Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176


www.elsevier.com/locate/jretconser

The effectiveness of verbal prompts on sales


Claus Ebstera,, Udo Wagnera, Sabine Valisb
a
Department of Marketing, University of Vienna, Bruenner Strasse 72, A-1210 Vienna, Austria
b
Erste Bank der oesterreichischen, Sparkassen AG, Austria

Abstract

The effectiveness of verbal prompts and prompting conditions on sales as well as the impact of the product ordered on the
effectiveness of suggestive selling are investigated in this study. In a field experiment in two fast food restaurants, customers are
exposed to suggestive selling, receiving differently worded prompts depending on the assigned experimental condition. Results show
that in all treatment conditions, prompts lead to increased sales over the control group. Whereas no difference in sales is observed
among the treatment groups, prompts in general are shown to be more effective in triggering the ordering of side dishes when used in
conjunction with atypical main dishes recently added to the menu and when there is a fit between the main and the side dish.
r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Prompting; Suggestive selling; Sales promotion

1. Introduction the employees delivering scripted purchase suggestions


might occasionally resent having to ‘‘push’’ products on
Customer: A hamburger and a medium coke, please. their customers, as the suggestions made could come
Sales clerk: Do you want fries with that? across as insincere or inappropriate (Leidner, 1993),
While customers might not always appreciate it when anecdotal evidence suggests that suggestive selling can
a sales clerk responds to an order by suggesting a related be a very effective method in increasing sales. In 1987,
item, such sales interactions seem to have become Martinko and, in 1989 Martinko, White, and Hassell
commonplace in many fast food restaurants. demonstrated that suggestive selling could effectively
In saturated markets, in addition to acquiring new increase sales. However, since then, little has been
customers, marketers have been concentrating on published on the effectiveness of suggestive selling in
increasing sales among their existing customers, en- academic consumer behavior literature.
couraging them to buy more, to buy more often or to As a result, this paper aims to examine the effective-
buy additional items. A sales promotion strategy ness of suggestive selling in fast food restaurants. Not
frequently mentioned in practitioners’ literature is only is the general effectiveness of suggestive selling
suggestive selling. This practice is defined as making a investigated, but its use in conjunction with a variety of
suggestion to potential buyers that they might have products as well as the effectiveness of different
additional needs related to what has already been suggestion wordings is also examined. Furthermore,
purchased (Rosenberg, 1995). the potential influence of descriptive variables (e.g.
Suggestive selling has been used in financial services gender and age of the customer, time of visit to the fast
(Andrews, 1999), retailing (D’Aversa-Williams, 2001) food restaurant, and location of the restaurant) is
and catalog marketing (Lauderbaugh, 2002). Although considered. Finally, given that the origin of this study
is Austria and most practitioners’ reports on the
Corresponding author. effectiveness of this promotional technique originate
E-mail address: claus.ebster@univie.ac.at (C. Ebster). from the US, it is also intended to assess whether

0969-6989/$ - see front matter r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jretconser.2005.08.003
ARTICLE IN PRESS
170 C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176

suggestive selling can effectively be implemented in a Furthermore, verbal prompts were shown to increase
cultural context outside the USA. sales of food and drinks in restaurants (Martinko, 1987;
Martinko et al., 1989). Therefore, prompts might also be
effective when cross-selling side dishes in a fast food
2. Theoretical background to suggestive selling restaurant. This leads us to propose Hypothesis 1:

A conceptual background to suggestive selling can be H1. The use of verbal prompts increases sales of side
found in operant psychology, a theoretical approach dishes.
which has received renewed interest from consumer
researchers in recent years (Foxall, 2002). Operant The specific wording of verbal prompts was found to
psychology focuses on directly observable behavior be of particular importance in their effectiveness
and was first introduced to a broader group of market- (Feeney et al., 1982). From informal discussions with
ing researchers by Nord and Peter (1980) under the managers and staff of fast food restaurants, we
name of ‘‘behavior modification perspective’’. A basic concluded that the effectiveness of prompting can be
assumption of operant conditioning is that people learn increased if prompts are delivered in a ‘‘forced choice’’
by producing changes in their environment. question, i.e. customers are prompted to order either
A central tenant of operant psychology is the one product or another. Support for the proposition
antecedent–behavior–consequence (ABC) model. Ante- that forced choice questions might lead to greater
cedent stimuli (A) trigger behavior (B), which is compliance with the request also comes from the
followed by positive or negative consequences (C). ‘‘mindlessness principle’’ proposed by Langer et al.
These in turn influence the probability that this behavior (1978). They found that for trivial, non-demanding
will recur in the future (Skinner, 1969). According to the requests, subjects avoid investing cognitive efforts and
‘‘law of effects’’ (Thorndike, 1911), if a behavior results rely strongly on general knowledge structures. It is
in positive consequences, it will be repeated more assumed that people are put into a state of mindlessness
frequently in the future. If followed by negative primarily in routine conversations that have frequently
consequences, the likelihood of recurrence decreases. occurred in the past (Langer & Moldoveanu, 2000).
For example, in a fast food restaurant, antecedent Therefore, by wording prompts in a forced choice
stimuli—such as product displays, flyers, and recom- question format, consumers might feel obliged to choose
mendations from accompanying friends—might trigger one of two alternatives, resulting in more orders of the
customers to order a side dish. The customer’s satisfac- prompted product, as stated in Hypothesis 2:
tion or dissatisfaction with the taste as well as a number
of other consumption related factors determine the H2. The use of forced-choice verbal prompts is more
likelihood that the individual will reorder this side dish effective in increasing the sales of side dishes than the
in the future. use of regular verbal prompts.
Prompts are a special type of antecedent stimuli, i.e.
supplemental stimuli that help to initiate a desired It seems probable that not only the wording of the
behavior (Touchette and Howard, 1984). The desired prompts, but also the product that the customers are
response can be prompted in different ways. In a sales prompted to consider influence the effectiveness of
situation, gestural, physical, environmental and verbal prompting to a greater extent. We hypothesized that
prompts can be used. For example, to stimulate sales in prompting would be more successful if the product
a fast food restaurant, the salespersons could point to a prompted (i.e. the side dish) has a good fit with the main
dessert (gestural prompt) or hold a dessert tray in the dish ordered. For example, in Austria, schnitzels
customer’s direction (physical prompt). Illuminated (breaded pork/veal cutlets) are a national dish usually
boards over the sales counter and displays offering served with potato salad or fries. However, in Austria,
‘‘specials’’ are examples of environmental prompts unlike in the US, sandwiches are considered a light
frequently used in fast food restaurants. When the staff snack and are not usually served with a side dish.
in fast food restaurants offers customers items related to Accordingly, Hypothesis 3 states:
their original order, such as fries to go with a burger
(cross-selling) or larger sizes (upselling), these sugges- H3. Prompting side dishes is more effective when
tions can be considered verbal prompts. consumers see a fit between the main dish and the
Verbal prompts were successfully used in social side dish.
marketing to increase blood donations (Ferrari et al.,
1985) and to encourage drivers to wear seatbelts Prompting is assumed to be a behavioral method used
(Engerman et al., 1997). In a services marketing context, to initiate a behavior not previously shown (Kazdin,
verbal prompts were found to improve appointment 1994). Therefore, it might be more effective to prompt
keeping in a pediatric clinic (Friman et al., 1985). customers to order a side dish with their main dish, if the
ARTICLE IN PRESS
C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176 171

main dish is a new addition to the menu rather than a Austrian fast food restaurant chain, specializing in a
dish traditionally ordered in a specific (type of) variety of schnitzels, but also serving sandwiches and
restaurant. The idea that prompting might be more chicken nuggets. Two weeks prior to the experiment,
effective when used in conjunction with dishes that vegetarian and fish dishes, two product categories not
customers are unlikely to have received previous offered by most Austrian fast food restaurants, were
prompting for, is supported by correspondent-inference added to the menu. Data were collected over a period of
theory (Jones and McGillis, 1976). This theory, which 2 weeks in two branches of the chain. One was located in
has been used by consumer researchers to explain some the food court of a shopping mall in a large city with a
of the effects of price promotions (Lichtenstein et al., population of approximately 1,750,000, the other on the
1991), states that consumers are more likely to process main street of a small town with approximately 10,000
and elaborate information when it is unexpected or inhabitants. Prior to the experiment, suggestive selling
unusual. When an event is unexpected, customers can no had not been used by the restaurant chain.
longer assimilate it into their existing knowledge
structures, which may lead them to consciously examine 3.2. Design and procedure
their previous assumptions (Försterling, 2001). This
might happen, for example, when a customer orders a Before starting the experiment, the sales clerks of the
newly added item, such as a vegetarian dish for the first two participating restaurants were trained in delivering
time, and is consequently prompted for the first time to the prompts to the customers. They were informed of
order a side dish with this meal. According to the general nature of the experiment, but were blind
correspondent-inference theory, behavioral observations to the specific hypotheses being tested.
are unexpected when the information is inconsistent An after-only with control group design was chosen.
with previous information or when it is distinct from Participants were assigned to one of four experimental
other current information (Jones and McGillis, 1976). situations based on the sequence of their arrival at the
Consequently, restaurant patrons might respond more counter. Successive groups of ten customers ordering one
positively to suggestive selling when it is unlikely that of the main dishes offered (schnitzel, fish, vegetarian
they have been previously prompted to order a side dish dishes, chicken nuggets, sandwiches) were assigned to one
with a specific main dish, or when they order a main dish experimental condition. After having served the tenth
not usually offered in a specific restaurant category. customer, the next experimental condition came into effect.
Accordingly, Hypothesis 4 states: In experimental condition 1 (EG 1), customers were
prompted to order potato salad with their main course.
H4. Prompting side dishes is more effective in increasing The wording of the suggestion was: ‘‘Would you like
sales of side dishes when the main dishes are less familiar potato salad with that?’’ In experimental condition 2
to customers in a restaurant. (EG 2), they were prompted to order fries. Customers
were asked: ‘‘Would you like fries with that?’’ In
Further support for this hypothesis comes from the condition 3 (EG 3), a forced-choice question format
concept of perceived risk (Bauer, 1960), a customer’s was used to prompt the ordering of a side dish: ‘‘Would
subjective anticipation of loss from a product that does you like potato salad with that or would you prefer
not provide all expected benefits (Roselius, 1971; fries?’’ In condition 4, the control group (CG), no
Sweeney et al., 1999). Ordering an unfamiliar main dish prompts were used at all.
or a main dish one has not tried in a restaurant might be In order to determine the effectiveness of prompting
somewhat risky for a restaurant patron. Ordering a side by mere comparison of the experimental and control
dish known to the customer together with the unfamiliar groups, suggestions to order a side dish were only used
main dish might lower perceived risk as the customer is when patrons did not request it themselves, i.e. not all
at least familiar with the taste and quality of the side customers within the experimental groups were exposed
dish, thereby increasing the likelihood of purchase upon to prompting.
exposure to a verbal prompt. Data were collected at lunch time (11:30 a.m.–2:00
p.m.) and at dinner time (6:00 p.m.–10:00 p.m.). In both
restaurants, there was only one cash desk, where
3. Method customers ordered and paid for their food. An
unobtrusive experimenter stood to one side, using
3.1. Research setting differently colored cards to signal which experimental
treatment the restaurant employee should adopt. The
In order to test the above hypotheses, an empirical cards were attached to a rolodex-like device (visible only
research project was undertaken. We decided to use a to the employee) and flipped to indicate a change in
causal design, and observation as the data collection the experimental condition after ten customers. The
method. A field experiment was conducted in an experimenter recorded the main and side dishes ordered,
ARTICLE IN PRESS
172 C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176

as well as the time of the sales interaction (lunch or not appear to influence their effectiveness. Therefore,
dinner time) on a standardized observational form. The customers belonging to all three experimental groups
customer’s gender was also recorded and judgment was were pooled into a single category called ‘experimental
used to classify customers as children (below the age of group’ (EG) for subsequent analyses.
16) or adults. The customers were only observed but not
interviewed in order to ensure an unobtrusive purchase 4.3. Further descriptive analysis of the data
situation and to exclude socially desirable response
behavior. In the previous section, it has already been argued
that, besides prompting, there are a number of other
potential influences on the probability of ordering a side
4. Results dish. Preliminary examination of the situation at hand
established ‘type of main dish ordered’ and ‘age of
4.1. Effectiveness of verbal prompts—H1 customer’ to be of relevance, the other variables being of
minor (‘location of the restaurant’) or no importance
The four groups were balanced with respect to (‘gender’, ‘time’). Looking at Fig. 1, four observations
location, time and size (each group contained 540 are apparent.
observations). A total of 2160 sales transactions were
recorded in the course of the experiment, of which 1589 (i) There is a consistent effect of the treatment
customers (74%) ordered side dishes with their main variable. The percentages of side dishes ordered in
courses. The greatest percentage of customers ordering the experimental group exceed the corresponding
side dishes (79%) was in experimental group 1 (potato numbers for the control group in all cases.
salad prompt), followed by 78% in experimental group (ii) The type of main dish chosen matters. The five
3 (potato salad and fries prompt) and 77% in different options vary with respect to the frequency
experimental group 2 (fries prompt) (see Table 1). of side dishes ordered. There are two extreme cases.
To investigate Hypothesis 1, the data from column 2 On the one hand, it is very popular to eat schnitzel
in Table 1 were used (i.e. experimental groups were with a side dish; while quite the opposite situation is
pooled into a single category EG). The w2-test rejected evident for sandwiches, which seem to be preferred
independence between the experimental variable and the without accompaniment. Focusing on the results
number of ‘‘side dishes ordered’’ (^w2 ¼ 59:1, 1 degree-of- for the control group only, approximately the same
freedom, which corresponds to a p-level less than 0.01). number of patrons order side dishes with fish (69%)
This result is in line with intuitive interpretation of the as with chicken nuggets (65%).
data and clearly supports H1: prompted customers (iii) Besides these disparities in magnitude, there is an
order side dishes more often, i.e. sales increased by a impact on the effectiveness of prompting depending
magnitude of about 17 percentage points. on the main dish chosen. This can be verified by
looking at the differences between the percentages
4.2. Effectiveness of different prompting conditions—H2 of EG and CG (these numbers are shown in Fig. 1).
With the vegetarian dish, for example, about a
Hypothesis 2 was analyzed by concentrating on the 31-percentage point difference is found, whereas
results of the three different experimental conditions with chicken nuggets there is a 6-percentage point
(data from column 3, rows 3 to 5 in Table 1). The w2-test difference. Consequently, if analyzing the odds of
did not reject independence between the experimental ordering a side dish (versus not ordering a side dish)
variable and the number of ‘‘side dishes ordered’’ and comparing the odds for the two categories of
(^w2 ¼ 0:9, 2 degrees of freedom, which corresponds to the treatment variable (e.g. for the vegetarian dish),
a p-level greater than 0.1). This result is consistent with a score of 3.6, i.e. (66/(10066))/(35/(10035))
visual inspection of the data. Thus, the specific format of results. In both cases prompting is found to be less
a verbal prompt, open or forced-choice question does effective for chicken nuggets and sandwiches.

Table 1

Number/percentages of respondents ordering side dishes Group size

CG: no prompt 329/61% 540


EG: prompt 1260/78% 427/79% EG 1: potato salad prompt 540
414/77% EG 2: fries prompt 540
419/78% EG 3: potato salad and fries prompt 540
1589/74% 2160
ARTICLE IN PRESS
C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176 173

Fig. 1. Differences in percentages between experimental group and control group.

(iv) The frequencies of side dishes ordered vary by age. lEX j effect of the experimental stimulus
Adults consume side dishes more often than results are given in a relative sense, i.e. with
children do. respect to the reference category control group
(lEX CG ¼ 0); je{EG, CG};
4.4. Logistic regression analysis lA k effect of age
results are given in a relative sense, i.e.
In Sections 4.1 and 4.2, only two variables at a time with respect to the reference category child
were studied by means of cross-tabulations. Section 4.3 (lAchild ¼ 0); ke {adult, child};
has shown that other influences (e.g. age, main dish lMDl effect of the main dish chosen
chosen) should be considered as well, and therefore, results are given in a relative sense, i.e. with
given the categorical character of the data, multiway respect to the reference category sandwiches
frequency analysis was applied. Since the ‘‘number of (lMDsandwiches ¼ 0); le {schnitzel, fish, vegetarian
side dishes’’ ordered is considered a dependent variable, dish, chicken nuggets, sandwiches};
logistic regression offers a convenient way to analyze the lEX j MDl second-order effect of experimental stimulus
data. Therefore, a log-linear model is postulated to by main dish chosen;
describe how ‘‘prompting’’, ‘‘age’’ and ‘‘main dish ujkl error term.
chosen’’ influence the probability of ordering a side
dish. Because of observation (iii) in Section 4.3, the
second-order effect /experimental stimulusmain Viewed as a special kind of multiway frequency
dishS was additionally included. In mathematical terms, analysis, our data is a 2225 table, with 2160
the model can be written as follows: observations in total, distributed among the 40 cells.
! Furthermore, as there are only two observed cell
Sd^ jkl frequencies with less than 5, the basic requirements for
ln
1  S d^ jkl running a logistic regression are fulfilled. The results are
presented in Table 2.
¼ const þ lEX j þ lAk þ lMDl þ lEX j nMDl þ ujkl
By looking at the parameter estimates, significant
with influences for all independent variables are found. Since
the parameter estimates describe the linear effects of the
S d^jkl estimated probability (percentage) that a side predictors on the log of the odds ratio of the outcome
dish is ordered for cell (j, k, l); category, interpretation is more intuitive in terms of the
ARTICLE IN PRESS
174 C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176

Table 2

Logistic regression: probability of ordering a side disha

Independent variables Parameter estimates Odds ratio

Constant 1.24
Experimental groupb 0.52 1.68
Adultc 0.64 1.90
Schnitzeld 2.63 13.80
Fishd 1.95 7.04
Vegetarian dishd 0.49 1.64
Chicken nuggetsd 2.11 8.21
Schnitzel  experimental group 1.12 3.06
Fish  experimental group 0.98 2.68
Vegetarian dish  experimental group 0.77 2.17
Chicken nuggets  experimental group 0.33 0.72
Goodness-of-fit
LR-test against ‘constant-only model’ w^ 2 ¼ 856:79 df ¼ 10
LR-test against ‘full model’ 2 df ¼ 9
G^ ¼ 5:01
‘Perfect model’ hit rate ¼ 84.2% N ¼ 2160
(Cmax ¼ 73.6%e) (Cpro ¼ 61.1%f)
Pseudo-R2 (McFadden, 1974) r2 ¼ 0:34
 Significant at the 10% level.
Significant at the 5% level.
Significant at the 1% level.
a
Reference category: no side dish ordered.
b
Reference category: control group.
c
Reference category: child.
d
Reference category: sandwiches.
e
Benchmark maximum chance criterion.
f
Benchmark proportional chance criterion.

coefficients’ odds ratio (i.e. expðl^  Þ). For example, a Goodness-of-fit is also satisfactory. Statistical testing
customer choosing fish as a main dish is seven times rejects the assumption that a constant-only model fits as
more likely to order a side dish than a person having well as the proposed one, but does not reject the full (i.e.
sandwiches. It can be noted that ‘‘main dish chosen’’ is saturated) model. Classification based upon the model
the most influential regressor. Moreover, adults con- fails only in about 16% of the cases (due to limited data,
sume side dishes almost twice as often as children. The it was not possible to divide the data into an estimation
reasons for this are numerous. For example, children do and a hold-out sample to provide a more stringent
not eat as much as adults and, therefore, are satiated calculation of the hit rate). A detailed analysis of the
with less food and do not need side dishes to satisfy their standardized residuals shows that all of them are smaller
appetite. In addition, children probably have more than 1.1 (in absolute value), and therefore there are no
restrictions on money at their disposal and, therefore, outliers in the data. Finally, pseudo R2 (McFadden,
cannot afford to buy a side dish (even if they wanted to). 1974) is also acceptable, enabling us to conclude that
When accounting for other effects, prompting is logistic regression provides results which are face valid
nevertheless found to have a significant influence, i.e. as well as statistically sound. In accordance with
the odds of ordering a side dish are 1.68:1 in this case preliminary results, the proposed model outperforms
(whereas a rough estimate of the odds based on the data alternative formulations (with other independent vari-
in Table 1 would be 2.27:1, i.e. (0.78/0.22)/(0.61/.039)). ables such as ‘‘gender’’, ‘‘location of restaurants’’ or
This result is in line with the approximation of the odds ‘‘time’’).
of ordering a side dish when belonging to the experi-
mental group and ordering the vegetarian dish. In 4.5. Investigation of H3 and H4
comment (iii) of Section 4.3 a preliminary estimate of 3.6
was derived which has to be compared with the estimate With respect to Hypotheses 3 and 4, it is interesting to
of 2.17 in Table 2. Simultaneous consideration of several observe the estimates of the interaction variable, e.g.
effects reduces the magnitude of individual effects when prompting a patron who orders a schnitzel is three times
looked upon in isolation. more effective than if he were to ask for sandwiches (or
ARTICLE IN PRESS
C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176 175

chicken nuggets). Bearing in mind that schnitzels are extended to related industries, and which factors, apart
usually served with a potato-based side dish in Austria, from the typicality of products, are influential, are
the significance of this parameter supports Hypothesis 3. warranted. An important factor in the effectiveness of
The second-order effect is also significant for fish and suggestive selling might be whether a functional or
the vegetarian dishes, although the odds ratios are hedonic product is being prompted. This distinction has
slightly lower. In view of the rather similar percentages been shown to influence price sensitivity (Wakefield and
of side dishes ordered within the control group for fish Inman, 2003) and could also extend to sales promotion
and chicken nuggets (c.f. remark (ii) in Section 4.3 effectiveness. Furthermore, further research could ex-
above), the increased effectiveness of prompting with amine the role of social influence on the effectiveness of
fish dishes must be due to some external influence not prompts. While only single orders (i.e. customers
considered until now. We attribute this to the fact that ordering one meal) were included in the present study,
fish dishes have only been added to the menu recently, the effectiveness of prompts when multiple orders are
but concede that causality (in the strictest sense) has not, placed and when customers order as part of a group,
as yet, been established. Similarly, when comparing the should be examined in future studies. Finally, it might
vegetarian dish and sandwiches, the same pattern is be worth investigating the long-term effects of verbal
identified, however, to a lesser extent. Subsequently, prompting, and its impact on customer satisfaction and
results are in line with Hypothesis 4. store loyalty.

5.2. Managerial implications


5. Discussion
From a managerial standpoint, it should be noted
This study examined the effectiveness of suggestive
that verbal prompts could be a promising and cost-
selling using verbal prompts. It has been shown that
efficient sales promotion tool. Nevertheless, it should
verbal prompts can lead to significant increases (16–18
not be forgotten that prompting could lead to resistance
percentage points) in sales of side dishes in fast food
from both employees and customers. Therefore, special
restaurants. This finding is consistent with the results of
care should be taken to overcome this reactance. To
studies conducted in social marketing. Moreover, the
mitigate employees’ reactance to prompting, clear rules
hypothesis based on correspondent-inference theory,
as to when sales should be prompted should be given,
whereby prompts are more effective in increasing sales
while at the same time allowing staff to use discretion
of side dishes when used in conjunction with behavior
when not to prompt (e.g. in the case of regular
not previously performed, i.e. ordering side dishes with
customers, fellow employees or children). In an empiri-
main dishes atypical for the restaurant, is supported by
cal study, Johnson and Masotti (1990) found that
the results. However, the wording of the prompts did
behavioral interventions such as feedback, goal setting
not appear to influence their effectiveness, and in
and positive reinforcement by management can increase
particular the forced-choice prompt was not found
appropriate suggested selling by employees. To mini-
to be more effective than regular verbal prompts. A
mize customers’ annoyance when prompted for addi-
possible explanation for this might be that both side
tional purchases, prompting should only be encouraged
dishes prompted were potato-based food items. While
with items complementing the purchase and prompts
the results suggest that both fries and potato salad are
should be delivered in a courteous, non-demanding way.
highly popular with the restaurants’ patrons, it seems
In conclusion, prompts can only achieve their full
unlikely that the particular phrase used to suggest a side
potential, if they are integrated in overall human
dish might have led customers who dislike potato-based
resource and sales training strategy. Nevertheless, they
dishes to overcome their dislike.
can be a very valuable marketing tool. In fact, as a result
of the obvious effectiveness of verbal prompting and its
5.1. Limitations and areas for future research
efficiency compared to alternative sales promotion
instruments, such as price promotions, the fast food
While this field experiment has demonstrated that
company involved in this experiment has been using
verbal prompts are effective in fast food restaurants,
suggestive selling ever since.
caution should be exercised in generalizing these
findings to other settings. Furthermore, correspondent-
inference theory can only partially explain the effective-
ness of prompts. In the course of this study, it was not Acknowledgements
possible to conclude how effective prompts should be
worded, a relevant issue for consumer researchers and The authors wish to thank two anonymous reviewers
practitioners alike. Further studies to investigate for their valuable comments on an earlier version of this
whether the effectiveness of suggestive selling can be article.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
176 C. Ebster et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 13 (2006) 169–176

References Langer, E.J., Blank, A., Chanowitz, B., 1978. The mindlessness of
ostensibly thoughtful action: The role of ‘‘placebic’’ information in
Andrews, K.J., 1999. Extending upselling and cross-selling efforts. interpersonal interaction. Journal of Personality and Social
Target Marketing 22, 36–40. Psychology 36, 635–642.
Bauer, R.A., 1960. Consumer behavior as risk taking. In: Hancock, Lauderbaugh, J.J., 2002. Embracing the upsell: Increase sales by
R.S. (Ed.), Dynamic Marketing for a Changing World: Proceed- shifting your phone reps’ mindset from service to sales. Catalog
ings of the 43rd Conference of the American Marketing Associa- Age 19 (6), 49–50.
tion, pp. 389–398. Leidner, R., 1993. Fast Food, Fast Talk: Service Work and the
D’Aversa-Williams, T., 2001. Merchandising+suggestive selling ¼ Routinization of Everyday Life. University of California Press,
accessory sales. Sporting Goods Business 34, 44. Berkeley, CA.
Engerman, J.A., Austin, J., Bailey, J.S., 1997. Prompting patron safety Lichtenstein, D.R., Burton, S., Karson, E.J., 1991. The effect of
belt use at a supermarket. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 30, semantic cues on consumer perception of reference price ads.
577–579. Journal of Consumer Research 18, 380–391.
Feeney, E.J., Staelin, J.R., O’Brien, R.M., Dickinson, A.M., 1982. Martinko, M.J., 1987. An O. B. Mod. analysis of consumer
Increasing sales performance among airline reservation personnel. behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management 8,
In: O’Brien, R.M., Dickinson, A.M., Rosow, M. (Eds.), Industrial 19–43.
Behavior Modification: A Management Handbook. Pergamon Martinko, M.J., White, J.D., Hassell, B., 1989. An operant analysis of
Press, New York, pp. 141–158. prompting in a sales environment. Journal of Organizational
Ferrari, J.R., Barone, R.C., Jason, L.A., Rose, T., 1985. The use of Behavior Management 10, 93–107.
incentives to increase blood donations. Journal of Social Psychol- McFadden, D., 1974. Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice
ogy 125, 791–793. behavior. In: Zarembka, P. (Ed.), Frontiers in Econometrics.
Försterling, F., 2001. Attribution: An Introduction to Theories, Academic Press, New York, pp. 105–142.
Research, and Applications. Taylor & Francis, Philadelphia, PA. Nord, W.R., Peter, J.P., 1980. A behavior modification perspective on
Foxall, G.R. (Ed.), 2002. Consumer Behaviour Analysis: Critical marketing. Journal of Marketing 44, 36–47.
Perspectives on Business and Management. Routledge, London. Roselius, T., 1971. Consumer ranking of risk reduction methods.
Friman, P.C., Finney, J.W., Rapoff, M.A., Christophersen, E.R., Journal of Marketing 35, 56–61.
1985. Improving pediatric appointment keeping with reminders Rosenberg, J., 1995. Dictionary of Retailing and Merchandising.
and reduced response requirement. Journal of Applied Behavior Wiley, New York.
Analysis 18, 315–321. Skinner, B.F., 1969. Contingencies of Reinforcement: a Theoretical
Johnson, C.M., Masotti, R.M., 1990. Suggestive selling by waitstaff in Analysis. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
a family style restaurant. Journal of Organizational Behavior Sweeney, J.C., Soutar, G.N., Johnson, LW., 1999. The role of
Mangement 11, 35–54. perceived risk in the quality-value relationship: a study in a retail
Jones, E.E., McGillis, D., 1976. Correspondent inference and the environment. Journal of Retailing 75, 77–105.
attribution cube: a comparative reappraisal. In: Harvey, J.H., Thorndike, E.L., 1911. Animal Intelligence. Macmillan, New York.
Ickes, W.J., Kidd, R.F. (Eds.), New Directions in Attribution Touchette, P.E., Howard, J.S., 1984. The effects of verbal performance
Research, vol. 1. Erlbaum, New York, pp. 389–419. descriptions on nonverbal operant responding. Journal of the
Kazdin, A.E., 1994. Behavior Modification in Applied Settings, fifth Experimental Analysis of Behavior 54, 273–291.
ed. Brooks/Cole Publishing, Pacific Grove, CA. Wakefield, K.L., Inman, J.J., 2003. Situational price sensitivity: the
Langer, E.J., Moldoveanu, M., 2000. Mindfulness theory and the role of consumption occasion, social context and income. Journal
future. Journal of Social Issues 56, 129–139. of Retailing 79, 199–212.

Potrebbero piacerti anche