT

10,000
A
B AB
IAT D GreenwaldNosek Banaji2003

IAT operate automaticallyDevine, Plant, Amodio, Harmon-Jones, & Vance, 2002

IAT Banse, Seise, &


Zerbes, 2001

60 1

IAT
Egloff & Schmukle, 2002IAT
Asendorpf, Banse, & Mcke, 2002; Chou, Chiu, Chen, Hsu, & Cho, 2009; Friese, Bluemke, & Wnke, 2007; Houben & Wiers, 2008; Nock et al., 2010
IAT
IAT
IAT
IAT

IAT
IAT
IAT target
IAT
IAT

IAT IAT IAT

IAT
IAT IAT
IAT

36 M-2000F-16IDF
E-2TOH-58F-5EC-130H UH1H

1.
IAT

IAT IAT
American Psychiatric Association, APADiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. DSM-IV
Cohen 1983Perceived Stress Scale, PSS El-Gilany 2010

Likert
IAT
M =
8.67SD = .57 IAT
33 Likert

M = 1.56SD = .21
15
focus group

30

M = 7.05SD = .44
IAT IAT

60 1

IAT Greenwald 2003


IAT

IAT

20
10 10

20

vs. vs.

20


40

20

20

40

IAT Greenwald et al., 2003


IAT 10,000
300 10%

600

A
B AB IAT D
IAT
IAT
2.

19 Likert
M = 5.00SD = .94 3.50

t(18)= 3.31p = .02

IAT
IAT

IAT IAT

t-test IAT
IAT M = .01SD = .58

60 1

M = -.17SD = .34t(34) = 1.09p


= .28effect sizeCohen's d = .38 IAT
IAT M = -.05SD = .46
M = -.30SD = .33t(34) = 1.90p =
.03 Cohen's d = .62
IAT IAT
IAT IAT

measurement of physiological signals


Galvanic Skin Response, GSR IAT
Criterion-related validityGSR
Boucsein, 1992 GSR sympathetic nervous
system, SNSactivationBacks Boucsein2000
GSR physiological indicator
arousal Fahrenberg Wientjes2000
empirical studyChou
2009 GSR
Mackersie Cones2011
(1)(2)
(3)

GSR GSR

IAT
GSR GSR IAT
IAT
GSR

30 F-16IDFE2T
OH58F-5EC130H UH1H

1.
IAT
2.

3.
Greenwald Farnham2000 IAT
explicit IAT

.95
4.
Greenwald Farnham2000 IAT

13

.83

60 1

5.
Thought Technology Ltd.
2011
script

GSR

IAT
GSR

baseline phase
stress phase
recover
phase GSR

IAT IAT

GSR
GSR
t(29) = 2.62p = .01
IAT GSR
1 IAT IAT
GSR r =
.53p = .003r = .48p = .008 IAT
GSR
IAT

SD

1. IAT

.01

.44

2. IAT

.36

.44

3.

7.72

4.
5.

1.00
.72*

1.00

1.09

-.05

-.16

1.27

.89

.30

.33

.97

.41

.53**

.48**

1.00
.41*
-.24

1.00
-.32

**p < .01; *p < .05

GSR r
-.24-.32p > .05 IAT

IAT
IAT explicit
implicit

F-16 AH1W
IAT

IAT
IAT
IAT M = .01SD = .30
M = .02SD = .53t(28) =
.02p = .98 IAT
IAT M = .51SD = .17M = .25
SD = .55t(28) = 1.69p = .05

IAT
IAT
IAT

60 1

IAT
IAT IAT (1)
IAT IAT
(2)state IAT
GSR GSR (3)
IAT (4) IAT

IAT
12 4 IAT IAT
IAT IAT

IATdomain-specific
IAT GSR r
.48 .53effect size
Cohen, 1988

GSR IAT
MODE motivation opportunity as determinants model
attitudespontaneous
deliberative

Fazio & Olson, 2003 MODE


Asendorpf et al., 2002
GSR IAT
IAT GSR

Banse et al., 2001; Cunningham, Preacher,


& Banaji, 2001

Banse et al.,
2001; Cunningham et al., 2001; Greenwald & Nosek, 2001

(1)
(2)
Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005

IAT
IAT IAT GSR

IAT fakeKim2003
IAT
noncompatible task BLACK/good-WHITE/
badcompatible taskWHITE/
good-BLACK/bad IAT IAT
IAT

IAT

60 1

IAT
IAT

IAT IAT
IAT IAT
IAT IAT IAT
IAT
IAT

Ahmadi, K., & Alireza, K. (2007). Stress and job satisfaction among air force military pilots. Journal of Social Sciences, 3(3), 159-163.
Alkov, R. A., Gaynor, J. A., & Borowsky, M. S. (1985). Pilot error as a symptom of inadequate stress coping. Aviation Space & Environmental Medicine, 56, 244-247.
Ames, S. L., Grenard, J. L., Sussman, S., & Wiers, R. W. (2007). Comparison of indirect
assessments of association as predictors of marijuana use among at-risk adolescents.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15, 204-218.
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mcke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and
explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 83, 380-393.
Backs, R. W., & Boucsein, W. (Eds.) (2000). Engineering psychophysiology: Issues and
applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Banse, R., Seise, J., & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitscrift for Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145-160.
Beehr, T. (1995). Psychological stress in the workplace. London, UK: Routledge.
Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Bowles, S., Ursin, H., & Picano, J. (2000). Aircrew perceived stress: Examining crewperformance, crew position, and captains personality. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 71, 1093-1097.
Carlisle, D. (2001). Features-pilot stress and what to do about it-excess stress can damage
physical and psychological well-being, and can interfere with your piloting ability:
Here's how to identify and overcome. Business and Commercial Aviation, 89, 92-99.
Chou, T. A., Chiu, F. C., Chen, H. C., Hsu, C. C., & Cho, S. L. (2009). Implicit and explicit
measures of the shyness. Bulletin of Educational Psychology, 41(2), 453-474.
Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (2010). Training implicit social anxiety associations:
An experimental intervention. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 300-308.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress.
Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 24, 385-396.
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing (5th ed.). New York, NY: Harper

60 1

& Row.
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures:
Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 121, 163-170.
Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The
regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 835-848.
Doyle, C., & Hind, P. (1998). Occupational stress, burnout and job status in female academics. Gender, Work & Organization, 5, 67-82.
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for
assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441-1455.
El-Gilany, A., Amr, M., Brar, A., Nejad, F., Gaidhane, M., & Vatsyayann, S. (2010). Perceived stress among tomorrows attorneys in Mansoura, Egypt. International Journal
of Collaborative Research on Internal Medicine & Public Health, 2, 20-30.
Fahrenberg, J., & Wientjes, C. J. E. (2000). Recording methods in applied environments.
In R. W. Backs & W. Boucsein (Eds.), Engineering psychophysiology: Issues and applications (pp. 111-136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Farmer, E. W., & Mclntyre, H. M. (2000). Crew resource management. In J. Ernsting, A.
N. Nicholson & D. J. Rainford (Eds.), Aviation medicine (pp. 609-610). London, UK:
Butterworth-Heinemann.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition: Their meaning
and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297-327.
French, S. E., Lenton, R., Walters, V., & Eyles, J. (2000). An empirical evaluation of an expanded nursing stress scale. Journal of Nursing Measurement, 8(2), 161-178.
Friese, M., Bluemke, M., & Wnke, M. (2007). Predicting voting behavior with implicit attitude measures. Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift fr Experimentelle
Psychologie), 54(4), 247-255.
Frost, P., Adie, M., Denomme, R., Lahaie, A., Sibley, A., & Smith, E. (2010). Application
of the Implicit Association Test to a study on deception. American Journal of Psychology, 123, 221-230.
Goethem, A., Scholte, R., & Wiers, R. (2010). Explicit and implicit bullying attitudes in relation to bullying behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 829-842.
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
1022-1038.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Association Test at age 3.

Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 85-93.


Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 197-216.
Grumm, M., Hein, S., & Fingerle, M. (2011). Predicting aggressive behavior in children
with the help of measures of implicit and explicit aggression. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 35(4), 352-357.
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwender, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A metaanalysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369-1385.
Holden, R. R., Wood, L. L., & Tomashewski, L. (2001). Do response time limitations
counteract the effect of faking on personality inventory validity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 160-169.
Houben, K., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). Implicitly positive about alcohol? Implicit positive associations predict drinking behavior. Addictive behaviors, 33(8), 979-986.
Houben, K., Nosek, B. A., & Wiers, R. W. (2010). Seeing the forest through the trees: A
comparison of different IAT variants measuring implicit alcohol associations. Drug &
Alcohol Dependence, 106(2/3), 204-211.
Johnson, S., & Cooper, C. (2003). The construct validity of the ASSET stress measure.
Stress and Health, 19(3), 181-185.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445-1452.
Kim, D. Y. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Social
Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83-96.
Lee, Y. H., & Liu, B. S. (2003). Inflight workload assessment: Comparison of subjective
and physiological measurements. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, 74
(10), 1078-1084.
Mackersie, C. L., & Cones, H. (2011). Subjective and psychophysiological indices of listening effort in a competing-talker task. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 22, 113-122.
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
McCarthy, D. M., & Thompsen, D. M. (2006). Implicit and explicit measures of alcohol
and smoking cognitions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20, 436-444.

60 1

McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010).
Measuring the suicidal mind. Psychological Science, 21(4), 511-516.
Ohrui, N., Kanazawa, F., Takeuchi, Y., Otsuka, Y., Tarui, H., & Miyamoto, Y. (2008). Urinary catecholamine responses in F-15 pilots: Evaluation of the stress induced by
long-distance flights. Military Medicine, 173, 594-598.
Perugini, M., & Banse, R. (2007). Personality, implicit self-concept and automaticity.
European Journal of Personality, 21, 257-261.
Pflanz, S. (2001). Occupational stress and psychiatric illness in the military: Investigation
of the relationship between occupational stress and mental illness among military
mental health patients. Military Medicine, 166, 457-462.
Roccato, M., & Zogmaister, C. (2010). Predicting the vote through implicit and explicit attitudes: A field research. Political Psychology, 31, 249-274.
Rosenberg, M. J. (1969). The conditions and consequences of evaluation apprehension. In
R. Rosenthal & R. L. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifact in behavioral research. New York, NY:
Academic Press.
Sheriden, T. B., & Laurence, R. Y. (1996). Human factors in aerospace medicine. In R. L. Dehart
(Ed.), Fundamentals of aerospace medicine. Baltimor, MD: Williams and Wilkins Copy.
Stieger, S., Gritz, A. S., & Burger, C. (2010). Personalizing the IAT and the SC-IAT: Impact of idiographic stimulus selection in the measurement of implicit anxiety. Personality & Individual Differences, 48, 940-944.
Theorell, T., & Karasek, R. (1996). Current issues relating to psychosocial job strain and
cardiovascular disease research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, 9-26.
Thought Technology Ltd. (2011). Biograph infiniti software manual. Canada: Author.
Turner, R. N., & Crisp, R. J. (2010). Imagining intergroup contact reduces implicit prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 129-142.
Veltman, J., & Gaillard, A. (1996). Physiological indices of workload in a simulated flight
task. Biological Psychology, 42, 323-342.
Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1999). Meta-analyses of fakability estimates: Implications for
personality measurement. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 197-210.
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The relationship of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41-78.
Wiggins, M., & O'Hare, D. (2003). Weatherwise: Evaluation of a cue-based training approach for the recognition of deteriorating weather conditions during flight. Human
Factors, 45, 337.

Potrebbero piacerti anche