Sei sulla pagina 1di 2

Comments 251

Eagly (2012), evolutionary approaches should not be placed in


opposition to studies that focus on the role of social factors. The
clearest understanding of sex differences in behaviour is likely to
result from considering the complex interactions between social
factors, predispositions, and developmental environments. Both
social factors and neuroendocrine responses, for instance, could
jointly influence womens performance on preference tasks, particularly given that developing neuroendocrine systems are sensitive to external environments. Current debates within the
evolutionary biology literature that question the usefulness of separating proximate and ultimate questions (e.g., Laland, Sterelny,
Odling-Smee, Hoppitt, & Uller, 2011) are highly relevant to
researchers studying human behaviour, including mating strategies.
In summary, we are supportive of the use of meta-analyses,
and we share Wood et al.s (2014) caution about inferring
domain-specific, evolved psychological mechanisms on the
basis of cycle shifts. Critics of evolutionary psychology have
more generally argued that inadequate consideration of domaingeneral mechanisms and the role of culture provides a misleading view of how the human mind is constructed (e.g., Bolhuis,
Brown, Richardson, & Laland, 2011). The target article provides an excellent opportunity to think more broadly about how
cycle shifts might be interpreted in the future and to explore the
middle ground between the current opposing viewpoints.

References
Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R., Richardson, R. C., & Laland, K. N. (2011).
Darwin in mind: New opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLoS
Biology, 9, e1001109. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001109

Derntl, B., Kryspin-Exner, I., Fernbach, E., Moser, E., & Habel, U. (2008).
Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated
with cycle phase. Hormones and Behavior, 53, 9095. doi:10.1016/j.
yhbeh.2007.09.006
Dreher, J.-C., Schmidt, P. J., Kohn, P., Furman, D., Rubinow, D., &
Berman, K. F. (2007). Menstrual cycle phase modulates rewardrelated neural function in women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 24652470.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0605569104
Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W. J. E., & Uller,
T. (2011). Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayrs proximate
ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science, 334, 15121516. doi:10.1126/
science.1210879
Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Caldwell, C. A. (2011).
Social learning and human mate preferences: A potential mechanism
for generating and maintaining between-population diversity in attraction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B,
366, 14712970. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0192
Pearson, M., & Schipper, B. C. (2013). Menstrual cycle and competitive
bidding. Games and Economic Behavior, 78, 120. doi:10.1016/j.
geb.2012.10.008
Scott, I. M. L., Clark, A. P., Boothroyd, L. G., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2013).
Do mens faces really signal heritable immunocompetence? Behavioral
Ecology, 24, 579589. doi:10.1093/beheco/ars092
van Wingen, G. A., Ossewaarde, L., Bckstrm, T., Hermans, E. J., &
Fernandez, G. (2011). Gonadal hormone regulation of the emotion
circuitry in humans. Neuroscience, 191, 3845. doi:/10.1016/j.
neuroscience.2011.04.042
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differences and similarities in behavior. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 46, 55123. doi:10.1016/B978012394281
4.000027
Wood, W., Kressel, L., Joshi, P., & Louie, B. (2014). Meta-analysis of menstrual
cycle effects on womens mate preferences. Emotion Review, 6, 229249.

Comment: Why Meta-Analyses Rarely Resolve Ideological Debates


Christopher J. Ferguson

Department of Psychology, Stetson University, USA

Abstract

as fatally flawed by those in disagreement. As such, meta-analyses have failed


in replacing narrative reviews as more objective.

In their meta-analysis Wood, Kressel, Joshi, and Louie (2014) argue little
evidence supports shifts in mating preferences across the menstrual cycle.
They imply this may represent a critical weakness of evolutionary psychology
theories of mating preferences more generally. This report represents a fairly
common use of meta-analysis: to assemble data to support or reject a particular
proposition over which there is debate. Yet, rarely do meta-analyses succeed at
resolving ideological debates. Multiple decision points related to the selection,
coding, effect size extraction, and interpretation of studies leaves considerable
room for meta-analytic authors to interject their own beliefs. Meta-analyses are
typically hailed by those who agreed a priori with their conclusion, and rejected

Keywords
evolutionary psychology, mating preferences, meta-analysis

In their meta-analytic review Wood et al. (2014), argue research


evidence refutes that womens mating preferences shift across the
menstrual cycle. Further, the authors interpret this as a problematic
finding for evolutionary psychology more generally. The metaanalysis by Wood et al. appears to be a competently executed

Corresponding author: Christopher J. Ferguson, Department of Psychology, Stetson University, 421 N. Woodland Blvd., DeLand, FL 32729, USA. Email: CJFerguson1111@aol.com

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com at University College London on September 24, 2016

252 Emotion Review Vol. 6 No. 3

analysis. I will note upfront several things. First, my a priori position on evolution is that evolution is a powerful determinant of our
behavior. Second, I served as a reviewer for this meta-analysis and
recommended its publication. Third, although the meta-analysis
by Wood et al. appears to be well done, I remain skeptical it will
resolve academic debates related to evolutionary psychology.
The purpose behind the development of meta-analysis was to
replace the subjective nature of narrative reviews which often
reflected a researchers individual biases (Quintana & Minami,
2006). Narrative reviews often were used to advance particular
ideological positions and thus competing narrative reviews
typically reflected the individual scholar rather than the actual
data. By quantifying the data available across a field of study, it
was thought, it would be possible to eliminate the subjectivity of
narrative reviews.
Having both conducted meta-analyses and watched debates
in other research areas, I struggle to think of an ideological
debate that was resolved through the use of meta-analysis.
Typically, proponents of the opposing view simply reject a
meta-analysis out of hand as fatally flawed. Just a small subset
of examples includes debates over the validity of fMRI studies
(Vul, Harris, Winkielman, & Pashler, 2009), an alleged narcissism epidemic (Trzesniewski, Donnellan, & Robins, 2008),
rejection, frustration and numbness (Gerber & Wheeler, 2009)
or even childhood sexual abuse (Lilienfeld, 2002). If researchers truly accepted meta-analyses as objective summaries of a
research field, meta-analyses would have more impact in shifting debates than they do.
It may be that meta-analyses are not capable of answering
big questions due to issues of methodological flexibility which
introduce exactly the type of subjectivity they were designed to
avoid. Consider the area of video game violence which experienced a war of the metas in which both sides of the debate
have produced meta-analyses supporting and critiquing beliefs
in harmful effects (e.g., Anderson et al., 2010; Sherry, 2007).
Examining the most recent, we can see why meta-analyses do
little to resolve academic debates. Several coauthors of the
Anderson et al. (2010) meta-analysis are strong advocates for
wide searches for unpublished studies (Rothstein & Bushman,
2012). Despite this, one of the coauthors recently acknowledged they made no such effort with their own meta-analysis
(Bushman & Huesmann, in press). Instead, the authors only
included their own unpublished studies and those of close colleagues, introducing selection bias. This demonstrates how
authors can stack a meta-analysis toward a particular outcome.
Part of the problem in using meta-analyses to resolve scholarly debates is misuse of an average effect size wins approach
to meta-analysis which has no basis in science. Imagine, 10 studies test the hypothesis that eating rhubarbs causes depression. All
of the studies are direct replications of equal quality. Five of the
studies find that rhubarbs are associated with depression at the r
= .3 level. The other five studies are exactly null r = .00. A
heated rhubarb debate ensues. Advocates of the rhubarbs are
bad theory conduct a meta-analysis and find that the average
rhubarb effect on depression is a statistically significant r = .15
and declare victory. This is entirely absurd as the meta-analysis,
in effect, washes away a 50% failed replication rate we should be

very curious about. And this issue ignores the widespread publication bias of our field (Fanelli, 2010). This is further exacerbated in meta-analyses of correlational data in which spuriously
high bivariate correlations are preferred over theoretically superior partial correlations or standardized regression coefficients in
the name of statistical purity (Pratt & Cullen, 2000). In this
sense, meta-analyses have their own bias and that is to favor the
maintenance of hypotheses over their falsification. Many metaanalyses place a reverse burden of proof on null studies or failed
replications by creating a scenario in which only a massive number of failed replications would warrant theory rejection. This use
of meta-analysis is inherently unscientific.
It is not my intent to rail entirely against meta-analysis.
Used properly and with care they can be informative, telling us
about methodological and population moderators that can
influence research results. But they seldom resolve academic
debates. Returning to the issue of the meta-analysis by Wood
et al. (2014), I suspect it has accomplished the task of putting
the ball in the court of those who would argue that womens
mating preferences vary across the menstrual cycle. But I suspect the debate is not over yet. Certainly, to imply that this is a
critical challenge for all of evolutionary psychology is probably unwarranted. Nonetheless, I suspect it will be good theater.

References
Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J.,
Sakamoto, A., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on
aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 151173. doi:10.1037/a0018251
Bushman, B., & Huesmann, L. R. (in press). Twenty-five years of
research on violence in digital games and aggression: A reply to
Elson & Ferguson (2013). European Psychologist.
Fanelli, D. (2010). Positive results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. PLoS ONE, 5(4), e10068.
Gerber, J., & Wheeler, L. (2009). On being rejected: A meta-analysis of
experimental research on rejection. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(5), 468488. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01158.x
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2002). When worlds collide: Social science, politics, and
the Rind et al. (1998) child sexual abuse meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 57(3), 176188. doi:10.1037/0003066X.57.3.176
Pratt, T., & Cullen, C. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschis
general theory of crime: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 38, 931964.
Quintana, S., & Minami, T. (2006). Guidelines for meta-analyses of counseling
psychology research. The Counseling Psychologist, 34(6), 839877.
Rothstein, H. R., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Publication bias in psychological science: Comment on Ferguson and Brannick (2012). Psychological
Methods, 17(1), 129136. doi:10.1037/a0027128
Sherry, J. (2007). Violent video games and aggression: Why cant we
find links? In R. Preiss, B. Gayle, N. Burrell, M. Allen, & J. Bryant
(Eds.), Mass media effects research: Advances through meta-analysis
(pp. 231248). Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.
Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M., & Robins, R. W. (2008). Do todays
young people really think they are so extraordinary? An examination of
secular trends in narcissism and self-enhancement. Psychological Science, 19(2), 181188. doi:10.1111/j.14679280.2008.02065.x
Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly
high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social
cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(3), 274290.
doi:10.1111/j.17456924.2009.01125.x
Wood, W., Kressel, L., Joshi, P., & Louie, B. (2014). Meta-analysis of menstrual
cycle effects on womens mate preferences. Emotion Review, 6, 229249.

Downloaded from emr.sagepub.com at University College London on September 24, 2016

Potrebbero piacerti anche