Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 34 45

www.elsevier.com/locate/gaitpost

Review

Symmetry and limb dominance in able-bodied gait: a review


Heydar Sadeghi a,b,c,d,*, Paul Allard a,c, Francois Prince a,b,c, Hubert Labelle a,b,e
b

a
Research Center, Sainte-Justine Hospital, 3175 Cote-Ste-Catherine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3T 1C5
Marie-Enfant Hospital, Rehabilitation Center of Sainte-Justine Hospital, 5200 Belanger East, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H1T 1C9
c
Department of Kinesiology, Uni6ersity of Montreal, 2100 Edouard-Montpetit Bl6d, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3C 3J7
d
Department of Kinesiology, Tarbiat Moallem Uni6ersity, Ministry of Sciences, Research and Technology, Tehran, I.R. Iran
e
Department of Orthopedic Surgery, Sainte-Justine Hospital, 3175 Cote-Ste-Catherine, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3T 1C5

Received 10 November 1999; received in revised form 7 May 2000; accepted 26 May 2000

Abstract
As one of the most universal of all human activities, gait in the able-bodied has received considerable attention, but many
aspects still need to be clarified. Symmetry or asymmetry in the actions of the lower extremities during walking and the possible
effect of laterality on gait are two prevalent and controversial issues. The purpose of this study was to review the work done over
the last few decades in demonstrating: (a) whether or not the lower limbs behave symmetrically during able-bodied gait; and (b)
how limb dominance affects the symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior of the lower extremities. The literature reviewed shows that
gait symmetry has often been assumed, to simplify data collection and analysis. In contrast, asymmetrical behavior of the lower
limbs during able-bodied ambulation was addressed in numerous investigations and was found to reflect natural functional
differences between the lower extremities. These functional differences were probably related to the contribution of each limb in
carrying out the tasks of propulsion and control during able-bodied walking. In current debates on gait symmetry in able-bodied
subjects, laterality has been cited as an explanation for the existence of functional differences between the lower extremities,
although a number of studies do not support the hypothesis of a relationship between gait symmetry and laterality. Further
investigation is needed to demonstrate functional gait asymmetry and its relationship to laterality, taking into consideration the
biomechanical aspects of gait. 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Gait asymmetry; Limb dominance; Symmetry; Laterality indices

1. Introduction
Gait is a basic requirement for daily activity [1] and
is known to be one of the most universal and complex
of all human activities. It is a complex motor skill
governed by several inter-linked pathways from the
cortex to the muscles [2]. The high interaction between
the central nervous system and various muscles allows
the individual to keep the body upright, while at the
same time moving around in an orderly, stable manner.
The interest in asymmetrical gait arises from questions
regarding limb coordination in producing the smooth
* Corresponding author. Tel.: + 1-514-3454931 ext. 6195; fax:
+1-514-3454801.
E-mail address: sadeghih@ere.umontreal.ca (H. Sadeghi).

rhythmical motion of the able-bodied. Gait interpretation can be affected by assuming lower limb symmetry,
since asymmetry is frequently considered to indicate
gait pathology. Therefore, accepting that gait is asymmetrical in the able-bodied population is an important
issue for clinicians and may also be an important
consideration in physical activities, gait evaluation, clinical decisions for patients requiring rehabilitation for
one or both lower extremities, and artificial limb design.
Our intent in this review paper was to summarize the
work done over the last few decades regarding the
assumption of lower limb symmetry during able-bodied
gait. Limb laterality and its possible effect on the
symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior of the lower
extremities during gait is also considered. This paper
consists of three unequal parts. After defining gait

0966-6362/00/$ - see front matter 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 9 6 6 - 6 3 6 2 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 0 7 0 - 9

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

symmetry and asymmetry, the first part deals with the


biomechanical aspects of symmetry or asymmetry of
the lower limbs during gait. Laterality and its effect on
gait is reviewed in the second part, while the indices
used and proposed to quantify gait symmetry and
laterality are described in the third part.

2. Symmetry and asymmetry

2.1. Gait symmetry definition


Gait symmetry has been defined as a perfect agreement between the actions of the lower limbs [3,4] while
Gundersen et al. [5], Griffin et al. [6], Hesse et al. [7]
and Gabbard [8] suggested using the term gait symmetry when no statistical differences are noted on
parameters measured bilaterally. Anatomical or physiological criteria have also been used to describe symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior in able-bodied gait. It
seems that the common idea in the different definitions
is that the term gait symmetry can be applied when
both limbs behave identically.

2.2. Support for gait symmetry


Historically, gait symmetry was assumed in the literature for the sake of simplicity in data collection and
analysis. In fact, simplification might be one of the
main reasons that many gait studies relied on unilateral
data collection [922] or pooled right and left limb data
[16,2326]. Gait symmetry was not tested in any of
these investigations, since gait symmetry was assumed.
In contrast, some gait experiments where both lower
extremities were included in the observations looked for
gait symmetry and confirmed its presence. For example,
symmetry was reported for foot-floor contact and photographic recording at the level of hips and knees
during three successive walking cycles [27]. The similarity between the lower extremities was also addressed
during gait for the vertical and horizontal reaction
forces of 214 normal subjects [28]. This author suggested that a normal gait pattern could be characterized
by marked population variability, step-to-step consistency and symmetry of the ground reaction forces from
both feet. Using bilateral and three-dimensional electrogoniometer data in 12 able-bodied subjects, Hannah
et al. [23] demonstrated joint motion symmetry in all
three planes of the hip and in the sagittal plane of the
knee during natural walking, using time and frequency
domain analysis. Hamill et al. [29] found no significant
differences between the limbs in 11 vertical, five anterior posterior and four medio-lateral characteristics of
the ground reaction forces during walking and running.
These results were later confirmed by Menard et al. [30]
who did not find significant asymmetry in any averaged

35

ground reaction force patterns at natural walking speed


in nine able-bodied subjects. Perfect symmetry was
reported in energy efficiency between both limbs [31].
Wall et al. [32] and Hesse et al. [7] also claimed that
normal gait is symmetrical. Symmetrical electromyographic (EMG) activities were observed for homologous muscles during level walking without
objective documentation. Although in a few studies
EMG data were collected from the dominant limb
[17,18,25,33], in many others data were collected only
from the right [9,11,33] or left [34] lower limb. In some
cases, the side was not even specified [3547]. Carlsoo
et al. [46] noted that the muscle actions are virtually the
same for the right and left legs in able-bodied gait.
Recording raw EMG data from hemiplegics and normal controls, Marks and Hirschberg [48] stated that the
EMG patterns of activity in both legs in the former
were identical to normal subjects. Arsenault and colleagues [17] pooled the data across subjects, reported an
almost perfect similarity of the profiles for muscle
activities for different subjects, and noted that both
EMG profiles and amplitudes are highly repeatable
within subjects. Although they did specify it in their
previous work [18], these investigators acknowledged
that pooling data smoothes out individual participation
and that these overall averages cannot be perceived as
the real profile of EMG activity [17]. O0 unpuus conclusions [49] corroborate this finding. Pierotti et al. [50]
reported symmetry on average EMG patterns in normal subjects for six knee muscle activities during free,
slow and fast gait.
In short, although it seems that gait symmetry has
been assumed for simplicity of data collection and
analysis, gait symmetry was actually reported in only a
few studies using quantitative biomechanical data,
where both lower limbs were evaluated. Also, there are
not enough studies where a substantial number of
subjects participated. Gait parameters which provide
information about the effect but not the cause of the
movement might also affect interpretation of lower
limb behavior. Unclear definitions of symmetry, using a
single gait parameter or applying simple statistical
methods for comparison, are additional limitations of
these studies.

2.3. Support for gait asymmetry


In pathological gait, marked differences have been
noted between the affected and unaffected limbs. The
most prominent asymmetries found in amputee gait
were related to shortened support times and decreased
ground reaction forces for the prosthetic limb compared to the natural limb [51]. In patients with an
anterior cruciate ligament deficiency, Devita [52] and
his colleagues showed how muscle moments were affected by functional bracing. Asymmetrical properties

36

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

were also reported for 34 gait variables in a group of 31


hemiplegic subjects [6]. In another study, the gait of
hemiparetic patients was characterized by slower velocity and more asymmetry as they swayed more laterally
on the unaffected leg compared to healthy persons [53].
Gait asymmetries in patients with limb length inequality have also been reported [54]. These few examples
illustrate how gait asymmetry can be a direct consequence of abnormality. However, the issue of gait
asymmetry resulting from disability is not considered
here since our purpose was to review the evidence
regarding whether or not the lower limbs in able-bodied
subjects behave symmetrically.
In able-bodied gait, differences between the right and
left limbs have been reported frequently. Singh [55] and
DuChatinier and Rozendal [56] claimed that the lower
limbs are not used equally during walking. Asymmetrical behavior of the lower extremities was observed in
spatio-temporal and kinematic parameters such as velocity profiles [57,58], step [59] and stride [60,61] length,
foot placement angle [60], maximum knee flexion [59]
and range of joint motion [62]. Rosenrot et al. [63,64]
stated that the duration of the initial and terminal
double support periods were not identical, as would be
expected in young healthy subjects. Gundersen et al. [5]
also reported gait asymmetry in temporal and kinematic parameters. Using spatio-temporal parameters,
Wheelwright and his colleagues [65] reported asymmetrical behavior of the lower limbs in 134 normal children
aged 318 years old. Although spatio-temporal
parameters can provide an overall impression of gait,
from another perspective they might not be considered
informative parameters since they are only able to
provide insight into the effect (e.g. stride length) of the
movement and not the cause (e.g. force) [66,67]. In
other words, regardless of whether one accepts or rejects the symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior of the
lower limbs, it would be reasonable to concentrate on
more informative biomechanical parameters which
provide insight into both effect and cause of the movement. The idea of gait asymmetry will be discussed in
the next section using kinetic and EMG data which
consider the cause of lower limb motion during ablebodied gait.
Gait asymmetry was also addressed in able-bodied
subjects where kinetic data were examined. Asymmetry
was found in plantar flexor strength and calf circumference [68] and for forces and moments in both sagittal
and frontal planes [69]. Differences were noted between
the right and left lower limbs of 62 able-bodied subjects
for the peak vertical, anterior posterior and medio-lateral components of ground reaction forces [70]. In
1989, Herzog and his colleagues [4] found that asymmetries were much larger than expected in 34 ground
reaction force data of a control group. Devita et al. [52]
suggested that normal walking can be characterized by

asymmetrical kinetics between left and right limbs


about the lumbar sacral region (L5 S1 joint). In 1992,
Dickey and Winter [71] identified an asymmetrical contribution for the muscular work done at the hip level in
able-bodied subjects. Using ground reaction force data,
Crowe et al. [72,73] claimed that consistent gait asymmetry should be assumed in the oscillation of the body
center of mass. Using time and frequency domain analysis, Giakas and Baltzopoulos [74] investigated the
variability and symmetry of ground reaction force measurements during walking of ten healthy young male
subjects. This study confirmed that human gait is a
symmetrical movement based on harmonic analysis,
while substantial asymmetries characterized time domain variables in the medio-lateral component of
ground reaction forces. Using EMG data, Arsenault et
al. [25] reported asymmetries in EMG amplitude profiles for the soleus and rectus femoris muscles. Subsequently, O0 unpuu and Winter [75] found asymmetrical
activities in seven selected muscles recorded simultaneously by EMG during walking. Gait asymmetry was
also documented by differences in the neuromuscular
drive from muscles involved for a given movement as
measured by EMG [76]. To summarize, based on these
observations, it may be concluded that gait is asymmetrical. However, can we argue that it is acceptable to
conclude that able-bodied gait is asymmetrical just
because of the existence of statistically significant differences between two corresponding parameters (which we
call local asymmetry) calculated from the right and left
limbs? If this is the case, how can we explain the effect
of other factors such as compensation and/or adaptation that directly influence lower limb behavior during
gait? Although this study mainly considers the issue of
gait symmetry in the able-bodied population, giving an
example of amputee gait provides insight into how
other issues such as compensation affect lower limb
behavior locally. In below-knee amputee gait, hip flexors on the amputated side compensate for the lack of
normal ankle function during push-off [77,78]. This
causes gait asymmetry as shown when this parameter is
compared with its corresponding value on the sound
limb. However, this asymmetrical behavior is most
likely related to compensation [78] rather than the
asymmetrical behavior of the lower limbs. To some
extent, compensation might also occur in able-bodied
gait. Based on this assumption it would be reasonable
to find some other way to explain the idea of gait
asymmetry which includes the global behavior of the
lower limbs rather than local parameters. We believe
that the idea of functional gait asymmetry which has
been proposed and discussed in recent gait studies
[79,80] might be a way to explain global asymmetrical
behavior of the lower limbs in relation to local
parameters.

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

2.4. Functional gait asymmetry


The aim of locomotion is to propel the body forward
while supporting the body against gravity [81]. Gait
asymmetry is explained in part by functional task discrepancies describing the relative contribution of control and propulsion during healthy able-bodied walking
performance. Hirasawa [82] and Vanden-Abeele [83]
were among the first to interpret gait asymmetry in
able-bodied subjects based on the support and mobility
associated with each limb. Hirasawa [84] confirmed his
previous finding [82] in stating that one lower limb is
mainly responsible for support and body weight transfer during walking while the contralateral limb contributes more to propulsion. Evaluating the lateral
component of ground reaction forces during walking of
28 healthy subjects, Matsusaka et al. [85] reported that
medio-lateral balance was mostly controlled by the left
limb. For 53 males and 39 females walking at slow, free
and fast speeds, Hirokawa [86] associated propulsion
with the right limb while the left limb was found to be
responsible for support. A few years later, Sadeghi et
al. [79] reported that propulsion was related to the leg
with predominantly muscle power generation whereas
support and control functions were associated with the
limb having predominantly power absorption behavior.
This study suggested that gait asymmetry in able-bodied subjects can be explained in terms of the actions
taken by the lower limbs to propel the body segments
and to control forward progression. Sadeghi et al. [87]
further tested the concept that the power activity of the
leading limb (propulsion) was related to that of the
trailing limb (stabilizing). They also reported that the
interaction between muscle powers in able-bodied gait
could reflect specific propulsion and control strategies
that are related to each limb.
According to the information discussed here, ablebodied gait seems to be naturally asymmetrical and this
asymmetrical behavior could be associated with the
different contributions of the lower limbs in carrying
out propulsion and control tasks. However, the main
reasons for able-bodied gait asymmetry remain unclear
and need to be determined. Do they include laterality,
the environment, skill or some other factor?
3. Limb dominance
The human body cannot just be viewed from a
biomechanical standpoint [88] since movement occurs
as a result of the integration of biomechanical and
neurophysiological factors. Although laterality and its
possible effect on human movement such as gait has
recently become an issue of interest in biomechanics,
laterality has a longer history in related sciences including neurophysiology [14,15,85,89 91] and motor control [85,9294].

37

Table 1 shows that not many studies to date examined the relationship between gait parameters and laterality, and even fewer investigated laterality and
similarities or dissimilarities in the behavior of the
lower limbs. The lack of collaboration between
biomechanists and researchers in other fields such as
neurophysiology and motor control may also account
for the fewer number of studies. In the next section, the
concept of limb dominance (laterality, limb preference)
and its possible effect in relation to symmetrical or
asymmetrical behavior of the lower limbs will be reviewed. Limb dominance is defined first and then laterality and its effect on gait symmetry or asymmetry is
discussed.

3.1. Limb dominance definition


In general, limb dominance is related to the notion
that the two hemispheres of the human brain are
functionally dissimilar [95,96]. Limb preference and laterality are also used to express the preferential use of
one limb in voluntary motor acts [8,52,76,92,9699].
Peters [92] attempted to define lower limb laterality by
emphasizing the different roles of the lower limbs. The
author used the term preferred foot for the foot or leg
used to manipulate an object or to lead out, as in
jumping. The foot that is used in activities is the
preferred foot while the non-preferred foot provides
postural and stabilizing support. However, a definition
in terms of role differentiation is not satisfactory in
some cases unless qualifications are made. One example
is balancing, in which head, arm, torso, and leg collaborate to achieve postural stability. Is the preferred leg
used for postural support or dynamic counter-balancing? Nevertheless, the definition of limb dominance is
sufficient to be specified here. In the literature, mobilization and stabilization are also used to characterize
the dominant and non-dominant lower limbs, respectively [92,100,101]. A reasonable argument could be
offered regarding the notion of a dominant foot. The
mobilizing or manipulating limb is the preferred (dominant) foot, whereas the foot that is used to support the
actions of the preferred foot is the non-preferred limb.
It appears that several studies on bilateral task behavior
[102104] support the general contention that humans
are typically right-footed for actions of mobilization
and left-sided for postural stabilization. Complementing this conclusion is the physiological observation of
contralateral control and general left-hemispheric dominance for motor asymmetries. From another perspective, Prevics neurodevelopmental theory [105] offers
explanations for foot dominance in the unilateral and
bilateral context. According to this theory, the idea that
there is no clearly dominant limb in the bilateral context seems reasonable, that is, one foot provides necessary postural support while the other executes

38

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

voluntary (mobilizing) action as the complementary


role action. However, to accept this proposition, one
must assume that the neurological demands placed on
each action (mobilization and stabilization) are equivalent. With most bilateral tasks, it seems that the greatest neurological demand is with mobilization (e.g.
kicking a ball). Perhaps the most interesting aspect of
Prevics theory is the notion that antigravity extension
(postural support) on the left side of the body emerges
before voluntary motor control (mobilization) on the
contralateral (right) side. This suggests that the dominant foot for either unilateral or bilateral task behaviors is the left one for most individuals. Although the
theory appears reasonable with regards to potential
neurodevelopmental structural changes, experimental
observations of unilateral task behavior in children and
adults have generally been nonsupportive [106 109],
that is, individuals frequently exhibited a preferential
bias for the right side, or there was no statistical
difference between the use of the right and left limbs.
To prevent any confusion because of a wide range of
terms suggested in different studies and to use a relevant, consistent definition, throughout the present
study, limb dominance refers to the leg that is used for
mobility while the non-dominant limb contributes to
support. Laterality is also used as a more general term
to express the existence of limb dominance.

3.2. Limb dominance, motor control, neurophysiology


and gait
Despite a shorter history in studies that consider the
issue of symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior of the
lower limbs, laterality has a longer history in other
scientific fields such as neurophysiology or motor control than in gait analysis. For example, in 1865 Broca
[110] was the first to draw attention to and use the term
laterality. He claimed that we talk with our left hemisphere and said that motor organization and brain
functions of the right and left hemispheres are different.
It has since been established that the left hemisphere is
specialized for precise control of fine motor actions on
both sides, while the right hemisphere is related to
bilateral somatosensory spatial ability, emotional expression, and motor functions [90,93,94,111]. Guyton
[91] claimed that the left hemisphere is dominant in
about 95% of humans while in the remaining 5% both
sides develop simultaneously. Rarely does the right side
alone become highly developed. Waziri [89] and Guyton [91] reported that Wernikes area of the cortex will
eventually become larger on the left hemisphere than
the right in more than one half of newborn babies.
Sutherland and his colleagues [14,15] also reported that
the large majority (90%) of individuals with left-sided
cerebral hemispheric dominance were right-footed.

Table 1
Studies that considered laterality and/or its effect on symmetrical/asymmetrical behavior of lower limbs during able-bodied gaita
Author(s)

Parameters

Statistic

Laterality

Symmetry

Singh [55]
Chodera et al. [61]
Damholt and Termansen [68]
Hirasawa [82]
Vanden-Abeele [83]
Rosenrot [63,64]
Claeys [28]
Hamill et al. [29]
Hannah et al. [23]
Matsusaka et al. [85]
Burnie and Brodie [120]
O0 unpuu [49]
Arsenault et al. [25]
Herzog et al. [4]
O0 unpuu and Winter [75]
Gundersen et al. [5]
Baker and Hewison [122]
Devita et al. [52]
Pierotti et al. [50]
Wheelwright et al. [65]
Menard et al. [30]
Crowe et al. [73]
Sadeghi et al. [79]
Hesse et al. [7]
Hart and Gabbard [124]

ST and foot pressure


ST
Kinetics
ST and kinematic
Descriptive
ST
Kinetic
Kinetic
Kinematic
Kinetic
Kinetic
EMG
EMG
Kinetic
EMG
ST and kinematic
Kinetic
EMG
ST
ST
Kinetic
ST and kinetic
Kinetic
Kinetic
Laterality test

Descriptive and t-test


Descriptive
PCC and t-test
T-test
Descriptive
PCC
T-test
T-test
TFDA and PCC
T-test
T-test
ANOVA
PCC
T-test and SI
ANOVA
MANOVA and t-test
ANOVA
PCC and VR
T-test
CV and PCC
Fourier series
PCA and t-test
TFDA, ANOVA and SI
T-test
Descriptive

No
No
No
No
No
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
No
Yes
No
No
No
No
No
No
Yes
No
No
No
No
Yes
Yes
No

Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes

a
ST, spatio-temporal; PCC, Pearson coefficient correlation; TFDA, time and frequency domain analysis; VR, variance ratio; CV, coefficient of
variation; SI, symmetry index; EMG, electromyography; PCA, principal component analysis.

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

Laterality and its possible relation to the lower extremities have also been considered from a structural
and functional point of view in the literature. Structural
and functional biases are of interest in athletics, dancing, use of some musical instruments, and operation of
machines [92]. Earlier explanations of laterality associated with the structure of the human body (anatomical
asymmetry) were influenced by the idea that dominance
extends to the entire body half. Bell [112] claimed that
in right-handed people the left side is the weaker side of
the body. Von Bonin [113] stated that we are generally
right-handed and left-footed. His statement was based
on X-ray studies by Ingelmark [114] who measured the
leg and arm lengths of right- and left-handers. His work
remains the most comprehensive and carefully conducted study on anatomical leg asymmetries to date.
He reported that 85% of right-handed subjects aged
6 13 years had a longer right leg and 85% of righthanded subjects aged 14 20 years had a longer left leg.
Ingelmark also reported that for left-handers, the situation was reversed: the 6 13 age group had a longer left
leg, whereas the 14 20 age group had a longer right
leg. This result is in agreement with Chibber and Singh
[115] who reported that in humans, the left lower limb
was heavier than the right lower limb. Chibber and
Singh [116] also found that the muscular tissue of the
left limb was significantly heavier than the right. Crosssectional studies indicate that the left leg tends to be
longer [92,117,118] and heavier [92,115]. A hint of the
possible connection between laterality and neuroanatomical variables comes from Irving et al. [119]
who counted the number of motor neurons in the
lumbar and sacral regions of the human spinal cord.
All of their five subjects showed remarkable rightleft
asymmetries in cell counts at the third sacral vertebral
level. A larger neural drive on the dominant side could
explain more energy generation by the plantar flexor
muscles [75]. However, little has been reported about
the relationship between laterality and neuroanatomical
variables. Further studies along these lines might be of
interest.
The influence of lateral dominance has been addressed in gait but to date there has been no conclusive
study on the possible effect of lateral dominance on the
symmetrical or asymmetrical behavior of the lower
limbs during able-bodied gait. Laterality was assessed
mainly as a secondary objective after evaluating the
idea of gait symmetry. Hamill et al. [29] tested the
influence of lateral dominance after reporting the presence of gait symmetry. They did not find any statistically significant differences between the dominant and
non-dominant limb when ground reaction force
parameters were compared. No significant difference
was also reported between peak torque values recorded
for the dominant and non-dominant limb of 18 preadolescent males during 15 knee flexions and extensions

39

[120]. In another study, the relationship between foot


width and limb dominance was examined in 996 subjects [121] and no significant relationship was reported.
Gundersen et al. [5] claimed that there is no relation
between footedness and gait asymmetry and that even
when footedness has been identified, gait asymmetry
could not be predicted by lateral dominance. Baker and
Hewison [122] also found no significant differences
between dominant and non-dominant limbs using temporal and kinematic data.
In contrast, several studies suggested that the asymmetrical behavior of the lower limbs was affected by
laterality. Asymmetry in toed-out and heel positions
relative to walking direction [61] were associated with
asymmetry in motor control function and in the effect
of brain hemispheric dominance. Kerstein et al. [123]
observed that lower limb amputees rehabilitated faster
when it was the non-dominant limb that had been
amputated. These authors also claimed that the dominant limb compensates better for interrupted proprioception and tactile inputs. Damholt and Termansen [68]
found that the dominant leg was stronger in plantar
flexion, while Hirasawa [82] suggested that the dominant leg played a stabilizing function. In the analysis of
proximal and axial neuromotor mechanisms of the
human body, Vanden-Abeele [83] might be among the
first to suggest a relationship between laterality and
functional gait asymmetry in the lower limbs. O0 unpuu
[49] observed a relationship between plantar flexion,
EMG profiles and limb dominance. Arsenault et al.
[17,25] hypothesized that the different functional behavior of the lower limbs might be related to the different
recruitment profiles of the dominant versus the nondominant leg. While Gundersen et al. [5] concluded that
asymmetry cannot be related to lateral dominance,
Singh [55] and Rosenrot [63] claimed that one lower
limb appears as the dominant limb during gait. Based
on the definition of the dominant leg by Hirasawa [82],
Matsusaka et al. [85] reported that medio-lateral balance in walking was controlled by the left limb, which
was the non-dominant side. Although Gundersen et al.
[5] concluded that asymmetry cannot be predicted from
lateral dominance, O0 unpuu and Winter [75] found
some evidence to suggest that plantar flexor EMG
activity was related to limb dominance. In 1991, Devita
and his colleagues [52] also documented that the dominant right limb of their subjects generated between 56
and 61% of total positive work during walking at
natural speed. A theory proposed by Previc [105] indicated that asymmetry in the lower limbs should correlate positively with measures that reflect unilateral
dominance. According to Wheelwright et al. [65], gait
asymmetry may be related to the position of the foot
during the swing phase, reflecting the subjects
laterality.

40

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

Laterality was also considered in the literature as


another explanation for the functional behavior of the
lower limbs during gait. For example, Sadeghi et al.
[79] stated that functional asymmetry in the gait of
able-bodied subjects might be related to limb dominance. Although they did not directly examine this
speculation, five activities kicking a ball, hopping on
one foot, throwing a ball, writing and opening a jar
were used to determine limb preference.
With 47 right- and 32 left-footed subjects, Hart and
Gabbard [124] examined the mobilizing feature of footedness in a unilateral and bilateral context. The subjects
were asked to perform two tasks: draw their initials in
a sandbox with one foot, and roll a golf ball around a
circle as quickly and accurately as possible, while they
were positioned in an adjustable chair with bare feet
flat on the ground and a knee angle of approximately
90. Hart and Gabbard found that 98% of right footers
and 84% of left footers used the preferred limb and
concluded that there is a strong relationship between
limb preference and mobilization tasks in a bilateral
and unilateral context.
Although laterality might be considered as one explanation of the existence of functional gait asymmetry in
able-bodied gait, it seems that the basic question of foot
dominance has not been settled. Researchers are still far
from accepting this speculation as proven until the
relationship between gait asymmetry and laterality has
been objectively examined. More information is needed
about the behavior of the lower limbs in all stability,
mobility and stability/mobility activities. Even then, it
is important to know if a single definition would be
appropriate for limb dominance and asymmetry. Although the general operational meaning of limb dominance seems reasonable with most bilateral conditions,
especially when mobilization rather than stabilization is
paramount, it is simply not representative of the multiple functional characteristics of footedness. Until more
research is conducted to identify the dominant limb in
all contexts, it seems reasonable that foot dominance
should be defined operationally in terms of a behavioral
context (stability, mobility, bilaterality). It can be also
be defined in terms of the demands of the levels of
complexity of the particular task(s) performed.
One other concern about gait symmetry or asymmetry and laterality is the way observations were
quantified. The following section briefly reviews the
different ways of quantifying symmetry and laterality.

4. Quantifying symmetry and laterality


Fig. 1 presents a way of categorizing different means
of determining whether or not symmetry and laterality
exist between the lower limbs using indices

Fig. 1. Quantification of symmetry and laterality.

[3,4,23,25,65,69,75,76,90,119,125136] and statistical


analysis [5,23,58,79,137,138].
To determine asymmetries in ground reaction force
patterns in normal gait, Robinson et al. [129] proposed
an equation called the symmetry index (SI) which was
also used by Herzog et al. [4]:
SI=

(XR XL)
100%
0.5(XR + XL)

where XR and XL are the values of the gait variable


measured for the right and left limb, respectively. When
SI= 0, the gait is symmetrical. Using such an equation,
however, may have major limitations because differences are reported against their average value. For
example, if a large asymmetry is present, the average
value does not correctly reflect the performance of
either limb. Also, parameters that have large values but
relatively small inter-limb differences will tend to lower
the index and reflect symmetry. In spite of these limitations, Becker et al. [136] were able to show that the
successful surgical treatment of ankle fractures in
young adults resulted in improved gait symmetry in
terms of plantar pressure distribution.
To quantify gait symmetry or asymmetry, the value
derived for a gait parameter at the right limb divided by
the associated values at the left limb (R= XR/XL, called
the ratio index) has been applied. A ratio of one was
indicative of a reciprocal gait pattern while higher or

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

lower values reflected asymmetries. Ganguli et al. [139]


and Seliktar and Mizrahi [140] applied this index to
investigate peak velocity of below-knee amputees, while
Andres and Stimmel [125] used it to assess lower limb
prosthetic alignment. The ratio index was also applied
by Wall and Turnbull [32] to determine temporal gait
asymmetries in 25 patients with residual hemiplegia. A
similar approach involving the ratio of the standard
deviations was considered by Kljajic et al. [141]. To
represent perfect gait pattern symmetry, Vagenas and
Hoshizaki [142] developed a new ratio:
Ia =

(L R)
100
max(L, R)

where R and L are the right and left limb values,


respectively. The scores were recorded to reflect trichotomized values of 1, 0 and 1. Asymmetries larger
than 1% were given a value of 91 and in all other
cases, 0 represented symmetry. As Wall and Turnbull
[32] pointed out, using the ratio index has limitations
particularly from a clinical viewpoint. The two main
potential limitations are relatively small asymmetry and
a failure to provide information regarding the location
of asymmetry. In addition, because of the large percentage of stride time spent in total support and the relatively small difference between the affected and
unaffected sides, low sensitivity could be another limitation of the ratio index.
Using statistical approaches to determine similarities
or dissimilarities between the lower limbs might eliminate the main limitations of using the ratio index. Few
studies have assessed asymmetry using several gait
parameters in a single analysis, though statistical tools
are available. Analyses have included correlation coefficients [25,50], coefficients of variation [143], and variance ratios [44,50,142]. Gundersen et al. [5] did a
two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
with repeated measures because the temporal and kinematic variables were not independent of each other. A
NewmanKeul post-hoc test was also performed for
the variables and showed a significant within-subject
difference between limbs. They also used a Pearson
product-moment correlation to determine the relationships among 12 measured gait variables. Paired t-tests
were also used by Allard et al. [58] between right and
left limb data to determine the presence of gait asymmetry. In their study, no statistically significant differences between the limbs for the spatio-temporal
parameters were observed. Differences were noted between the mechanical energies, which mostly occurred
in the sagittal plane and were generally associated with
energy absorption. Sadeghi et al. [79] also applied ttests for paired data to determine whether or not there
were significant differences between spatio-temporal
parameters, muscle powers and their associated mechanical energies, identified by applying principal com-

41

ponent analysis (PCA) to the data for each lower limb.


Hannah et al. [23] claimed that time and frequency
domains can be considered indices of symmetry when a
correlation coefficient of 1.0 is obtained. Giakas and
Baltzopoulos [74] later applied frequency domain analysis, suggesting that it is an appropriate method to
evaluate gait symmetry, since it allows better comparisons between normal and pathological subjects, if variability is known.
Using sophisticated statistical models could provide
better insight into understanding a complex phenomenon such as gait asymmetry. This is because many
factors are involved, including a large number of gait
parameters that should be evaluated at the same time,
e.g. laterality, disability, functional behavior of the
lower limbs, compensatory mechanisms, etc. This might
explain why recent gait studies have applied advanced
multivariate data analysis to examine the similarity or
dissimilarity in the behavior of the lower limbs during
walking performance. PCA, as a classification and data
structure detection method, was used by Sadeghi et al.
[79] to identify the most informative biomechanical
muscle powers in both limbs during gait of 19 young
healthy subjects. PCA was also used to characterize the
large number of variables calculated for hemiplegic
patients [144]. This study also evaluated gait symmetry
in stroke patients. Recently, Sadeghi et al. [80] applied
PCA as a curve structure detection method, to identify
the main functional tasks of hip sagittal muscle power
curves. Functional gait asymmetry was reported because of the task priority performed by the hip flexors/
extensors during the gait cycle. The Time Fourier Series
was used by Crowe et al. [72,73], who reported consistent gait asymmetry in the oscillation of the body center
of mass during gait.
While symmetry indices or statistical approaches
were used to evaluate gait asymmetry, laterality (Fig. 1)
was mainly determined by self-report or questionnaires
[79,80,87,101,145152]. Observations and testing were
also used to identify laterality. The few well-known and
common tests used to determine laterality include kicking a ball, leading off, stepping up on a chair, pressing
a sheet of paper down with the foot, and choice of foot
for picking up a pebble, one or more of which were
used by different researchers [95,153162].

5. Summary and conclusion


Symmetrical behavior of the lower limbs during gait
has often been assumed, mainly for simplicity in data
collection and analysis, while gait asymmetry seems to
reflects a natural functional difference between the
limbs. This functional difference does not appear to be
the consequence of abnormality, but rather relates to
the contribution of each limb to propulsion and control
tasks. Laterality may be another explanation for the

42

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

presence of functional differences between the lower


extremities. Basically, an action towards a goal is carried out by the preferred limb, while support is provided by the other limb. However, more work is needed
to evaluate gait symmetry and to determine its possible
relationship to laterality. For example, gait symmetry
and the influence of laterality could be examined by a
research team involving both biomechanists and neurophysiologists, since gait is the result of the integration
of biomechanical, neuro-physiological and motor control actions. In addition, it would be important to use
gait parameters which provide information about both
the cause and the effect of the movement, and to avoid
considering redundant measurements. For example,
muscle power appears to be a good indicator of a
persons ability both to propel the body and to control
balance during gait.

References
[1] Pratt DJ. Some aspects of modern orthotics. Physiol Meas
1994;15(1):1 27.
[2] Joffeir J. Gait disturbance. Austr Family Physicians
1992;21(10):1437 40.
[3] Soudan K. Standardization of gait kinematic data using a gait
symmetry index and Fourier analysis. In: Huiskes R, Van
Camen DK, De Wijn JR, editors. Biomechanics: Principles and
Applications, Development in Biomechanics, vol. 1. Martinus
Nijhoff, 1982:135 40.
[4] Herzog W, Nigg BM, Read LJ, Olsson E. Asymmetries in
ground reaction force patterns in normal human gait. Med Sci
Sports Exerc 1989;21(1):1104.
[5] Gundersen LA, Valle DR, Barr AE, Danoff JV, Stanhope SJ,
Snyder-Mackler L. Bilateral analysis of the knee and ankle
during gait: an examination of the relationship between lateral
dominance and symmetry. Phys Ther 1989;69(8):64050.
[6] Griffin MP, Olney SJ, McBride ID. Role of symmetry in gait
performance of stroke subjects with hemiplegia. Gait Posture
1995;3:132 42.
[7] Hesse S, Reiter F, Jahnke M, Dawson M, SarkodieGyan T,
Mauritz KH. Asymmetry of gait initiation in hemiparetic
stroke subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehab 1997;78(7):719 24.
[8] Gabbard C. Coming to terms with laterality. J Psychol
1997;131(5):561 4.
[9] Milner M, Basmajian JV, Quanbury AO. Multifactorial analysis of walking by electromyography and computer. Am J Phys
Med 1971;50(5):23558.
[10] Cavanagh PR, Gregor RJ. Knee joint torque during the swing
phase of normal treadmill walking. J Biomech 1975;8:337 44.
[11] Dubo HIC, Peat M, Winter DA, Quanbury AO, Hobson DA,
Steinke T, Reimer G. Electromyograpic temporal analysis of
gait: normal human locomotion. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
1976;57:415 20.
[12] Elliot BC, Blansky BA. Reliability of averaged integrated electromyograms during running. J Hum Mov Stud 1976;2:28 35.
[13] Crowinshield RD, Brand RA, Johnston RC. The effects of
walking velocity on hip kinematics and kinetics. Clin Orthop
1978;132:140 4.
[14] Sutherland DH, Olshen R, Cooper L, Daniel D. The development of mature gait. Am J Bone Jt Surg 1980;62:33653.
[15] Sutherland DH. Gait Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence.
Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1984:201.

[16] Yang JF, Winter DA. Surface EMG profiles during different
walking cadences in humans. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1985;60:485 91.
[17] Arsenault AB, Winter DA, Marteniuk RG. Is there a normal
profile of EMG activity in gait. Med Biol Eng Comput
1986;24:337 43.
[18] Arsenault AB, Winter DA, Marteniuk RG, Hayes KC. How
many strides are required for the analysis of electromyographic
data in gait? Scand J Rehabil Med 1986;18:133 5.
[19] Apkarian J, Naumann S, Cairns B. A three-dimensional kinematic and dynamic model of the lower limb. J Biomech
1989;22:143 55.
[20] Kadaba MP, Ramakrishnan HK, Wootten ME, Gainey J,
Gorton G, Cochran GVB. Repeatability of kinematic, kinetic
and electromyographic data in normal adult gait. J Orthop Res
1989;7:849 60.
[21] Vaughan CL, Davis BL, OConnor JC. Dynamics of Human
Gait, Champaign, Human Kinetics, 1992:137.
[22] Eng JJ, Winter DA. Kinetic analysis of the lower limbs during
walking: what information can be gained from a three-dimensional model? J Biomech 1995;28(6):753 8.
[23] Hannah RE, Morrison JB, Chapman AE. Kinematic symmetry
of the lower limbs. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1984;65(4):1558.
[24] Winter DA. Kinematic and kinetic patterns in human gait:
variability and compensating effects. Hum Move Sci
1984;3:51 76.
[25] Arsenault AB, Winter DA, Marteniuk RG. Bilateralism of
EMG profiles in human locomotion. Am J Phys Med
1986;65(1):1 16.
[26] O0 unpuu S, Gage JR, Davis RB. Three-dimensional lower extremity joint kinetics in normal pediatric gait. J Pediatr Orthop
1991;11:341 9.
[27] Van der Straaten JHM, Scholton PMJ. Symmetry and periodicity in gait patterns of normal and hemiplegic children. In:
Easmussen R, Jorgense R, editors. Biomechanics VI. University
Park Press, 1978:287 92.
[28] Claeys R. The analysis of ground reaction forces in pathological gait secondary to disorders of the foot. Int Orthop
1983;7(2):113 9.
[29] Hamill J, Bates BT, Knutzen KM. Ground reaction forces
symmetry during walking and running. Res Q 1984;55:28893.
[30] Menard MR, McBride ME, Sanderson DJ, Murray DD. Comparative biomechanical analysis of energy-storing prosthetic
feet. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1992;73(5):451 8.
[31] Chou LS, Song SM, Draganich LF. Predicting the kinematics
and kinetics of gait based on the optimum trajectory of the
swing limb. J Biomech 1995;28(4):377 85.
[32] Wall JC, Turnbull GI. Gait asymmetries in residual hemiplegia.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1986;67:550 3.
[33] Gray EG, Basmajian JV. Electromyography and cinematography of leg and foot during walking. Anat Rec 1968;161(1):1
15.
[34] Lyons K, Perry J, Gronley JK, Barnes L, Antonelli D. Timing
and relative intensity of hip extensor and abductor muscle
action during level and stair ambulation: an EMG study. Phys
Ther 1983;63(10):1597 605.
[35] Battye CK, Joseph J. An investigation by telemetring of the
activity of some muscles in walking. Med Biol Eng 1966;4:125
35.
[36] Eberhart HD, Inman VT, Bresler B. The principal elements in
human locomotion. In: Klopsteg PE, Wilson PD, editors. Human Limbs and their Substitutes. New York: McGraw Hill,
1954.
[37] Sheffield R, Gersten J, Mastellone A. Electromyographic study
of the muscles of the foot in normal walking. Am J Phys Med
1956;35:223 36.

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445


[38] Sutherland DH. An electromyographic study of the plantar
flexors of the ankle in normal walking on the level. Am J Bone
Jt Surg 1966;48(1):6671.
[39] Grieve DW, Cavagnah PR. How electromyographic patterns
and limb movements are related to speed of walking. In:
Human Locomotor Engineering. London: Institution of Mechanical Engineers, 1974.
[40] Shiavi R, Champion S, Freemon F, Griffin P. Variability of
electromyographic patterns for level-surface walking through a
range of self-selected speeds. Bull Prosth Res 1981;18:5 14.
[41] Yang JF, Winter DA. Electromyographic amplitude normalization methods. Improving their sensitivity as diagnostic tools in
gait analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1984;65:51721.
[42] Murray MP, Mollinger, Gardner GM, Sepic SB. Kinematic and
EMG patterns during slow, free, and fast walking, J Orthop
Res 1984;2:272 280.
[43] Kadaba MP, Wootten ME, Gainey J, Cochran GVB. Repeatability of phasic muscle activity: performance of surface
and intra-muscular wire electrodes in gait analysis. J Orthop
Res 1985;3:350 9.
[44] Winter DA, Yack HJ. EMG profiles during normal human
walking: stride-to-stride and inter-subject variability. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987;67:40211.
[45] Mero A, Komi P. Electromyographic activity in sprinting at
speeds ranging from sub-maximal to supra maximal. Med Sci
Sports Exerc 1987;19:26674.
[46] Carlsoo S, Dahllof A, Holm J. An analysis of the gait in
patients with hemiparesis and in patients with intermittent
claudication. Scand J Rehabil Med 1974;6:16679.
[47] Shiavi R, Green N, McFadyen B, Frazer M, Chen J. Normative childhood EMG gait patterns. J Orthop Res 1987;5:283
95.
[48] Marks M, Hirschberg G. Analysis of the hemiplegic gait. Ann
NY Acad Sci 1958;74:5977.
[49] O0 unpuu S. Bilateral analysis of the lower limb during walking
in normal individuals. Unpublished Masters Thesis. University
of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, 1986.
[50] Pierotti SE, Brand RA, Gabel RH, Pedersen DR, Clarke WR.
Are leg electromyogram profiles symmetrical. J Orthop Res
1991;9(5):720 9.
[51] Skinner HB, Effeney DJ. Gait analysis in amputee. Am J Phys
Med 1985;64(2):82 9.
[52] Devita P, Hong D, Hamill J. Effects of asymmetric load
carrying on the biomechanics of walking. J Biomech
1991;24(12):1119 29.
[53] Titianova EB, Tarkka IM. Asymmetry in walking performance
and postural sway in patients with chronic unilateral cerebral
infarction. J Rehabil Res Dev 1995;32(3):23644.
[54] Kaufman KR, Miller LS, Sutherland DH. Gait asymmetry in
patients with limb-length inequality. J Pediatr Orthop
1996;16:144 50.
[55] Singh I. Functional asymmetry in the lower limbs. Acta Anat
1970;77:131 8.
[56] DuChatinier K, Rozendal RH. Temporal symmetry gait of
selected normal subjects. Anatomy 1970;73:35361.
[57] Law HT. Microcomputer-based, low cost method for measurement of spatial and temporal parameters of gait. J Biomed Eng
1987;9(2):115 20.
[58] Allard P, Lachance R, Aissaoui R, Duhaime M. Simultaneous
bilateral 3-D able-bodied gait. Hum Mov Sci 1996;15:327 46.
[59] Barr A, Andersen JC, Danoff JV, et al. Symmetry of temporal,
Spatial and kinematic events during gait. Read at the Third
Annual East Coast Gait Laboratory Conference, Bethesda,
MD, 1987.
[60] Chodera JD, Levell RW. Footprint patterns during walking. In:
Kenedi RM, editor. Perspectives in Biomedical Engineering.
Baltimore: University Park Press, 1973:8190.

43

[61] Chodera JD. Analysis of gait from footprints. Physiotherapy


1974;60:179 81.
[62] Stefanyshyn DJ, Ensgberg JR. Right to left differences in the
ankle joint complex range of motion. Med Sci Sport Exerc
1994;26(5):551 5.
[63] Rosenrot P. Asymmetry of gait and the relationship to lower
limb dominance. Proceeding of the special Conference of the
Canadian Society for Biomechanics, 1980:26 7.
[64] Rosenrot P, Wall JC, Charteris J. The relationship between
velocity, stride time, support time and swing time during normal walking. J Hum Mov Stud 1980;6:323 35.
[65] Wheelwright EF, Minns RA, Law HT, Elton RA. Temporal
and spatial parameters of gait in children, I: normal control
data. Dev Med Child Neurol 1993;35(2):102 13.
[66] Vaughan CL. Are joint torques the Holy Grail of human gait
analysis? Hum Mov Sci 1996;15:423 43.
[67] Vaughan CL, Davis BL, OConner JC. Dynamic of Human
Gait. Champaign: Human Kinetics, 1992.
[68] Damholt V, Termansen NB. Asymmetry of planter flexion
strength in the foot. Acta Orthop Scand 1978;49:215 9.
[69] Balakrishan S, Thornton-Trump AB. Integral parameters in
human locomotion Proceeding of the 2nd Biannual Conference
of the Canadian Society for Biomechanics, Human Locomotion
II, 1982:12 3.
[70] Herzog W, Nigg BM, Read LJ. Quantifying the effects of spine
manipulations on gait using patients with low back pain. J
Manipulative Physio Ther 1988;11:151 7.
[71] Dickey JP, Winter DA. Adaptations in gait resulting from
unilateral ischaemic block of the leg. Clin Biomech 1992;7:215
25.
[72] Crowe A, Schiereck P, de Boer R, Keessen W. Characterization
of gait of young adult females by means of body centre of mass
oscillations derived from ground reaction forces. Gait Posture
1993;1:61 8.
[73] Crowe A, Schiereck P, de Boer RW, Keessen W. Characterization of human gait by means of body center of mass oscillations
derived from ground reaction forces. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng
1995;42(3):293 303.
[74] Giakas G, Baltzopoulos V. Time and frequency domain analysis of ground reaction forces during walking: an investigation of
variability and symmetry. Gait Posture 1997;5:189 97.
[75] O0 unpuu S, Winter DA. Bilateral electromyographical analysis
of the lower limbs during walking in normal adults. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989;72:429 38.
[76] Boucher JP, Hodgdon JA. Anatomical, Mechanical, and functional factor underlying overuse syndrome. Communication
presented at ICSM-91 convention, Arlington, VA, 1991.
[77] Czerniecki JM, Gitter AJ, Beck JC. Energy transfer mechanisms as a compensatory strategy in below-knee amputee runners. J Biomech 1996;29(6):717 22.
[78] Winter DA, Sienko SE. Biomechanics of below knee amputee
gait. J Biomech 1988;21:361 7.
[79] Sadeghi H, Allard P, Duhaime M. Functional gait asymmetry
in able-bodied subjects. Hum Mov Sci 1997;16:243 58.
[80] Sadeghi H, Allard P, Prince F. Functional characteristics of
bilateral hip power in human gait using principal component
analysis, J Physics Med Eng (submitted).
[81] Winter DA. Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Gait:
Normal, Elderly and Pathological, 2nd ed. Waterloo, Ont.,
Canada: University of Waterloo Press, 1991.
[82] Hirasawa Y. An observation on standing ability of Japanese
males and females. J Anthrop Soc Nippon 1979;87:8192.
[83] Vanden-Abeele J. Comments on the functional asymmetry of
the lower extremities. Cortex 1980;16(2):325 9.
[84] Hirasawa Y. Left leg supporting human straight (bipedal)
standing. Saiensu 1981;6:32 44.

44

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445

[85] Matsusaka N, Fujitta M, Hamamina A, Norimatsu T, Suzuki


R. Relationship between right and left leg in human gait from
a viewpoint of balance control. In: Winter DA, Norman R,
Wells R, Hayes K, Patla A, editors. Biomechanics IX-A. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers, 1985:42730.
[86] Hirokawa S. Normal gait characteristics under temporal and
distance constraints. J Biomed Eng 1989;11(6):44956.
[87] Sadeghi H, Allard P, Duhaime M. Lower limb muscle powers
contributions in bilateral able-bodied gait, Phys Ther
(submitted).
[88] McClay IS. The use of gait analysis to enhance the understanding of running injuries. In: Craik RL, Oatis CA, editors. Gait
Analysis: Theory and Application. Mosby, 1995:395411.
[89] Waziri R. Lateralization of neuroleptic-induced dyskiesia indicates pharmacologic asymmetry in the brain. Psychopharmacology 1980;68:51 3.
[90] Cavagna GA, Tesio L, Fuchimoto T, Heglund NC. Ergometric
evaluation of pathological gait. J Appl Physiol Resp Env Exerc
Physiol 1983;55(2):60713.
[91] Guyton AC. Textbook of Medical Physiology, 8th ed. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1991:6389.
[92] Peters M. Footedness: asymmetries in foot preference and skill
and neuro-psychological assessment of foot movement. Psychol
Bull 1988;103(2):17992.
[93] Colborne GR, Naumann S, Longmuir PE, Brebrayer D. Analysis of mechanical and metabolic factors in the gait of congenital
below knee amputees: a comparison of the SACH and the
Seattle feet. Am J Phy Med Rehabil 1992;71(5):2728.
[94] Colborne GR, Wright FV, Naumann S. Feedback of triceps
square EMG in gait of children with cerebral palsy: a controlled study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1994;75(1):405.
[95] Nachshon I, Denno D, Aurand S. Lateral preference of hand,
eye and foot: relation to cerebral dominance. Int J Neurosci
1983;18(1 2):1 9.
[96] Gabbard C, Hart S. A question of foot dominance. J Gen
Psychol 1996;123:28996.
[97] Edelstein JE. Current choices in prosthetic feet. Crit Rev Phys
Rehabil Med 1991;2:1326.
[98] Engsberg JR, Tedford KG, Harder JA, Mills JP. Timing
changes for stance, swing and double support in a recent below
knee amputee child. Pediatr Exerc Sci 1990;2:25562.
[99] Engsberg JR, Lee AG, Patterson JL, Harder JA. External
loading comparisons between able-bodied and below-knee amputee children during walking. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
1991;72:657 61.
[100] Gabbard C. Foot lateralization and psychomotor control in
four-years olds. Percept Motor Skills 1989;68(2):6758.
[101] Dargent-Pare C, De Agostini M, Mesbah M, Dellatolas G.
Foot and eye preferences in adults: relationship with handedness, sex and age. Cortex 1992;28(3):34351.
[102] Gentry V, Gabbard C. Foot preference behavior: a developmental perspective. J Gen Psychol 1995;122(1):3745.
[103] Spry S, Zebas C, Visser M. What is leg dominance? In: Hamill
J, editor. Biomechanics in Sport XI. Proceedings of the XI
Symposium of the International Society of Biomechanics in
Sports. MA: Amherst, 1993.
[104] Whittington JE, Richards PN. The stability of childrens laterality prevalences and their relationship to measures of performance. Br J Edu Psychol 1987;57(1):4555.
[105] Previc FH. A general theory concerning the prenatal origins of
cerebral lateralization in humans. Psychol Rev 1991;98:299
334.
[106] Rudel RG, Healey J, Denckla MB. Development of motor
co-ordination by normal left-handed children. Dev Med Child
Neurol 1984;26(1):10411.
[107] Armitage M, Larkin D. Laterality, motor asymmetry clumsiness in children. Hum Mov Sci 1993;12:15577.

[108] Katsarkas A, Smith H, Galiana H. Postural instability on one


foot in patients with loss of unilateral peripheral vestibular
function. J Vest Res 1994;4(2):153 60.
[109] Gabbard C, Hart S. Probing Prevics theory of postural control
(foot dominance). Brain Cogn 1996;30(3):351 3.
[110] Broca P. Sur le siege de la faculte du langage articule. Bull Soc
Anthrop 1865;6:377 93.
[111] White MJ. Hemispheric asymmetries in tacbistoscopic information processing. Br J Psychol 1972;63(4):497 508.
[112] Bell C. The hand; its mechanism and vital endowments, as
evincing design and illustrating the power, wisdom, and goodness of God. London: Bell and Daldy, 1870.
[113] Von Bonin F. Anatomical asymmetries of the cerebral hemispheres. In: Mountcastle VB, editor. Interhemispheric relations
and cerebral dominance. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1962.
[114] Ingelmark BE. Asymmetries in the length of the extremities and
their relation to right- and left-handedness. Upsala Laekarefoerening Forhandlingar 1974;52:17 82.
[115] Chibber SR, Singh I. Asymmetry in muscle and bone weight in
the lower limb of human foetuses. J Anat Soc Ind 1972;21:33
9.
[116] Chibber SR, Singh I. Asymmetry in muscle weight and onesided dominance in the human lower limbs. J Anat Lond
1970;106:553 6.
[117] Levy J, Levy JM. Human lateralization from head to foot:
sex-related factors. Science 1978;200:1291 2.
[118] Pomerantz AP, Harris L. Are there sex-related asymmetries in
foot size? Percept Motor Skill 1980;51:675 8.
[119] Irving O, Rebeiz JJ, Tomlinson BE. The numbers of limb
motor neurons in the individual segments of the human lumbosacral cord. J Neurol Sci 1974;21:203 12.
[120] Burnie J, Brodie DA. Isokinetic measurement in preadolescent
males. Int J Sport Med 1986;7(4):205 9.
[121] Didia BC, Nyenwe EA. Foot breath in children its relationship to limb dominance and age. Foot Ankle 1988;8(4):198
202.
[122] Baker PA, Hewison SR. Gait recovery pattern of unilateral
lower limb amputee during rehabilitation. Prosthet Orthot Int
1990;14(2):80 4.
[123] Kerstein MD, Zimmer H, Dugdale FE, Lerner E. Successful
rehabilitation following amputation of dominant versus nondominant extremities. Am J Occup Ther 1977;31(5):3135.
[124] Hart S, Gabbard C. Examining the mobilizing feature of footedness. Percept Motor Skill 1998;86:1339 42.
[125] Andres RO, Stimmel SK. Prosthetic alignment effects on gait
symmetry: a case study. Clin Biomech 1990;5:88 96.
[126] Dingwell JB, Davis BL, Frazier DM, Campbell JH. Real time
symmetry assessment and feedback in amputee rehabilitation.
Proceeding of the Eight Biennial Conference, Canadian Society
for Biomechanics, 1994:18 20.
[127] Hannah RE, Morrison JB, Chapman AE. Prostheses alignment: effect on gait of person with below-knee amputations.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1984;65(4):159 62.
[128] Herzog W, Conway PJW, Willox BJ. Effects of different treatment modalities on gait symmetry and clinical measures for
sacroiliac joint patients. J Manipulative Physiol Ther
1991;14(2):104 9.
[129] Robinson RO, Herzog W, Nigg BM. Use of force platform
variables to quantify the effects of chiropractic manipulation on
gait symmetry. J Manipulative Physiol Ther 1987;10:1726.
[130] Schneider K, Hart T, Zernicke RF, Setoguchi J, Oppenhiem W.
Dynamics of below knee-child amputee gait: SACH foot versus
flexfoot. J Biomech 1993;26(10):1191 204.
[131] White SC, Lifeso RM. Bilateral asymmetry in foot ground
reaction force of candidates for a unilateral total hip replacement. Gait Posture 1994;2(1):63.

H. Sadeghi et al. / Gait and Posture 12 (2000) 3445


[132] Liu XC, Fabry G, Molenaers G, Lammens J, Moens P. Kinematic and kinetic asymmetry in patients with leg-length discrepancy. J Pediatr Orthop 1998;18(2):1879.
[133] Roth EJ, Merbitz C, Mroczek K, Dugan SA, Suh WW. Hemiplegic gait, Relationships between walking speed and other
temporal parameters. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 1997;76(2):128
33.
[134] Hurmuzlu Y, Basdogan C, Stoianovici D. Kinematic and dynamic stability of locomotion of post-polio patients. J Biomech
Eng 1996;118(3):40511.
[135] Shiomi A. Idiopatic Scoliosis by spinal fusion and brace treatment. Evaluation by gait analysis. J Jap Orthop Assoc
1995;69(9):665 74.
[136] Becker HP, Rosenbaum D, Kriese T, Gerngrob H, Claes L.
Gait asymmetry following successful surgical treatment of ankle
fracture in young adults. Clin Orthop Rel Res 1995;311:262 9.
[137] Loslever P, Laassel EM, Angue JC. Combined statistical study
of joint angles and ground reaction forces using component and
multiple correspondence analysis. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng
1994;41(12):1160 7.
[138] Yamamoto S, Suto Y, Kawamura H, Hashizume T, Kakurai S,
Sugahara S. Quantitative gait evaluation of hip diseases using
principal component analysis. J Biomech 1983;16(9):717 26.
[139] Ganguli S, Mukherji P, Bose KS. Gait evaluation of unilateral
below-knee amputees fitted with patellar-tendon-bearing prostheses. J Ind Med Assoc 1974;63(8):2569.
[140] Seliktar R, Mizrahi J. Some gait characteristics of below-knee
amputees and their reflection on the ground reaction forces.
Eng Med 1986;15(1):2734.
[141] Kljajic M, Bajd T, Stanic U. Quantitative gait evaluation of
hemiplegic patients using electrical stimulation orthoses. IEEE
Trans Biomed Eng 1975;22(5):43841.
[142] Vagenas G, Hoshizaki B. A multivariable analysis of lower
extremity kinematic asymmetry in running. Int J Sports
Biomech 1992;8(1):1129.
[143] Hershler C, Milner M. An optimality criterion for processing
electromyographic (EMG) signals relating to human locomotion. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 1978;25(5):41320.
[144] Olney SJ, Griffin MP, McBride ID. Multivariate examination
of data from gait analysis of persons with stroke. Phys Ther
1998;78(8):814 28.

45

[145] Elias LJ, Bryden MP, Bulman-Fleming MB. Footedness is a


better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization.
Neuropsychology 1998;36(1):37 43.
[146] Dean RS. Assessing patterns of lateral preference. Clin Neuropsychol 1982;4:124 8.
[147] Liederman J, Healey JM. Independent dimensions of hand
preference: reliability of the factor structure and the handedness
inventory. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1986;1:371 86.
[148] Harris AJ. The Harris Tests of Lateral Dominance, 2nd ed.
New York: NY Psychological Corporation, 1955.
[149] Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the
Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychology 1971;9:97 113.
[150] Coren S, Porac C. The validity and reliability of self report
items for the measurement of lateral preference. Br J Psychol
1978;69:207 11.
[151] McMeckan ERL, Lishman WA. Retest reliabilities and interrelationship of the Annett Hand Preference Questionnaire and
the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. Br J Psychol
1975;66:53 9.
[152] Roszkowski MJ, Shelbecker GE. Temporal stability and predictive validity of self-assessed hand preference with first and
second graders. Brain Cogn 1982;1:405 9.
[153] Gould GM. Right Handedness and Left Handedness. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, 1908.
[154] Harris AJ. Harris Tests of Lateral Dominance. New York:
Psychological Corporation, 1958.
[155] Stern G, Schilf E. Uber Linksfussigkeit [About left footedness].
Zeitschrift Gesamte Neurologie Psychiatrie 1932;139:413.
[156] Komai T, Fukuoka G. A study of the frequency of left-handedness and left-footedness among Japanese school children. Hum
Biol 1934;6:33 42.
[157] Gardner LP. Experimental data on the problem of motor
lateral dominance in feet and hands. Psychol Rec 1941;5:163.
[158] Blau A. The dominant hand (Research Monograph No. 5).
New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association, 1964.
[159] Cohen J. Handedness, speech pathology and hemispheric asymmetry. Triangle 1976;15:93 101.
[160] Nachshon I, Denno D. Birth order and laterality preferences.
Cortex 1986;22:567 78.
[161] Haefner R. The relation between hand and foot tendencies of
children. Ped Semin 1930;38:338.
[162] Augustyn C, Peters M. Measures of foot preference and performance. Unpublished manuscript, 1986.

Potrebbero piacerti anche