Sei sulla pagina 1di 17

Neuroplasticity After Aquired Brain Injury

By Heidi Reyst, Ph.D., CBIST


Dr. Reyst holds a Ph.D. in Applied Social Psychology from The George Washington University in
Washington, D.C. She is a Certified Brain Injury Specialist Trainer, Academy of Certified Brain
Injury Specialists, and has worked in various capacities within the field of brain injury rehabilitation
since 1991. She currently oversees professional staff allocation, billing and service provision,
professional staff training, accreditation readiness and outcomes management. Dr. Reyst is
currently a member of the board of governors for the Academy of Certified Brain Injury Specialists,
and is the Vice Chairperson for Information Management. She is a member of the American
Psychological Association and is a frequent volunteer for the Brain Injury Association of Michigan.

Annually, 1.7 million people incur a traumatic brain injury (TBI); (Faul, Xu, Wald and
Coronado; 2010) and over 795,000 people sustain a stroke in the U.S. alone (Roger et al.,
2012). Collectively, nearly 2.5 million individuals sustain an acquired brain injury (ABI)
annually. The annual incidence rate of TBI from 2002 to 2006 was 579 people per 100,000
(Faul, Xu, Wald and Coronado; 2010). The corresponding annual incidence rate for stroke
was 189 persons per 100,000 based on a standardized sampling schema (Kleindorfer et
al., 2010). Taken together, the annual incidence rate for TBI and Stroke combined is 768
persons per 100,000. Comparing this number to all cancers combined at 463 persons per
100,000 highlights the significant prevalence of acquired brain injury (Howlader, 2012).
See Figure 1. In light of these numbers, it is critical that the processes underlying ABI
injury as well as the processes modulating recovery are understood. Only then can
treatment and rehabilitation be further refined to enhance recovery.

Figure 1. Number of persons affected per 100,000 (CDC)


Brain injury cascade
When a traumatic brain injury occurs, there are two distinct phases of injury. The first is the
primary insult or injury, where the injury etiology is direct mechanical damage. The second
is the secondary insult or injury, following mechanical damage, with the etiology being a
cascade of pathophysiological processes. Because the cure for the primary phase is
prevention, research has focused on improvement of the second phase processes in
hopes for increasing outcomes post injury (Shlosberg, Benifla, Kaufer and Friedman,
2010). It is also important to note that depending on the mechanism of injury (for example
closed versus penetrating injuries, etc.), the process can differ, as it can depending on
other factors like age, location of primary injury etc. Figure 2outlines the general process
of the TBI cascade.

Figure 2. Injury cascade


Phase One
In the primary phase, injuries typically include direct tissue damage, impaired cerebral
blood flow, and impaired metabolic activity, leading to edema formation and
cytoarchitecture changes like membrane permeability (Werner and Engelhard, 2007).
There are contact forces which cause contusion, hemorrhage and lacerations throughout,
and inertial forces which cause shearing and/or compression of brain tissue (Werner and
Engelhard, 2007). These forces cause multifocal injuries (usually termed diffuse axonal
injury) affecting axons, blood vessels, junctions between white and gray matter, and other
select focal areas like the corpus callosum and junctions between the frontal and parietal
lobes (McAllister, 2011). As a result of direct damage, a cascade of pathological processes
begins.
Phase Two
After the initial injury, neurons are disrupted resulting in depolarization and then a
substantive release of excitatory neurotransmitters (McAllister, 2011; Werner & Engelhard,
2007). This results in release of Ca++ (calcium) and Na+ (sodium) ions, which lead to
intracellular breakdowns. This sets in motion the release of caspases and calpains, both of
which initiate processes leading to cell death. The release of calpains quickly leads to
necrosis where cells die as a response to mechanical or hypoxic damage and metabolic
failure. This leads to an inflammatory response with the cells being removed (Werner &
Engelhard, 2007; McAllister, 2011). The release of caspases initiates the process of
apoptosis (programmed cell death), which can take hours to weeks to progress. Apoptosis,
contrary to necrosis, is an active process, whereby initially intact cells cause cell
membrane disintegration, disruption of cell transport and ultimately cell death (McAllister,
2011).
Throughout the injury processes, there are other critical factors in the injury process
affecting outcome. One is the breakdown of the blood brain barrier (BBB). There can be
direct injury to the BBB in the primary phase and in injury to the endothelium of the BBB in

the secondary phase. This increases permeability of the blood vessels and results in
vascular pathology (Shlosberg, Benifla, Kaufer and Friedman, 2010). Breakdown of the
BBB is implicated in the formation of edema (causing fluid accumulation within the brain),
excitotoxicity, inflammation, and cell death. When the BBB breaks down, an inflammatory
response begins, where injured tissue (and tissue adjacent to it) is eliminated, further
impacting functional outcomes (Werner & Engelhard, 2007). While inflammation is
generally thought to be primarily maladaptive, it is now known that a limited amount of
inflammation plays an essential role for repair after injury (Ziebell and Morganti-Kossmann,
2010).
The processes after stroke are similar to those in TBI. For example, the pathophysiological
cascade (secondary phase) after ischemic stroke includes loss of cell homeostasis,
calcium ion release, neurotransmitter release, excitotoxicity, disruption of the BBB,
reduced cerebral blood flow, inflammation, necrosis, and apoptosis. Thus, after acquired
brain injury, both primary and secondary injuries can lead to significant deficits and
functional problems for individuals. While researchers attempt to find treatments that
ameliorate the secondary injury factors (e.g., progesterone, t-PA, etc.), the main recourse
after brain injury is neuroplasticity.
Neuroplasticity and brain function after acquired brain injury
Probably the easiest way to conceptualize neuroplasticity after injury to the brain is to view
it simply as re-learning (Plowman and Kleim, 2010; Warraich and Kleim, 2010). As Kleim
(2011) noted, the brain will rely on the same fundamental neurobiological process it used
to acquire those behaviors initially. The basic rules governing how neural circuits adapt to
encode new behaviors do not change after injury (p. 522). For example, the changes
seen in the motor cortex after brain injury in response to motor re-learning are the same
motor changes seen in the motor cortex during development of those motor functions.
While we can view re-establishing function as a re-learning process, there are two
conceptual differences when it occurs after a brain injury.
First, because neural circuits for a particular function were previously established during
the brains neurodevelopmental process, it may be possible to take advantage of those
learned behaviors should they persist in residual areas of the brain during the rehabilitation
(Kleim, 2011). This presents as a potentially adaptive circumstance.
Second, a more maladaptive consequence which occurs post injury relates to the concept
of learned non-use. Just as increasing dexterity of motor function leads to increased motor
cortex representation of neural circuitry (and therefore improved function), non-use can
lead to decreased motor cortex representation, and therefore decreased function
(Plowman and Kleim, 2010). Post stroke, research indicates that learned non-use of a
paretic limb, combined with an increased reliance on the unaffected limb can result in
major brain reorganization.
Learned non-use
This occurs when, post stroke, a paretic limb is not used due to the infarct affecting the
area of the primary motor area (M1) controlling that limb. Consequently, the individual
relies heavily on the intact (unaffected) limb. Holding to the maxim of use it or lose it, in

the acute phase after stroke, if the affected limb goes unused, the motor map size
decreases (see previous article titled Neuroplasticity in the intact brain). At the same time,
the unaffected limb is substantially utilized, and the motor map for that area increases in
size. Thus, experience (or lack thereof) impacts the cortical representations of M1 during
the stage of spontaneous recovery, but learned non-use in particular may also be
implicated in a more nefarious manner, as it may be a contributing factor to
interhemispheric imbalance (Takeuchi & Izumi, 2012).
Interhemispheric imbalance
Studies have found that in the affected hemisphere where the infarct or lesion occurred
(termed the ipsilesional hemisphere) there is decreased excitability leading to a reduction
in the likelihood of neurons generating an action potential (which is the precipitant in
neuron-to-neuron firing). The overall result of decreased excitability is a reduction in
neuronal communications within that hemisphere. On the contrary, in the unaffected
hemisphere (termed the contralesional hemisphere) there is increased excitability. Studies
have shown that the over-excitability of the unaffected hemisphere inhibits the excitability
of the affected hemisphere, resulting in decreased motor functioning (Corti et al., 2011).
Learned non-use has been theorized as a contributing factor in interhemispheric
imbalance additionally by the attenuated neuronal activity in the affected hemisphere,
coupled with the greatly increased use of the intact limb driving neuronal activity higher in
the unaffected hemisphere (Takeuchi & Izumi, 2012). Credence is given to this idea, in
that research has shown that if the unaffected hemisphere is artificially inhibited, this leads
to excitability of the affected hemisphere, impacting motor movements positively (PascualLeone, Amedi, Fregni & Merabet, 2005).
As noted above, there is substantial biological change to the brain after focal injury (e.g.,
stroke) and diffuse injury (e.g., TBI). The effect of this biological change is profound. There
can be damage to the tissue directly, due to the loss of oxygen resulting from a stroke, or
due to inert forces like in a traumatic injury. In addition to these direct effects, additional,
and potentially equally damaging biological changes occur at sites of the brain both distant
and close to the lesioned areas. This includes the inflammatory process, attenuated blood
flow, changes to metabolic processes, edema, and neuronal excitability (Kleim, 2011).
These cascade processes result in disruption to intact areas of the brain particularly those
areas with connectivity to the injured regions, and has been termed diaschisis.
Diaschisis is in essence a disturbance or loss of function in one part of the brain due to a
localized injury in another part of the brain, and these areas can be of considerable
distance from the lesioned area including the opposite hemisphere (Stein, 2012). One
effect post stroke that affects function within the brain considerably is hyper-excitability in
the opposite hemisphere. This, coupled with under-excitability in the damaged hemisphere
results overall in a disrupted neural network (Pascual-Leone, Amedi, Fregni and Merabet,
2005). Research has shown that these changes can occur up to 12 months after the initial
injury (Cramer and Riley, 2008). With the likelihood of widespread neural dysfunction after
injury, what then are the mechanisms for recovery?

Mechanisms of recovery
After injury to the brain, there are two mechanisms whereby functional improvement may
occur. These are recovery and compensation (Kleim, 2007). Using World Health
Organization definitions,
Recovery relates to:
1. Restoration of neural tissue initially perturbed after the injury (neural level)
2. Restoration of movement exactly as it was performed prior (behavioral level)
3. Restoration of activity exactly as it was performed prior (activity level)
Compensation refers to:
1. Recruitment of new neural circuits (neural level)
2. Training of new movement sequences (behavioral level)
3. Training of activity in a new way after injury (activity level)
Recovery therefore relates to lost functions being restored, and compensation relates to
the acquisition of new functions or behaviors to replace those lost after injury (Kleim,
2011). Research has shown that after a stroke, for motor deficits, notable recovery takes
place within 30 days for mild, moderate, and moderate-severe severity with additional
recovery up to 90 days for severe strokes (Duncan, P., Goldstein, L., Matchar, D., Divine,
G. and Feussner, J., 1992). These times frames are similar with other areas of dysfunction
where the final level of language function was achieved within six weeks post stroke for
95% of patients (with mild, moderate and severe aphasia; Pedersen, Jorgenson,
Nakayama, Raaschou and Olsen, 1995). The level of recovery from spatial neglect was
maximized within nine weeks (Hier, Mondlock and Caplan, 1983, cited in Cramer and
Riley, 2008). With these types of findings, what is the neurobiological explanation of these
changes early on post-injury?
Neurobiological plasticity changes during recovery
Figure 3 displays a model that incorporates a two-stage process of recovery, and within
those two stages, provides the neural strategies utilized within the central nervous system.
The first stage is Spontaneous Recovery, and the second stage is Training Induced
Recovery (Chen, Epstein and Stern, 2010). Depending on the stage of recovery, different
neural mechanisms are at work to either initiate recovery strategies or in response to
changes in experience in the form of training or rehabilitation. Each aspect of the model is
described below.

Figure 3. Two-stage model of recovery with corresponding neurological strategies and


recovery vs. compensation distinctions.
STAGE ONE: Spontaneous Recovery
With spontaneous recovery, even in the absence of training or rehabilitation, there is
resolution of injury and functional change in close time proximity after injury which plateaus
within three months for focal injury and six months for diffuse injury (Chen, Epstein and
Stern, 2010). Within that time frame three processes have been theorized to explain this
early recovery after injury when specific intervention has not ensued (Dancause and Nudo;
2011). They are:
1. Diaschisis reversal
2. Changes in kinematics.
3. Cortical reorganization.
Diaschisis Reversal
Diaschisis as previously described begins to resolve, whereby the inflammatory process,
blood flow changes, metabolic changes, edema, and neuronal excitability begin to subside
(Warraich and Kleim, 2010). The result of diaschisis reversal is improved function due to
intact brain areas that were previously disrupted now being restored. Restoration is
therefore a crucial neural strategy after injury. From a purely neurobiological level, this
may be thought of as the only true level of recovery in the strictest sense of the word, in
that the same brain circuits are facilitating function post injury as they were pre injury.
Restoration has been found in both cognitive (e.g., language and attention) and physical
(e.g., motor movement) domains (Kleim, 2011).
Changes in Kinematics
The second aspect of early recovery relates to changes in kinematic (movement) patterns
where compensatory patterns are utilized. The individual intrinsically begins to complete
motor movements in a different manner, resulting in improved function, sometimes in
drastically different ways than prior to injury. While these new movements likely contribute

to functional improvement, these compensatory strategies have the potential to be


maladaptive.
Cortical Reorganization
The third strategy identified as spontaneous recovery is that the nervous system
undergoes within-area and between-area reorganization or rewiring. For example, many
researchers have found elements of neuroplasticity near the infarct area after stroke,
including cortical reorganization, neurogenesis, axonal sprouting, dendritic plasticity, new
blood vessel formation (Kerr, Cheng and Jones, 2011), as well as excitability changes
(Nudo, 2011). Chen, Epstein and Stern (2010) outlined neural shifts in recruitment of brain
areas in the spontaneous recovery period. Soon after stroke, in homologus (similar) areas,
the opposite side of the brain is recruited. Later on during spontaneous recovery, there is a
shift in activation back to the injury side. An example would be if the left-sided language
area (Brocas area) was damaged, the right-sided equivalent Brocas area would be
recruited. After a period of time, it would then shift back to the left side.
Another key change in brain function relates to activation of learning networks in the early
phase, where plasticity similar to when the brain was developing is induced. This includes
motor control and task-learning networks (Chen, Epstein and Stern, 2010).
Overall, cortical reorganization during spontaneous recovery is thought to be
compensatory as different circuits or networks of neurons are utilized post injury than
those utilized pre injury. While spontaneous recovery occurs in the absence of
rehabilitation, there is certainly the opportunity for overlap of training induced recovery
while spontaneous recovery takes its course.
STAGE TWO: Training-induced recovery
Training in the form of rehabilitation can induce plasticity post injury, but is not necessarily
time limited like spontaneous recovery processes demonstrate (Chen, Epstein and Stern,
2010). Recovery in this stage involves compensation, in that either new brain areas or
neural networks are enlisted to complete previous functions. Through the process of
training, neuroplasticity is induced. Chen, Epstein and Stern (2010) note that adaptive
changes after injury are the outcome of new patterns of activation which include plasticity
in areas surrounding the damaged cortex, reorganization of existing networks or
recruitment of new cortical areas or networks.
Recruitment
During training-induced recovery, areas which did not make a significant contribution to
that particular function pre-injury now contribute to function post-injury (Kleim, 2011). Often
times this may be in the form of recruitment of neural areas from the undamaged
hemisphere. From a physical perspective, this may include changes in motor maps where
the non-injured hemisphere motor cortex can play a distinct role in producing motor
movements in an impaired limb, which was previously controlled by the injured motor
cortex. From a cognitive perspective, neural recruitment may entail the enlistment of the
right side homologue (similar) to Brocas area to improve language function if Brocas area
(left frontal lobe) is damaged. Rehabilitation to induce such changes may involve
constraint induced manual therapy or completion of cognitive tasks while using complex

hand movements in the opposite hemisphere which promotes a shift to the uninjured
hemisphere.
Retraining
Retraining involves the training of residual brain areas, resulting in reorganization within
the cortex and compensation for lost function (Kleim, 2007). This often comes in the forms
of reorganization within the damaged hemisphere. In the case of motor function, if tissue is
lost which controlled finger movements, other cortical tissue nearby can reorganize to
control that lost movement.
Ultimately, recruitment and retraining involve rewiring or reorganization of neural networks.
What then are the properties of the brain which, after injury, provide the mechanisms for
recovery? Two basic properties provide us the answer:
The first is that our brains have a tremendous amount of redundancy. There is internal
redundancy in areas like the primary visual cortex, the somatosensory areas, the primary
auditory cortex and the primary motor cortex (Warraich and Kleim, 2010). So within
primary cortex areas there may be multiple areas that respond to the same or similar
stimuli. External redundancy refers to similar functionality being processed across different
areas of the brain (Warraich and Kleim, 2010). Both of these redundancies allow for better
information integration, but they also provide a pathway to improved function after brain
injury.
The second property relates to a concept discussed in the previous article; that of
experience dependent plasticity. This is where changes in behavior or experience result in
changes at a neurobiological level.
Neurobiological Changes after Acquired Brain Injury
After injury to the brain, the processes of neuroplasticity are thought to be the
underpinnings of recovery (Carmichael, 2010). To begin, research has found a variety of
neuroplastic changes which occur after injury, including:
1. Increases
or
changes
to
synapses:
This includes synaptogenesis and synaptic plasticity (Chen, Epstein and Stern;
2010; Nudo, 2011) Dendrite changes including increased arborization, dendritic
growth and spine growth (Nudo, 2011) Axonal changes including axonal sprouting
(Nudo, 2011; Charmichael, 2010)
2. Increased
neuron
growth:
Neurogenesis in specific brain areas like the hippocampus subgranular zone of
the dentate gyrus and subventricular zone in some areas (Schoch, Madathil and
Saatman, 2012), substantia nigra and perinfarcted areas (Font, Arboix & Krupinski,
2010).
3. Angiogenesis
Angiogenesis is the process through which new blood vessels form from preexisting vessels.
4. Excitability
changes:
Excitability refers to the ability of a neuron to generate action potentials, which is
a short-term change in the electrical potential on the surface of a cell. It is an all or

nothing proposition as it either fires or does not fire depending on the strength of
the potential.
The first two items on the list above relate to increases in either the number of neurons
(this occurs in a very limited sense) or the numbers of synapses or increased strength of
existing synapses (this far more prevalent). These changes seen post injury mirror
changes seen in the intact brain in the form of experience dependent learning. But instead
of it being a learning process, it is a relearning process, aided substantially by
rehabilitation.
With experience dependent learning, new synapses form (synaptogenesis) or strengthen
through changes in dendrites (new dendritic spine formation), axonal sprouting and long
term potentiation (synaptic plasticity). Both synaptogenesis and synaptic plasticity are the
main underpinnings of cortical reorganization, recruitment and retraining as identified in
Mechanisms of Recovery above. For a general overview of experience dependent learning
see the side bar on page 35. For a detailed overview of both synaptogenesis and synaptic
plasticity, see the previous article titled Neuroplasticity in the Intact Brain: ExperienceDependent Learning and Neurobiological Substrates.
The third and fourth items on the list relate to changes in excitability homeostasis within
the brain (electrophysiological balance across the two hemispheres) and new blood vessel
formation. These are described further in the next section.

Figure 4. Dendritic Arbor Expansion and Retraction.


Findings Related to Neurobiological Changes
Synaptic, Dendritic and Axonal Related Changes
Perederiy and Westbrook (2013) reported post injury that researchers found when an area
of the brain stops receiving inputs from the body via afferent nerves, the dendritic arbor
retracts (Figure 4). This results in the loss of synapses with other neurons. On the other

hand they also reported that in areas of the brain not affected after injury, dendritic arbors
increased (Figure 4). This former finding indicates a maladaptive response after injury,
while the latter finding reflects the brains response post injury to increase synapses in
intact areas, thereby providing cortical reorganization or rewiring, which is an adaptive
response.
Axonal sprouting and reorganization occurs post injury. This sprouting has adaptive
consequences in that increased axonal growth leads to greater levels of synapses allowing
reinnervation (Perederiy & Westbrook, 2013). Re-innervation can then lead to adaptive
changes. However, there are issues with axonal regeneration in that glial scars can
prevent axons from reaching their target, and for patients with temporal lobe epilepsy,
specific axonal sprouts can synapse onto granule cells which may relate to the recurrence
of seizures (Perederiy & Westbrook, 2013).
Research has found that there may be changes within the damaged hemisphere. For
example in motor areas, topographical map changes occur, where different areas
controlling motor movements compensate for the damaged areas. The neurobiological
foundation of motor map changes is synaptic change. This includes synaptogenesis where
new synapses form through dendritic growth and axonal sprouting, and synaptic plasticity
which strengthens existing synapses through the process of long-term potentiation
(see previous article for a description).
Nudo, Wise, SiFuentes and Milliken (1996) mapped the motor areas of monkeys to
determine the areas of the brain which controlled hand motor movements. After training on
a skilled-hand task, infarcts were induced in the monkeys mapped motor area. The
monkeys were then retrained on the same skilled task. Initially, the monkeys demonstrated
significant deficits on the skilled-hand task. After retraining, however, their skills
substantially improved, and this related to significant changes to their motor maps.
Specifically the hand and digit areas increased significantly during spontaneous recovery
between the injured monkeys and a control group. In addition, for monkeys who received
re-training, there was no loss of spared hand motor map in nearby intact areas, suggesting
that therapy prevented further loss of hand areas representation.
Angiogenesis
Angiogenesis is the process through which new blood vessels form from pre-existing
vessels. In ischemic stroke, which is loss of blood flow leading to neuronal death,
increased vasculature relates to increase circulation (Font, Arboix, Krupinski, 2010). The
benefit is return of blood flow to previously damaged areas, which is assists in establishing
metabolic support (Krum, Mani, & Rosenstein, 2008).
In a review assessing research on neurovascular response after stroke, Arai, Jin,
Navaratna & Lo (2009) examined the role of angiogenesis. The authors distinguish injury
in the acute phase where neurovascular damage causes the primary disruption of the
blood brain barrier. After stroke, it is now widely held that the penumbra (which is an area
around the infarct affected by vascular compromise) is more than just dying cells it may
be a precursor of neuroplasticity. In the delayed phase after acute stroke, angiogenesis
and neurogenesis, which are closely tied together, are primary responses post stroke. One
cytokine of note relating to angiogenesis is vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF),
which in its endogenous form relates to brain neuroprotection. Krum, Mani and Rosenstein

(2008), found that VEGF is an important factor in post-injury recovery. In particular, by


blocking VEGF receptors, preventing them from upregulating, they found that vascular
proliferation was decreased. By blocking VEGF, and showing a clear decrease in positive
vascular changes, they were able to isolate its effect vascular remodeling (i.e.,
angiogenesis).
Changes to Network Organization
While reorganization of neural networks has been found post injury, the amount of
reorganization depends on the size of the injured area. For example, with areas of smaller
damage, reorganization tends to occur close to the injury area. For larger areas of
damage, reorganization or recruitment is more widespread to other areas of the brain
(Chen, Epstein and Stern; 2010).
Schlaug, Marchina and Norton (2009), using melodic intonation therapy to treat aphasia
found that after intensive treatment, significant white matter changes occurred. In
particular, through use of diffusion tensor imaging (which detects functionality of white
matter tracts) they found increases in the right arcuate fasiculus, which is a white matter
tract connecting Wernickes area and Brocas area. Key to this finding is that the right
arcuate fasiculus is not typically well developed, indicating that the right hemisphere
reorganized to improve function. Another important factor in this finding is that increases in
the number of fibers in the arcuate fasiculus correlated with measurable improvement in
conversational skills.
Activation and Excitatory Changes
After injury, changes in the excitability of the damaged and intact hemispheres can impact
cortical functioning. Excitatory changes across hemispheres can occur quickly after brain
injury, where cortical excitability in the affected areas is generally decreased. A model of
interhemispheric rivalry has been suggested, where there are distinct differences in the
excitability of analogous areas between hemispheres (e.g., motor areas). For example in
the damaged hemisphere there is hyperpolarization (inhibition of neurons) and in the intact
hemisphere there is depolarization (excitation of neurons; Bolognini, Pascual-Leone &
Fregni, 2009). Calautti & Baron (2003) reported that in the chronic phase after stroke,
researchers found that better recovery was found if activation of the affected-side is more
predominant than the unaffected hemisphere over time. This shift of activation to the
unaffected side is the sign of a distressed system (Cramer et al., 2011, p. 1593). So from
a long term perspective if the damaged side was more involved in function, that related to
better outcomes. However, if the patient had to rely on the unaffected side more for
function, that related to poorer outcomes.
In a study on memory and attention deficits after damage to the prefrontal cortex (PFC) by
Voytek, Davis, Yago, Barcelo, Vogel and Knight (2010) they found evidence that the PFC
in the undamaged hemisphere compensates for the damaged PFC areas in the opposite
hemisphere on a trial by trial basis dependent on cognitive load (p. 401). In other words,
the undamaged hemisphere dynamically compensates for the damaged hemisphere
depending on the level of challenge the damaged hemisphere must deal with. This
demonstrates that the intact hemisphere can adapt rapidly and that it is not an all or
nothing proposition, where function is relegated to either the intact or damaged
hemisphere post injury.

Collectively, this research highlights processes post injury which are similar to those
neuroplastic changes in the intact brain. Namely, that when experience in the form of
changes to nerve inputs or motor outputs occurs, cortical changes like those of experience
dependent learning occurs where synaptogenesis, synaptic plasticity, and axonal
sprouting take place. Furthermore, after injury, through aspects of the injury cascade,
certain adaptive processes initiate, resulting in changes like angiogenesis, and network
reorganization changes. Neuroplasticity is a remarkable tool in our cortical toolbox. And,
like many other adaptive tools, it too can have maladaptive consequences.
The maladaptive side of neuroplasticity
While there is huge upside to neuroplasticity, we cannot afford to overlook the downside.
While beyond the scope of this article to go into any depth there are plenty of examples to
point out that neuroplasticity has its dark side, too. Just a few examples include addictions
to alcohol, elicit substances or prescription drugs, pornography addictions (Doidge, 2007),
seizure disorders post injury (Cramer et al., 2011), phantom limb pain (Doidge, 2007),
hand dystonias in musicians (Candia, Rosset-LLobet, Elbert and Pascual-Leone, 2005),
learning and memory interference (Carmichael, 2010) and chronic pain (Cramer et al.,
2011). Thus, while we search for adaptive examples of neuroplasticity and ways to
promote it both in the intact brain and after injury, we must also seek to prevent these
brain changes which can have profound impacts on function, not to mention societal
implications.
Final thoughts

Figure 5. Synapses
In an article put together by 27 leading neuroscientists from the National Institutes of
Health Blueprint for Neuroscience Research (Cramer et al., 2011), they noted that
[n]europlasticity occurs with many variations, in many forms, and in many contexts (p.
1952). This reminds us that brain injury in all of its forms is quite heterogeneous. The host
of variables which affect outcomes after acquired injury are vast and varied (e.g., age,
lesion area, pre-injury characteristics, genetic profile, etc.).

Yet with all of this heterogeneity, there are similar neuroplastic processes after injury.
Cramer et al. (2011) write that common themes in plasticity that emerge across diverse
central nervous system conditions include experience dependence, time sensitivity and the
importance of motivation and attention (p. 1952). Thus it is important that neuroscience
and its practitioners continue to identify at the key factors which contribute to neuroplastic
changes be it at the molecular, cellular, architectural, behavioral or network level.

As we better understand the neurobiological level of neuroplasticity, we can then begin to


better understand how to harness treatments that enhance the recovery process and
ultimately patient function. There is currently a tremendous amount of research
addressing neuroplasticity at both a basic and applied level. Both are needed to continue
to learn more about effective treatments. Some are pharmacologic and focus on drugs or
molecules that may impact the secondary phase of injury, or that prime the central
nervous system in preparation of traditional neurorehabilitation in the form of occupational
therapy, physical therapy, and speech & language pathology. Such priming includes noninvasive brain stimulation (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation, transcranial direct
simulation), deep brain stimulation, and neuropharmacology. Others focus on the timing of
rehabilitation efforts, in order to maximize plastic states where the opportunity for recovery
is at its highest. Still others focus on the principles of Hebbian learning wherein
understanding how experience shapes the brain can best be utilized in any form of
treatment.
In closing, neuroplasticity is not an idea, it is a state. This state exists from our earliest
years of neurodevelopment (prenatal and postnatal), to our ongoing changing brains as a
result of experience, to changes after injuries to our most precious resource, our brain. Our
transformative experiences shape and mold our neurochemicals, axons, dendritic arbors
and spines, motor maps, cortical networksin other words, our very essence. And
likewise, when our very essence is transformed via injury, the change to our networks and
synapses then shape our experiences. How we can best reclaim those experiences by
harnessing neuroplasticity is the focus of an article in the next issue of RainbowVisions
Magazine.

Overview of Experience-Dependent Learning


For neurons or networks of neurons to communicate they need to have extensive
connections with one another (or, quite literally hundreds to thousands of connections for
each neuron.) These extraordinarily complex connections require junctions or connections
termed synapses.
For example, neuron A connects via its axon terminal to neuron B at its dendrite
(see Figure 5). The space between the axon and dendrite is the synapse. In a basic
sense, the greater the number of synapses, the greater and stronger the connections
between neurons. Likewise, the more synapses, dendrites and axons that develop, the
greater the opportunity to connect more neurons together and strengthen the existing
connections.

As our experiences change, at a neurobiological level, we either increase or decrease the


numbers of synapses, dendrites and axons. If we stop a function, we lose synapses, etc.,
and if we increase an activity we proliferate synapses, etc., resulting in experiencedependent learning.
Abbott, L. and Nelson S. (2000). Synaptic Plasticity: Taming the Beast. Nature Neuroscience
Supplement, 3, 1178-1183.
Arai, K., Jin, G., Navataratna, D. and Lo, E. (2009). Brain angiogenesis in developmental and
pathological processes: Neurovascular injury and angiogenic recovery after stroke. Federation of
European Biochemical Societies, 276, pp. 4644-4652.
Bach y Rita, P., Collins, C., Saunders, F., White, B., Scadden, L. (1969). Vision substitution by
tactile image projection. Nature, 221, pp. 963-964.
Bolognini, N., Pascual-Leone, A. and Fregni, F. (2009). Using non-invasive brain stimulation to
augment motor training-induced plasticity. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 6:8.
Bosch, M and Hayashi, Y. (2011. Structural plasticity of dendritic spines. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 22, pp. 1-6).
Candia V., Rosset-Llobet J., Elbert T. and Pascual-Leone A. (2005), Changing the Brain through
Therapy for Musicians Hand Dystonia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, pp.
335342.
Calautti, C. and Baron J. (2003). Functional neuroimaging studies of motor recovery after stroke in
adults: A review. Stroke, 34, pp. 1553-1566.
Carmichael, S. (2010). Translating the frontiers of brain repair to treatments: Starting not to break
the rules. Neurobiology of Disease, 37(2), pp. 1-10.
Chen, H., Epstein, J. and Stern, E. (2010). Neural plasticity after acquired brain injury: Evidence
from functional imaging. PM & R, supplement 2, pp. s306-s312.
Corti, M, Patten, C and Triggs, W. (2011). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of motor
cortex after stroke. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91 (3), pp. 254-270.
Cramer, S. and Riley, D. (2008). Neuroplasticity and brain repair after stroke. Current Opinion in
Neurology, 21, pp. 76-82.
Cramer, S., Sur, M., Dobkin, B., OBrien, C., Sanger, T., Tojanowski, J., Rumsey, J., (2011).
Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications. Brain, 134, pp. 1591-1609.
Dancause N., Nudo R. J. (2011). Shaping plasticity to enhance recovery after injury. Prog. Brain
Res. 192, 273295.
Doidge, N. (2007). The brain that changes itself: Stories of personal triumph from the frontiers of
brain science. Viking Penguin: New York, NY.
Duncan, P., Goldstein, L., Matchar, D., Divine, G. and Feussner, J., (1992). Measurement of motor
recovery after stroke. Outcome assessment and sample size requirements. Stroke, 23, pp. 10841089.

Faul M, Xu L, Wald MM, Coronado VG. (2010). Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States:
Emergency Department Visits, Hospitalizations and Deaths 20022006. Atlanta (GA): Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control.
Finger, S., Beyer, T. and Koehler, P. (2000). Dr. Otto Soltmann (1876) on development of the motor
cortex and recovery after its removal in infancy. Brain Research Bulletin, 53 (2), pp. 133-140.
Font, A., Arboix, A. and Krupinski, J. (2010). Angiogenesis, neurogenesis and neuroplasticity in
ischemic stroke. Current Cardiology Reviews, 6, pp. 238-244.
Franz, E. and Gillett, G. (2011). John Hughlings Jacksons evolutionary neurology: a unifying
framework for cognitive neuroscience. Brain, 134, pp. 3114-3120.
Harris, K. and Kater, S. (1994). Dendritic spines: Cellular specialization imparting both stability and
flexibility to synaptic function. Annual Review of Neurology, 17, pp. 341-371.
Hill, T. and Zito, K. (2013). LTP- induced long term stabilization of individual nascent dendritic
spines. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), pp. 678-686.
Howlader N., Noone A., Krapcho M., Garshell J., Neyman N., Altekruse S., Kosary C., Yu M., Ruhl
J., Tatalovich Z., Cho H., Mariotto A., Lewis D., Chen H., Feuer E.,Cronin K. (eds). SEER Cancer
Statistics Review, 1975-2010, National Cancer Institute. Bethesda, MD,
Kerr, A., Cheng, S. and Jones, T. (2011). Experience dependent neural plasticity in the adult
damaged brain. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, pp. 538-548.
Kleim, J., Barbay, S. and Nudo, R. (1998). Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortex
following motor skill learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, pp. 3321-3325.
Kleim, J., Barbay, S., Cooper, N., Hogg, T., Reidel, C., Remple, M. and Nudo, R. (2002). Motor
learning-dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex.
Neurobiology of learning and Memory, 77, 63-77.
Kleim, J., Hogg, T., Vandenberg, P., Cooper, N., Bruneau, R. and Remple, M. (2004). Cortical
synaptogenesis and motor map reorganization occur during late, but not early, phase of motor skill
learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(3), pp. 628-633.
Kleim, J. (2007). Role of neural plasticity in motor learning and motor recovery after brain injury.
Pocketbook of Neurological Physiotherapy, S. Lennon, M. Stokes: Elsevier, 41-50.
Kleim, J. (2011). Neural plasticity and neurorehabilitation: Teaching the new brain old tricks. Journal
of Communication Disorders, 44, pp. 521-528.
Kleindorfer, D., Khoury, J., Moomaw, C., Alwell, K., Woo, D., Flaherty, M., Khatari, P., Adeoye, O.,
Ferioli, S., Broderick, J. and Kissela, B. (2010). Stroke incidence is decreasing in whites but not in
blacks. Stroke, 41, 1326-1331.
Krum, J., Mani, N., Rosenstein, M. (2008). Roles of endogenous VEGF receptors flt-1 and flk-1 in
astroglial and vascular remodeling after brain injury. Experimental Neurology, 212(1), pp. 108-117.
Koleske, A. (2013). Molecular mechanisms of dendrite stability. Nature, 14, pp. 536-550.

McCallister, A. (2000). Cellular and molecular mechanisms of dendrite growth. Cerebral Cortex, 10,
pp. 963-973.
McCallister, T. (2011). Neurobiological consequences of traumatic brain injury. Dialogues in Clinical
Neuroscience, 13, pp. 287-300.
Merzenich, M., Kaas, J., Wall, J., Sur, M., Nelson, J. and Felleman, D (1983). Progression of
change following median nerve section in the cortical representation of the hand in areas 3b and 1
in adult owl and squirrel monkeys.
Neuroscience, 10(3), PP. 639-665. Merzenich, M., Nelson, R., Stryker, M., Cynader, M.,
Schoppmann, A. and Zook, J. (1984). Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit
amputation in adult monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 224, pp. 591-605.
Mizui, T. and Kojima, M. (2013). BDNF and synaptic plasticity: The recent cell biology for
understanding of brain disorders. Clinical Pharmacology and Biopharmaceuticals, S1:004, pp. 1-5.
Nudo, R., Milliken, G., Jenkins, W. and Merzenich, M. (1996). Use-dependent alterations of
movement representations in primary motor cortex adult squirrel monkeys. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 16(2), pp. 785-807.
Nudo, R., Wise, B., SiFuentes, F. and Milliken, G. (1996). Neural Substrates for the Effects of
Rehabilitative Training on Motor Recovery After lschemic Infarct. Science, 272, pp. 1791-1794.
Nudo, R. (2011). Neural bases of recovery after brain injury. Journal of Communication Disorders,
44, pp. 515-520.
Oe, Y., Tominaga-Yoshino, K., Hasegawa, S. and Ogura, A. (2013). Dendritic spine dynamics in
synaptogenesis after repeated LTP inductions: Dependence on pre-existing spine density.
Scientific Reports, 3:1957, pp. 1-8.
Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F. and Merabet, L. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, pp. 377-401.
Pascual-Leone, A., Nguyet, D., Cohen, L., Brasil-Neto, J., Cammarota, A. and Hallett, M. (1995).
Modulation of muscle response evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition
of new fine motor skills. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, pp. 1037-1045.
Pearce, J. (2009). Marie-Jean-Pierre Flourens (1794-1867) and cortical localization. European
Neurology, 61, pp. 311-314.
Pedersen, P. M., Stig Jrgensen, H., Nakayama, H., Raaschou, H. O. and Olsen, T. S. (1995),
Aphasia in acute stroke: Incidence, determinants, and recovery. Annals of Neurology, 38, pp. 659
666.
Perederiy, J. and Westbrook, G. (2013). Structural plasticity in the dentate gyrus revisiting a
classic injury model. Frontiers in Neural Ciruits, Plowman, E. and Kleim, J. (2010). Motor cortex
reorganization across the lifespan. Journal of communication disorders, 43, pp. 286-294.
Raisman, G. (1969). Neuronal plasticity in the septal nuclei of the adult rat. Brain Research, 145,
pp. 25-48.

Roger, V., Go, A., Lloyd-Jones, D., Benjamin, E., Berry, J., Bordon, W., et al. (2012). Heart
Disease and Stroke Statistics2012 Update: A Report From the American Heart Association.
Circulation, 125, pp. e2-e220.
Schoch, M., Madathil, S. and Saatman, K. (2012). Genetic manipulation of cell death and
neuroplasticity pathways in traumatic brain injury. Neurotherapeutics, 9, pp. 323-337.
Schlaug, G., Marchina, S. and Norton, A. (2009). Evidence for Plasticity in White Matter Tracts of
Chronic Aphasic Patients Undergoing Intense Intonation-based Speech Therapy Ann N Y Acad Sci.
1169, pp. 385394.
Schlosberg, D., Benifla, M., Kaufer, D. and Friedman, A. (2010). Blood-brain barrier breakdown as a
therapeutic target in TBI. Nature Reviews Neurology, 6(7), pp. 393-403.
Stein, D. (2012). Concepts of central nervous system plasticity and their implications for recovery
after brain damage. Brain Injury Medicine: Principles and Practice: eds. Nathan D. Zasler MD,
Douglas I. Katz MD, Ross D. Zafonte DO, David B. Arciniegas MD, M. Ross Bullock MD, PHD,
Jeffrey S. Kreutzer PHD, ABPP; pp. 162-174.
Takeuchi, N. and Izumi, S. (2012). Maladaptive plasticity for motor recovery after stroke:
Mechanisms and approaches. Neural Plasticity, 2012, pp. 1-9.
Toni, N., Teng, E., Bushong, E., Aimone, J., Zhao, C., Consiglio, A., Praag, H., , (2007). Synapse
formation on neurons born in the adult hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 10, pp. 727-734.
Venkataramani, P. (2010). Santiago Ramon y Cajal: Father of Neuroscience. Resonance,
November, pp. 968-975.
Voytek, B., Davis, M., Yago, E., Barcelo, F., Vogel, E. and Knight, R. (2010). Dynamic
Neuroplasticity after Human Prefrontal Cortex Damage. Neuron, 68, pp. 401-408.
Warraich, Z. and Kleim, J. (2010). Neural plasticity: The biological substrate for neurorehabilitation.
PM & R, 2, S208-S219.
Werner, C. and Engelhard, K. (2007). Pathophysiology of traumatic brain injury. British Journal of
Anesthesia, 99, pp. 4-9.
Yildirim, F. and Sarkcioglu, L. (2007). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 78(8), p.
852.
York, G. and Steinberg, D. (2011). Hughlings Jacksons neurological ideas. Brain, 134, pp. 31063113.
Yuste, R. and Bonhoeffer, T. (2001). Morphological changes in dendritic spines associated with
long-term synaptic plasticity. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 24, pp. 1071-1089.
Ziebell, J. and Morganti-Kossman, M. (2010). Involvement of pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines
and chemokines in the pathophysiology of traumatic brain injury. Neurotherapeutics, 7, 22-30.

Potrebbero piacerti anche