Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Studies in Educational Evaluation


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/stueduc

A bullying intervention system in high school: A two-year school-wide follow-up


Kathleen P. Allen *
University of Rochester, Margaret Warner Graduate School of Education and Human Development, Box 270425, Rochester, NY 14627-0425, United States

A R T I C L E I N F O

A B S T R A C T

Article history:
Received 15 March 2010
Received in revised form 21 October 2010
Accepted 25 January 2011

This study is an evaluation of a systemic, two-year, whole-school bullying intervention initiative that
was implemented in a US public high school. Students and staff members were anonymously surveyed
before and after the intervention. The goals of the initiative were to reduce bullying and victimization,
increase disclosure, increase intervention efforts, and reduce student aggression. Except for a reduction
in victimization, all goals were achieved in some measure. Self-reported bullying decreased 50% or more.
Students reporting that peers intervened in bullying increased. Staff-reported reductions in student
aggression, and staffs belief that the schools efforts to address bullying were adequate increased. This
evaluation points to the possible success of a whole-school, systemic approach to managing bullying at
the high school level.
2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords:
Program evaluation
Bullying
Victimization
Bullying in high school
Bullying among adolescents

Introduction
Statement of the problem
Bullying in schools is a problem which thrust itself onto the
national stage in the United States in the late 1990s, primarily
because of the attention given to the fact that bullying was
implicated in the Columbine shootings (Vossekuil, Fein, Reddy,
Borum, & Modzeleski, 2002). While this event took place in a high
school context, suggesting that bullying was a problem for
adolescents, most research on bullying in schools has focused
on elementary and middle school children with the one major US
survey on the problem excluding students in eleventh and twelfth
grades (Nansel et al., 2001). A review of research on bullying, which
to a large extent reects work done outside of the US, indicates that
studies of bullying seldom go beyond subjects aged 14 (Atlas &
Pepler, 1998; Boulton & Smith, 1994; Craig & Pepler, 1997;
Kaukiainen et al., 2002; OConnell, Pepler, & Craig, 1999; Olweus,
1978; Salmivalli & Kaukiainen, 2004; Salmivalli & Nieminen, 2002;
Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999), and with a rare exception,
consider students up to age 16 (Olweus, 1991; Rigby & Slee, 1991;
Whitney & Smith, 1993).
Perhaps because research on bullying tends to focus on
elementary aged children (Yoon, Barton, & Taiariol, 2004), interventions generally exclude high school students. A review of three

* Correspondence address: 58 Nobleman Court, Fairport, NY 14450, United


States. Tel.: +1 585 509 4893.
E-mail address: katyallen@rochester.rr.com.
0191-491X/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.stueduc.2011.01.002

syntheses of bullying prevention/intervention evaluations suggests


that elementary and middle schools are more often the target of
these programs than are high schools (Baldry & Farrington, 2007;
Smith, Schneider, Smith, & Ananiadou, 2004; Vreeman & Carroll,
2007). Smith et al. (2004) reviewed 14 programs, only two of which
targeted students older than 16 years of age. In a review of 16
evaluations of bullying prevention programs by Baldry and
Farrington (2007), only two included students older than 16 years
of age, and in another review of school-based bullying interventions,
Vreeman and Carroll (2007) included 10 program evaluations, none
of which were implemented in a high school setting. In addition to
fewer interventions with older students, researchers have found
that the effectiveness of the programs targeting older children tends
to be less successful than those targeting younger children (Smith,
Ananiadou, & Cowie, 2003; Smith et al., 2004; Smith, 2010),
indicating the need for more and better interventions for older
children. In fact, Tto, Farrington, and Baldry (2008) recommend
that anti-bullying programs might well target children 11 years of
age or older, as opposed to younger students.
While research and interventions tend to be limited to children
and young adolescents, some studies have begun to describe the
psychosocial problems that adolescent victims of bullying may
experience. In a review of 37 studies that examined the association
between bullying and suicide, Kim and Leventhal (2008) concluded
that not only does bullying interfere with normal development
and educational processes but also places adolescents at an
unnecessary and additional risk for suicidal thoughts and actions
(p. 151). This trend was conrmed in a study by Klomek, Marrocco,
Kleinman, Schonfeld, and Gould (2007) with high school students
in New York State who concluded that frequent exposure to

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

84

victimization or bullying others was related to high risks of


depression, ideation, and suicide attempts compared with adolescents not involved in bullying behavior. Infrequent involvement in
bullying behavior also was related to increased risk of depression
and suicidality, particularly among girls (p. 40). Finally, in a review
of psychiatric conditions associated with bullying among seventh
through twelfth graders, Kumpulainen (2008) suggests that bullying
is so troubling that it should be viewed as an experience that points
to the need for psychiatric evaluation.
Additionally, other researchers have suggested that bullying in
adolescence is related to other forms of aggression including
dating violence, romantic relational aggression, sexual harassment, and workplace harassment (Chapell, Hasselman, Kitchin,
Lomon, MacIver, & Sarullo, 2006; Connolly, Pepler, Craig, &
Taradash, 2000; Gruber & Fineran, 2008; Linder, Crick, & Collins,
2002; Pepler, Craig, Jiang, & Connolly, 2008; Porhola, Karhunen, &
Rainivaara, 2006). Thus, while prevalence rates for bullying and
victimization appear to go down as students get older (Madsen,
1996; Monks & Smith, 2006; Olweus, 1993; Smith, Madsen, &
Moody, 1999) bullying continues to be a problem for some
students as they move through high school and beyond, and is thus
worthy of attention and intervention.
Literature review
A review of meta-analyses of school-based bullying preventionintervention evaluations indicates that there is disagreement
among researchers as to the effectiveness of these types of
programs. Tto et al. (2008) indicate that overall, school programs
designed to reduce bullying and victimization are generally

[()TD$FIG]

effective. Another meta-analysis (Merrell, Gueldner, Ross, & Isava,


2008) draws the conclusion that the majority of outcomes
evidenced no meaningful change, positive or negative. . . school
bullying interventions may produce modest positive outcomes. . .
they are more likely to inuence knowledge, attitudes, and selfperceptions rather than actual bullying behaviors (p. 26).
Vreeman and Carroll (2007) found that there was mixed success
when they reviewed 26 programs that were designed to reduce
bullying and victimization. Some resulted in reductions, but others
did not. Likewise, Smith et al. (2004) found that a small number of
programs yielded positive outcomes with the majority not
showing statistically signicant changes. In a recent article, several
leading bullying researchers concluded that these mixed results
suggest that, although school-based and schoolwide bullying
efforts can (italics in the original) be effective, success in one school
or context is no guarantee of success in another (Swearer,
Espelage, Vaillancourt, & Hymel, 2010).
While evaluation research has not sent clear signals with
regards to the effectiveness of preventionintervention programming in general, the research is fairly clear in indicating that
programs that have at least marginal success, generally adopt a
whole-school approach (Tto et al., 2008; Vreeman & Carroll,
2007). These programs are patterned after the Olweus Bullying
Prevention Program (Olweus, 1993) and include most or all of the
following features: a coordinating committee to oversee the
initiative; surveys to assess needs and measure change; well
disseminated policies and clear rules prohibiting bullying;
education that promotes awareness for parents, staff and students;
and individual support for victims and consequences for bullies.
These activities reect the multi-leveled nature of the intervention

ACTIVITIES
INPUTS
People: Building Planning
Team, SEL* Committee,
Teachers, Support Staff,
Administrators, Parents,
Students
Time
Monetary funds for surveys,
training, group meetings outside
of the school day, consultant
services, and evaluation
activities
Results of the April, 2005
Olweus Survey
Staff Responses to Olweus
Survey Data, Spring, 2006
Charge from the Building
Planning Team to address the
issue of bullying
Support from district level
administrators to design,
implement and evaluate a
bullying preventionintervention initiative

Two, four hour summer workshops


(2006) to explore issues and develop
recommendations for the Building
Planning Team
SEL* Committee meetings, held
monthly throughout the school year
SELiT meetings, held 1-2 times per
month as needed
SEL Committee members study, read,
and explore research on whole-school
bullying intervention efforts
SEL Committee planning for
professional development activities
with MPA staff for 2006-2007-2008
SEL Committee getting and processing
feedback from staff regarding initiative
progress
Development of a system to respond to
bullying
Ongoing professional development for
staff to support their use of the system
Programming for students on the Social
Support System, including a student
prepared video on bullying, assemblies
led by an educational expert on school
bullying, class discussions of the
assemblies, student orientation to the
Social Support System, and a parent
information night on the Social Support
System.

PROXIMAL
OUTCOMES

OUTPUTS
A Continuum of
Responses to
bullying for adult
intervention in
bullying situations

Students, staff and parents report


bullying situations.
Adults effectively intervene in, or
respond to, bullying situations.

A safe, bullying
reporting and
follow-up system
for students, staff
and parents
Heightened student
and staff awareness
of problems around
bullying and how to
access help to solve
these problems
Acceptance of
norms which
support respectful
peer treatment and
a rejection of
bullying as a form
of social interaction

INTERMEDIATE and
DISTAL
OUTCOMES
There is an overall reduction in the
amount and severity of bullying.
The quality of social interactions
among students improves.
The schools social climate becomes
more positive.

* Social-emotional Learning
Social-emotional Learning Intervention Team

Fig. 1. Logic model Meliora Public Academy bullying preventionintervention initiative.

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

in that they address problems at the individual, class, school, and


community levels (Rahey & Craig, 2002; Tto et al., 2008).
Theoretical framework
Researchers who study bullying recommend that those who
seek to reduce bullying and aggression adopt a socialecological
perspective (Swearer & Espelage, 2004; Swearer et al., 2010).
Swearer and Espelage are of the view that bullying should be
considered across multiple contexts that include the individual,
family, peer, school and community. Drawing upon Bronfenbrenners ecological-systems theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979), these
authors have proposed that bullying and victimization occur in
ecologies which are the result of complex inter- and intraindividual variables. They believe that effective prevention and
intervention efforts must take into consideration the social ecology
that promotes and maintains bullying behaviors. This perspective
is reected in the whole-school approach in that it seeks to alter
the context of bullying on multiple levels while addressing interand intra-individual concerns.
Goals of the current study
Following the framework of a whole-school approach to
resolving problems around bullying, the goals of this intervention
were to reduce bullying and victimization by bullying; increase
disclosure of bullying and victimization to adults; increase adult
and student interventions in bullying problems; reduce fear of
bullying; increase empathy for victims of bullying; reduce peer
aggression; increase staff knowledge of bullying and victimization;
and increase staff commitment to intervene in bullying and
victimization.
Program theory-driven evaluation
The evaluation of this initiative was designed using a theorydriven approach to evaluation (Chen & Rossi, 1983, 1992;
Donaldson, 2007; Donaldson & Gooler, 2003). Fig. 1 is a logic
model which describes the inputs, activities, outputs, and outcomes that made up the initiative and which were based on the
goals of the initiative. While the overall evaluation design resulted
in multiple evaluation questions, this report deals primarily with
questions concerning outcomes related specically to bullying and
victimization. An additional process evaluation is planned for
future publication.
Methods
Program description
Meliora Public Academy (a pseudonym), a public ninth through
twelfth grade high school, developed its bullying intervention
initiative after doing a needs assessment and organizing a
committee to oversee the initiative. The core component of the
initiative is the Social Support System (see Allen, 2009, 2010 for a
detailed description) and is comprised the following: (1) a bullying
reporting form which triggers the process; (2) a follow-up form
which documents the steps taken to resolve the problem; (3) an
intervention team which gathers information on problems,
coaches staff members who are dealing with bullying, and acts
as a clearing house for reports; and (4) a continuum of responses
for intervening in bullying which offers alternatives to the
traditional method of assigning blame and punishing bullies.
The Social Support System was considered the main feature of
the initiative because it addressed the most pressing question that
teachers ask: What am I supposed to do when I think bullying is

85

going on? Most bullying preventionintervention programs tend


to focus heavily on staff education that considers denitions,
prevalence, policy, and effects on victims and bystanders, while
giving little clear guidance on what to do and say when bullying is
occurring.
Because the staff was quite knowledgeable about bullying, the
committee felt that their most important work was to create a
continuum of responses to bullying problems that could be
implemented in a consistent way across the entire school. The
committee presented the Social Support System to the entire
faculty at the beginning of the 20072008 school year for piloting
during the rst semester, and then informed students of the
system after the start of the second semester.
The committee decided that the system should be presented to
the student body at a time when sensitivity to the issue of bullying
was high. Thus, the initiative rollout included: (1) the viewing of a
student-made video on bullying by all students on the rst day
back from February break; (2) followed the next day by interactive
assemblies for the student body where a nationally known speaker
discussed respect and bullying; (3) further discussion of the
assembly content during English classes over the next several
days; (4) the presentation of the Social Support System to students
by English teachers; and (5) a parent presentation on the Social
Support System three weeks after the student assemblies. This
initiative approximates a whole-school approach to addressing
bullying (Rahey & Craig, 2002), but does so in a way that considers
the structural features of a ninth through twelfth grade high school
and the developmental nature of adolescents aged 1418.
Design
This study is part of an evaluation which collected data before
and after the intervention. Students and staff members anonymously completed surveys before the implementation of the
initiative. Approximately two years later, during which time the
initiative was implemented, the surveys were administered again.
It was not possible to use repeated measures because there had
been a 50% change in the population of the school due to
graduation of eleventh and twelfth graders and the introduction of
two new classes of students. Likewise, turnover in staff precluded
the ability to survey an entirely identical set of staff members.
Thus, data from the surveys were not matched and participants
reected different groups pre- and post-assessment.
Participants
Participants for this study were the students and staff members
of Meliora Public Academy, a large suburban high school in the
Northeastern United States. Meliora Public Academy (MPA) is one
of two equal-sized high schools in a school district located in an
afuent community outside of a mid-sized city. The National
Center for Education Statistics (Ofce of Educational Research &
Improvements, 2010) indicates that the total student population of
MPA was 992 (485 males), 25 of whom were eligible for free or
reduced lunch (2.5%) for the 20072008 which was the rst year of
the intervention. The racial/ethnic prole for students as reported
by NCES (Ofce of Educational Research & Improvements, 2010)
for 20072008 was 5.4% Asian, 2.6% Black, 1.0% Hispanic, and 90.9%
White. Teachers totaled 79.1 and the student-to-teacher ratio was
12.5. The total student population of the entire school district for
20072008 was 6028 with 53 of those students being described as
English language learners.
Because repeated measures were not employed chi-square
tests of independence were performed to determine if the beforeintervention and after-intervention student samples differed
signicantly by gender or grade. Results indicated that there

86

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

was no statistically signicant difference between the two groups


with regards to gender (x2(1) = 1.515, p = .218) or grade
(x2(1) = 1.28, p = .733). A chi-square was also performed to
determine if the staff members differed before- to after-intervention with regards to gender. The two groups were not found to
differ signicantly (x2(1) = .207, p = .649).

Bullying
Nine items from the student version of the Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire (1996) were considered when determining if the
amount of bullying was different following the intervention as
compared to before. These questions mirrored the victimization
questions and thus the same methods for analysis were used to
measure bullying as were used to measure victimization.

Procedure
Students anonymously completed the Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire (1996) in April, 2007. Two years later, students took
a survey that included questions from the Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire and an additional set of questions that were
developed by the evaluator/author and piloted at a local high
school. The rst administration of the student survey yielded
approximately 870 completed surveys and the second yielded
approximately 820 surveys which represent 88% and 83% of the
student population respectively. The fact that less than 100% of the
students participated in the survey is due to absenteeism, parental
objection to student participation, incomplete surveys, or elimination of the surveys because the responses were grossly
inconsistent (e.g., student reports being a ninth grader and 19
years of age). Because the student body had changed by 50% due to
graduation and new enrollment of younger students, and due to
the inability to track surveys pre- to post-administration, this was
not a matched sample, and thus, repeated measures were not
possible.
Staff members anonymously completed a survey in January,
2007 which was designed by the evaluator/author and piloted by
the bullying initiative committee. Two years later staff members
took another survey which included all of the original questions
and an additional set of questions that mirrored the ones added to
the second student survey. The rst administration of the staff
survey yielded 120 completed surveys and the second yielded 78
surveys. This represents approximately 78% and 50% of the staff
population respectively. Again, this was not a matched sample due
to staff turnover and the inability to track surveys pre- to postadministration.
MPA followed district procedures for administering the student
surveys which were completed during homeroom periods under
the supervision of a teacher. Staff surveys were administered at
faculty or department meetings under the supervision of school
administrators or department leaders. All surveys were anonymous. The data were hand entered into SPSS v14 by district hires
and the program evaluator/author.
Measures
Victimization
Nine items from the student version of Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire (1996) were considered when determining if the
amount of victimization by bullying was different following the
intervention as compared to before. If students indicated that they
had been bullied in any of the nine ways, they were considered to
have been victimized. The original questions were answered using
a 15 point Likert scale with 1 = It has not happened, and 5 = It
happens several times a week. In keeping with Solberg and
Olweus (2003) the variables for victimization were recoded into a
binary variable where 0 = It has not happened or it happened only
once or twice, and 1 = It has happened 23 times per month, once
a week or several times per week. Frequencies and percentage of
change for each item were calculated for the before- and afterintervention groups for the individual items. The nine original
items were combined into a composite variable and recoded into a
binary variable in keeping with Solberg and Olweus (2003) in order
to create a single measure of victimization.

Disclosing and reporting victimization


Four items from the student version of the Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire (1996) elicited information on disclosing to adults
and reporting of victimization to the school. Students who had
been bullied indicated if they told a teacher, told another adult at
school, told a parent or guardian, and if someone from home had
contacted the school about the bullying. The data from the rst
three items were dichotomous and were answered with a yes or a
no. The data from the fourth item were ordinal and included the
following three responses: (1) No, they have not contacted the
school; (2) Yes, they have contacted the school; and (3) Yes,
they have contacted the school more than once.
Responding to bullying
Students were asked two questions from the Olweus Bully/
Victim Questionnaire (1996) regarding how others at school
respond to bullying. Regarding adults, students were asked: How
much do teachers do to counteract bullying? with possible
responses being on a 15 Likert scale with 1 = Little or nothing,
and 5 = Much. Regarding their peers, students were asked to
respond to the following statement: When students are aware of
bullying, they take action, with possible responses being a 15
Likert scale with 1 = Never or almost never, and 5 = Always or
almost always.
Fear of being bullied
Students were asked one question from the Olweus Bully/
Victim Questionnaire (1996) regarding fear of being bullied.
Students responded to the following question: How often are you
afraid of being bullied at school? with possible responses being a
16 Likert scale with 1 = Never, and 6 = Very often.1
Empathy
Students were asked three questions from the Olweus Bully/
Victim Questionnaire (1996) which measured empathy for victims of
bullying. The rst question: Do you think you could join in bullying a
student whom you dislike? was answered using a 16 Likert scale
with 1 = Yes, and 6 = Denitely, no (see footnote 1). Two other
questions addressed empathy for victims of bullying: When you see
a student your age being bullied at school, what do you feel or think?
and How do you usually react if you see or understand that a student
your age is being bullied by other students? Both variables were
recoded into binary variables which reected negative/neutral or
positive affect towards the victims of bullying.
Student aggression
Staff members were asked to consider a list of 16 aggressive
behaviors and to answer the following question on a 15 point
Likert scale with 1 = Not at all and 5 = Very often, with regard
to each behavior: How often do you feel the following behaviors
happen among students at our school or at school sponsored
activities? These 16 behaviors were: hitting, kicking, punching,
pushing, intimidation, stalking, sexual harassment, damaging
property, theft, taunting/ridicule, name calling, threats, hostile
gesturing, spreading lies, gossiping, and shunning.
1
It should be noted that this particular question on the Olweus Bully/Victim
Questionnaire had six possible responses, thus the 16 point Likert scale.

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

87

Table 1
Percentages and percentage of change for self-reported student victimization and bullying, before and after the intervention.

. . . called mean names, made fun of, or teased in a hurtful way


. . . left out of things on purpose, excluded from groups of friends,
or completely ignored
. . . hit, kicked, pushed, and shoved around or locked indoors
. . . spread false rumors and tried to create dislike
. . . took money or other things or damaged belongings
. . . threatened or forced to do unwanted things
. . . mean names or comments made about race or color
. . . mean names, comments, or gestures made with a sexual meaning
. . . not mentioned previously

Victimization I was bullied in this


way. . .

Bullying I bullied in this way. . .

Pre survey
N = 874

Post survey
N = 817

Percentage
of change

Pre survey
N = 870

Post survey
N = 818

8.8%
5.9%

11.4%
6.7%

+29.5%
+13.7%

10.2%
7.1%

4.3%
3.5%

57.8%
50.7%

2.9%
7.2%
1.5%
3.1%
5.1%
5.3%
4.2%

3.4%
6.5%
1.5%
2.1%
5.1%
5.5%
2.8%

+17.2%
9.7%
0.0%
32.2%
.0%
+3.7%
33.3%

3.9%
3.9%
2.5%
2.8%
4.0%
3.8%
3.4%

1.7%
1.5%
.6%
1.1%
2.2%
2.1%
1.2%

56.4%
61.5%
76.0%
60.7%
45.0%
44.7%
64.7%

Knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors: staff perceptions


Staff members were asked eight questions regarding their
knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors with respect to bullying and
victimization within the school. These items were scored on a 15
Likert scale with 1 = Strongly disagree, and 5 = Strongly agree
(see Table 2 for a complete list of items).

Results
Victimization and bullying
Victimization
A calculation of frequencies comparing the before- and afterintervention groups for victimization indicated that some forms of
self-reported victimization increased and some decreased during
the time of the intervention (see Table 1). The Cronbach a for the
nine items was .900 so a composite variable was created and then
recoded to a binary variable. A chi-square test of independence was
performed to determine if there was a difference between the
before-intervention group and the after-intervention group. The
difference approached, but was not statistically signicant
(x2(1) = 2.83, p = .092) with a negligible effect size (.04),2 with
15.2% of students reporting victimization before the intervention
and 18.3% reporting victimization after. Although victimization
seems to have increased slightly the difference was not statistically
signicant, thus it can be concluded that self-reported victimization remained relatively stable during the time of the intervention.
Victimization by gender. A chi-square test of independence
comparing males before and after the intervention, and females
before and after the intervention was performed. The results
indicated that males reported more victimization after the
intervention (21.0% as compared to 15.9%) than before and that
the difference trended towards signicance (x2(1) = 2.40, p = .065).
There was no statistically signicant difference in self-reported
victimization for females after the intervention as compared to
before the intervention (p < .05).
Victimization by grade. Chi-square tests of independence comparing the before and after groups by grade level indicated a
statistically signicant increase in reporting of victimization for
ninth graders (x2(1) = 6.755, p = .009) with 26.0% reporting
victimization after as compared with 16.3% before. There were
no statistically signicant differences for tenth, eleventh or twelfth
2
Effect sizes for Cohens d (Cohen, 1988) used for chi-square test of
independence and independent samples t-test: r = .10 small, r = .30 medium,
r = .50 large.

Percentage of change

graders when comparing self-reports of victimization after the


intervention as compared to before the intervention (p < .05).
Bullying
The difference in bullying between the before- and afterintervention groups indicated that all forms of self-reported
bullying decreased during the time of the intervention by 4576%
(see Table 1). The Cronbach a for the nine items was .936 so a
composite variable was created and then recoded to a binary
variable. A chi-square test of independence was performed to
determine if there was a difference between the before-intervention group and the after-intervention group. Results showed a
statistically signicant difference between the before- and afterintervention groups (x2(1) = 17.75, p = .001) with 7.3% of students
reporting that they had bullied others after the intervention as
compared to 13.6% of students before, indicating that self-reported
bullying decreased during the time of the intervention although
the effect size was small (.10)2.
Bullying by gender and grade. A chi-square test of independence
comparing the before and after groups by gender indicated that the
trend of less self-reported bullying was found for both males and
females and was statistically signicant (p < .05). Chi-square tests
of independence comparing the before and after groups by grade
level also followed the same trend indicating less self-reported
bullying at each grade level. These differences were statistically
signicant for every grade (p < .05).
Disclosing victimization to adults and reporting victimization to the
school
Disclosure of victimization to adults was measured by three
items and reporting of victimization to the school by adults at
home was measured by a fourth item. Frequencies were calculated
and chi-square tests of independence were performed to determine statistical signicance for each item. In all cases there was an
increase in the percentages after the intervention when compared
with the before-intervention percentages. The percentage of
students saying they had told a teacher increased from 10.1% to
19.8%. This difference was statistically signicant (x2(1) = 3.98,
p = .046), but the effect size was small (.13)2, although it represents
a 51.0% percentage of change. The percentage of students telling
another adult at school increased from 21.2% to 23.2% which was
not statistically signicant (x2(1) = .120, p = .729). The percentage
of students reporting bullying to a parent or adult at home
increased from 40.3% to 45.1% but was not statistically signicant
(x2(1) = .49, p = .482). Parental notication of the school was
measured as contacting the school once, or contacting the school
more than once. Responses to these items increased following the
intervention, from 10.2% to 17.5% for the former and 4.8% to 8.8%

88

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

for the later. These differences were not statistically signicant


(x2(2) = 3.87, p = .144) although they represent a 58.8% and 54.4%
percentage of change respectively. Students slightly increased their
reporting of victimization to all adults, but the increase in reporting
to teachers was the only difference that was statistically signicant.
Beforeafter differences by gender. Chi-square tests of independence
were conducted to determine if there were differences with regards to
gender when comparing the after-intervention group to the beforeintervention group. The only variable which showed statistically
signicant differences for males was having an adult at home report to
the school (x2(2) = 8.56, p = .014) with 37.5% having reported once or
more after the intervention as compared to 11.9% before the
intervention. None of the variables indicated a statistically signicant
difference when comparing the females in the after-intervention
group to the before-intervention group (p < .05).
Beforeafter differences by grade. Chi-square tests of independence
were conducted to determine if there were differences with
regards to grade level when comparing the after-intervention
group to the before-intervention group. Among the four variables
there were no statistically signicant differences when comparing
ninth, tenth and twelfth grade students after the intervention to
before the intervention (p < .05). However, for eleventh grade
students 30.0% of students told a teacher about being bullied after
the intervention as compared to 3.0% before the intervention and
the difference was statistically signicant (x2(1) = 7.90, p = .009).
Responding to bullying: student perceptions
Teacher intervention in bullying
An independent samples t-test was performed and effect sizes
were calculated to determine if there was a difference in how students
viewed teachers efforts to intervene in bullying before and after the
intervention. The mean score of students after the intervention
(m = 2.38, sd = 1.185) was greater than before the intervention
(m = 2.26, sd = 1.193 and the difference was statistically signicant
(t(1670) = 2.112, p = .035) with a small effect size of .102 indicating
that students perceived teachers to respond more to bullying
problems after the intervention than they did before the intervention.
Beforeafter differences by gender. An independent samples t-test
was done to see if there was a difference for gender when
comparing the after-intervention group with the before-intervention group. No statistically signicant differences (p < .05) were
found for males or for females.
Beforeafter differences by grade. An independent samples t-test
was done to see if there were differences with regards to grade
level when comparing the after-intervention group with the
before-intervention group. No statistically signicant differences
were found for ninth, eleventh or twelfth graders (p < .05).
However, there was a statistically signicant difference
(t(413) = 2.098, p = .037) for tenth graders with more students
reporting that teachers increased their efforts to counteract
bullying following the intervention (m = 2.43, sd = 1.236) as
compared to before the intervention (m = 2.18, sd = 1.169).
Student intervention in bullying
An independent samples t-test was performed and effect sizes
were calculated to determine if there was a difference in how
students viewed their peers responses to bullying when they were
aware of it before and after the intervention. The mean score of
students after the intervention (m = 2.74, sd = 1.059) was greater
than before the intervention (m = 2.21, sd = 1.004) and the
difference was statistically signicant (t(1673) = 10.523,

p = .001) and produced a large effect size of .512 indicating that


students perceived a large increase in their peers responses to
bullying problems after the intervention as compared with before
the intervention.
Beforeafter differences by gender and grade level. An independent
samples t-test was done to see if there was a difference for gender
when comparing the after-intervention group with the beforeintervention group. The differences for both males and females were
statistically signicant (p < .05), mirroring the pattern of ndings for
the aggregated data. Likewise, an independent samples t-test
comparing the after-intervention group to the before-intervention
group by grade levels followed the same pattern of results with the
differences being statistically signicant (p < .05) at all grade levels.
Fear of being bullied
Because the data on fear of being bullied at school were highly
skewed, a MannWhitney U-test was performed to determine if
the difference after the intervention was statistically signicant
when compared to before the intervention. Prior to the intervention, students were signicantly (m place = 866.97) more likely to
fear being bullied than students following the intervention (m
place = 822.7), (U = 337,978.00, p = .031). Thus, fear of being
bullied decreased following the intervention, but with a very
small effect size (.05)3 that was statistically signicant.
Beforeafter differences by gender. A MannWhitney U-test was
done to determine if there were differences before the intervention
as compared to after the intervention by gender. Males were more
afraid of being bullied before the intervention than after, but the
difference was not statistically signicant (p < .05). Females were
also more afraid of being bullied before the intervention than after,
and the difference was statistically signicant (p = < .05).
Beforeafter differences by grade. A MannWhitney U-test was
done to determine if there were differences before the intervention
as compared to after the intervention by grade level. Ninth, tenth
and twelfth graders were less afraid of being bullied following the
intervention than before the intervention and the difference
approached or was statistically signicant (p < .05). However,
eleventh graders were slightly more afraid of being bullied after
the intervention as compared to before the intervention, but the
difference was not statistically signicant (p < .05).
Empathy for bullied students
Bullying a disliked student
An independent samples t-test was done to see if the beforeintervention group differed from the after-intervention group with
regards whether or not a student could join in bullying a disliked
student. The mean of the before-intervention group was lower
(m = 3.67, sd = 1.67) than the mean of the after-intervention group
(m = 3.90, sd = 1.63) indicating an increase in prosocial feelings
towards students who are disliked. The difference after the
intervention as compared to before the intervention was statistically
signicant (t(1687) = 2.952, p = .003) and the effect size was small
(.14)3.
Beforeafter differences for gender. An independent samples t-test
was done to determine if there were differences by gender. Males
indicated that they were less likely to join in bullying a student
whom they disliked after the intervention (m = 3.42, sd = 1.697)
than before the intervention (m = 3.16, sd = 1.659), and the
3
Effect size ranges for MannWhitney U (Rosenthal, 1991): <.09 trivial, .12.3
small (minor), .24.36 moderate, .371.0 large.

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

difference was statistically signicant (t(801) = -2.188, p = .029).


Females, on the other hand, who seemed to indicate both before
and after the intervention that they were not likely to bully a
student whom they disliked, were not different after the
intervention (m = 4.33, sd = 1.441) when compared with before
the intervention (m = 4.17, sd = 1.526) (t(869) = 1.603, p = .109),
although their mean scores before and after the intervention were
higher than males scores. This suggests that while females appear
to be more empathic than males overall, the intervention may have
had a particular effect on males and their empathic feelings for
victims, particularly with regards to bullying a disliked student.
Beforeafter differences for grade. An independent samples t-test
was done to determine if there were differences by grade level.
Ninth graders indicated that they were less likely to join in bullying
a student whom they disliked after the intervention than before
the intervention and the difference was statistically signicant
(p < .05). For the other three grade levels, students were less likely
to join in bullying a student whom they disliked after the
intervention, but the differences were not statistically signicant
(p < .05).
Thoughts and feelings towards victims
A chi-square test of independence was performed to determine
if thoughts and feelings about victims were different after the
intervention than before the intervention. Results indicated that
there was a statistically signicant difference (x2(1) = 27.86,
p = .001) and that the effect size was small (.13)2 with 56.2% of
students indicating empathy for victims after the intervention as
compared to 42.8% of students before the intervention.
Beforeafter differences for gender. A chi-square test of independence was performed to determine if thoughts and feelings about
victims were different after the intervention as compared to before
the intervention by gender. The difference for males was
statistically signicant (x2(1) = 6.573, p = .010) with 42.1% of
males feeling empathy for victims after the intervention as
compared to 32.9% before. The difference for females was also
statistically signicant (x2(1) = 21.505, p = .001) with 67.9% of
females feeling empathy for victims after the interventions as
compared to 52.0% before. Again, while females seem to be
generally more empathic than males, the results indicate that the
intervention may have had a particular effect on males with
regards to increasing their empathy for victims of bullying.
Beforeafter differences for grade. A chi-square test of independence was done to see if thoughts and feelings about victims were
different after the intervention as compared to before the
intervention by grade level. At all grade levels, the percentage of

89

students feeling empathy for victims increased. The differences


were statistically signicant for ninth and tenth graders, but not for
eleventh and twelfth graders (p < .05).
Reactions to observing victimization
A chi-square test of independence was performed to determine
if students reactions to observing bullying was different after the
intervention than before the intervention. Results indicated that
there was a statistically signicant difference (x2(1) = 9.85,
p = .002) and that the effect size was small (.08)3 with 87.2% of
students indicating empathy for victims after the intervention as
compared to 81.0% of students before the intervention.
Beforeafter differences for gender. A chi-square test of independence was performed to determine if reactions to observing
victimization were different after the intervention as compared to
before the intervention by gender. The difference for males was not
statistically signicant (x2(1) = 1.956, p = .162) with 42.1% of
males feeling empathy for victims after the intervention as
compared to 32.9% before. However, the difference for females
was statistically signicant (x2(1) = 8.820, p = .003) with 94.1% of
females feeling empathy for victims after the intervention as
compared to 87.7% before. As before, females more than males are
more likely to feel sympathy for victims of bullying, but in the case
of how they respond to observing victimization, females more than
males may have been affected by the intervention with regards to
this variable.
Beforeafter differences for grade. A chi-square test of independence was done to see if reactions to observing victimization were
different after the intervention as compared to before the
intervention by grade level. At all grade levels, the percentage of
students feeling empathy for victims increased. The differences
were statistically signicant for ninth and eleventh graders, but not
for tenth and twelfth graders (p < .05).
Student aggression: staff perceptions
Staff members considered 16 items that described aggressive
behaviors that students might engage in. Because the Cronbach a
for these 16 variables was .895, a composite variable was created.
An independent samples t-test indicated that there was a
statistically signicant difference (t(196) = 3.143, p = .002) in staff
perceptions of student aggression following the intervention. The
before-intervention mean of the composite variable was 2.55
(sd = .556) and the after-intervention mean of the composite
variable was 2.31 (sd = .495) with the effect size (.46)2 falling in the
moderate range, indicating that staff members saw a noteworthy
decrease in student aggression following the intervention as
compared to before the intervention.

Table 2
Staff percentages for pre- and post-survey responses strongly agree and agree, effect sizes and statistical signicance for knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors with regards to
bullying and victimization.

I am condent that I know what bullying is


I am familiar with the school policy on bullying in the handbook
I feel that our schools efforts to deal with bullying are adequate
I am condent in my ability to recognize bullying in school
I am condent in my ability to distinguish fun teasing from bullying
I am condent in my ability to distinguish normal conict
from bullying conict
I can condent in my ability to respond to bullying in school
When I am aware of bullying I take action to intervene
p < .05.

Pre-survey: strongly
agree + agree N = 120

Post-survey: strongly
agree + agree N = 78

Percentage
of change

Effect size

93.3%
81.7%
52.5%
85.8%
83.3%
85.8%

97.4%
91.1%
82.0%
91.0%
87.2%
91.0%

+4.3%
+11.5%
+56.1%
+6.0%
+4.6%
+6.0%

1.152
1.393
5.048*
1.295
1.606
1.956*

.10
.09
.35
.09
.11
.13

66.7%
86.7%

83.4%
94.8%

+25.0%
+13.4%

2.719*
2.081*

.19
.14

90

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

Knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors: staff perceptions


Eight items assessed staff members knowledge, beliefs and
behaviors with regards to bullying using a 15 Likert scale.
Responses for strongly agree and agree were combined and
percentage of change was calculated (see Table 2). Frequencies
indicated increases in knowledge about bullying, stronger antibullying beliefs, and greater ability and willingness to respond to
bullying behaviors after the intervention as compared to before the
intervention. The MannWhitney U-test was performed on each
variable to determine statistical signicance and to calculate effect
size (see Table 2).
Four of the items were statistically signicant and had minor to
moderate effect sizes. It appears that staff are quite condent in
their abilities to recognize and intervene in bullying as evidenced
by the high percentages of strongly agree plus agree on these
items in the follow-up survey. Staff members also showed small
but statistically signicant differences following the intervention
as compared to before the intervention in their abilities to
recognize and intervene in bullying problems. In particular it is
noteworthy that there was a statistically moderate difference
following the intervention in staff members feelings regarding the
adequacy of the schools response to bullying problems.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to assess the differences that may
have occurred as a result of a bullying intervention initiative at a
high school. In particular the study considered differences
following the intervention as compared to before the intervention
in amounts of bullying and victimization, student and family
disclosure or reporting of bullying, adult and student responses to
bullying, fear of being bullied at school, empathy for victims, staff
perceptions of student aggression, and staff members knowledge,
beliefs and behaviors with regards to bullying. Except for
victimization, which remained relatively stable, the differences
after the intervention as compared to before the intervention
indicate that self-reported bullying decreased, disclosure of
victimization increased, fear of bullying decreased, empathy for
victims increased, student aggression decreased, and staff increased knowledge, positively altered their beliefs, and positively
altered their behaviors with regards to responding to bullying.
Because the data did not reect a matched sample, it is difcult
to know if the self-reported stability of victimization is due to no
differences in victimization or if it is due to differences with
regards to awareness of bullying and victimization. It is possible
that the effect of the intervention was to cause some students who
had not viewed themselves as victims to realize that they had been
victimized and to report it on the post-survey when they might not
have reported it on the pre-survey. Likewise, some students who
thought themselves to be victims might have concluded as a result
of the intervention that they were not victims, although they had
reported that they were on the pre-survey. While differences of
this sort might result in a net effect that points to stabilized
victimization, it is feasible that there might have been an increase
or a decrease in victimization that is not accounted for in the data.
While it is discouraging that reductions in victimization by
bullying were not observed, it is quite encouraging that all forms of
self-reported bullying decreased in rather substantial amounts,
particularly when the percentage of decrease is considered.
Although this seems somewhat contradictory that victimization
might not change when bullying is reported to have decreased, it is
not uncommon to achieve ndings of this sort in bullying research,
particularly when victimization and bullying are measured
separately (Frey, Hirschstein, Snell, Edstrom, MacKenzie &
Broderick, 2005; Leadbeater, Hoglund, & Woods, 2003; Orpinas,
Horne, & Staniszeski, 2003; Pepler, Craig, Ziegler, & Charach, 1994).

The signicant reduction in self-reported bullying may have


occurred because of the heightened awareness that resulted from
the intervention and the recognition of students that their
previously reported behaviors were unacceptable. This new
awareness might have resulted in underreporting of behaviors
as opposed to an actual reduction in bullying in the post-survey. On
the other hand, the self-reported reduction in bullying may have
been due to the fact that a number of students who had previously
engaged in bullying realized that their behaviors were hurtful and
stopped these behaviors. Regardless, it seems that the ndings on
bullying and victimization suggest that there is perhaps a small
amount of chronic victimization which may be being perpetrated
by a small, rather intransigent group of bullies, and that the
intervention did not alter these dynamics to any large extent. If this
is the case, then the intervention may have had the greatest impact
on students who engage in milder forms of bullying or who support
the aggression of chronic bullies, resulting in an overall decrease of
these behaviors.
One of the differences which produced a rather noteworthy
effect size was in regards to student perceptions of their peers
actions with regards to intervening in bullying. Effect sizes in the
moderate range are rare in research of this sort (Merrell, Gueldner,
Ross, & Isava, 2008) and so it is quite encouraging that students
indicated that following the intervention they observed that their
peers were much more willing to intervene in situations that they
recognized as bullying.
While the data on victimization and bullying were self-reports
from students and may be subject to bias, the data from staff
members with regards to student aggression would not reect this
bias. Although not all aggression is bullying, staff perceptions of
how students interact with each other does reect to some degree
the social climate of the school which is affected by all aggression
including bullying. Staff perceptions of students indicated that
there was less aggression following the intervention and that the
differences produced noteworthy effect sizes. The fact that staff
members observed less student aggression following the intervention supports the possibility that the reduction in self-reported
student bullying may have been an actual reduction as opposed to
one that occurred because of new awareness about the social
unacceptability of bullying behaviors. Again, it should be emphasized that bullying interventions seldom produce effect sizes of
even moderate magnitude, so it is encouraging that staff members
perceived a reduction in student aggression that was in the
moderate range.
Eliminating or reducing bullying is dependent on changing
adults as well as students, so this study sought to collect data on
staff differences following the intervention as compared to before
the intervention. It is noteworthy that staff members beliefs that
the schools efforts to deal with bullying were adequate, increased
signicantly and reected a minor to moderate effect size during
the time of the intervention. Likewise, the small but signicant
increase in staff members commitment to intervening in bullying
situations indicates that staff members have changed their
behaviors regarding how they address problems around bullying.
It is possible that these changes may have contributed to the
reported reduction in bullying and the observed decrease in
student aggression.
One of the challenges in bringing about change with respect to
this intervention is the fact that Meliora Public Academy is a very
safe school. This is reected in the low levels of physical bullying
and aggression reported by both students and staff, before as well
as after the intervention. Physical altercations on school property
average one or two over the course of a school year. Study data
indicate that the bullying and aggression that occurs tends to be of
a non-physical or social nature, and that when bullying takes place,
it is more covert and indirect. Throughout the data there is often

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

little difference after the intervention as compared to before the


intervention because a high level of prosocial behavior and a low
level of antisocial behavior existed among students before the
intervention. This is not to suggest that bullying did not exist
before the intervention, or that it has subsequently been
eradicated. The ndings of a small amount of stable victimization
would preclude that position. However, given the absence of
confounding variables which cannot be ruled out because of the
study design, the results suggest that the intervention may have
contributed to some of the positive outcomes observed.

91

comparison group to ascertain causality. Additionally, it would be


benecial to replicate and evaluate this intervention in high
schools that are more representative of schools and communities
across the country.
Conclusion
The results of this evaluation suggest that bullying continues to
be an intervention issue into the high school years and that efforts
to counteract bullying at this level can be affected by designing and
implementing a school-wide system at this level.

Limitations
References
This study was limited by its before- and after-intervention
design. The fact that there was no control or comparison group
does not allow for conclusions regarding causality. Thus it cannot
be determined if the intervention produced the effects that were
observed or whether they were the result of other confounding
factors.
Another limitation of this study has to do with the fact that the
data were not repeated measures, thus reducing the statistical
power of the analysis and making interpretation of effect sizes
challenging. Additionally, due to some highly skewed data, this
study makes use of nonparametric analyses. Such analyses are not
as powerful as parametric analyses reducing the certainty with
which conclusions may be drawn.
One limitation of this study is that the intervention had only
been in place for two years when the post-intervention data were
collected. A longer pre- to post-assessment timeframe would have
given the program a longer time in which to make the types of
cultural and systemic changes that are needed for programs of this
sort to become rmly established. Ideally, a four year time span
would have been preferable because that would have allowed for
inclusion of students who came to the school as ninth graders
during the rst year of implementation with follow-up data being
collected during their last year (twelfth grade) at the school. This
group of students would have had a longer and stronger dosage
than the students who have been assessed for the current
evaluation, increasing the possibility that greater effects might
have resulted.
A further limitation of this study is that the second administration of the staff survey yielded 35% fewer completed surveys than
the rst administration. Attempts to include more staff members
were unsuccessful, so whereas the rst survey included approximately 78.0% of staff members, the second survey only reected
approximately 5.0% of the staff members. This may have affected
the reliability of the data.
Lastly, a nal limitation of this study is that the school where it
was conducted is not representative of high schools across the
United States. Meliora Public Academy resides in a wealthy
community, and the school itself is regarded as exemplary
because of its high academic standards and rigor. Thus,
generalizing these ndings to schools that do not t this prole
has its limitations.
Future directions
Bullying tends to be framed as a problem that diminishes as
students move into high school. The present study indicates that if
bullying does decrease in amount, it does not completely go away,
and in fact, this study conrms that bullying still exists at the high
school level. For this reason researchers should expand their
exploration of bullying to include the high school environment.
This study points to the possible effectiveness of a whole-school
bullying intervention at the high school level. Future research
could benet from a study design that includes a control or

Allen, K. P. (2009). Dealing with bullying and conict through a collaborative intervention process: The social and emotional learning intervention team. School Social
Work Journal, 33(2), 7085.
Allen, K. P. (2010). A bullying intervention system: Reducing risk and creating support
for aggressive students. Preventing School Failure, 54(3), 199209.
Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the classroom. The Journal
of Educational Research, 92(2), 8699.
Baldry, A. C., & Farrington, D. P. (2007). Effectiveness of programs to prevent bullying.
Victims and Offenders, 2(2), 183204.
Boulton, M. J., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Bully/victim problems in middle school children.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 315329.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature
and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Chapell, M. S., Hasselman, S. L., Kitchin, T., Lomon, S. N., MacIver, K. W., & Sarullo, P. L.
(2006). Bullying in elementary school, high school, and college. Adolescence,
41(164), 633648.
Chen, H. T., & Rossi, P. H. (1983). Evaluating with sense: The theory-driven approach.
Evaluation Review, 7(3), 283302.
Chen, H. T., & Rossi, P. H. (Eds.). (1992). Using theory to improve program and policy
evaluations. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Taradash, A. (2000). Dating experiences of bullies in
early adolescence. Child Maltreatment, 5(4), 299310.
Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (1997). Observations of bullying and victimization in the
school yard. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 13(2), 4160.
Donaldson, S. I. (2007). Program theory-driven evaluation science: Strategies and applications. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Donaldson, S. I., & Gooler, L. E. (2003). Theory-driven evaluation in action: Lessons from
a $20 million statewide Work and Health Initiative. Evaluation and Program
Planning, 26, 355366.
Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Snell, J. L., Edstrom, L. V. S., MacKenzie, E. P., & Broderick, C.
J. (2005). Reducing playground bullying and supporting beliefs: An experimental
trial of the steps to respect program. Developmental Psychology, 41(3), 479491.
Gruber, J. E., & Fineran, S. (2008). Comparing the impact of bullying and sexual
harassment victimization on the mental and physical health of adolescents. Sex
Roles, 59, 113.
Kaukiainen, A., Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Tamminen, M., Vauras, M., Maki, H., et al.
(2002). Learning difculties, social intelligence, and self-concept: Connections to
bully-victim problems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 269278.
Kim, Y. S., & Leventhal, B. (2008). Bullying and suicide: A review. International Journal of
Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20(2), 133154.
Klomek, A. B., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., Schonfeld, I. S., & Gould, M. S. (2007). Bullying,
depression, and suicidality in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 4049.
Kumpulainen, K. (2008). Psychiatric conditions associated with bullying. International
Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20(2), 121132.
Leadbeater, B., Hoglund, W., & Woods, T. (2003). Changing contexts? The effects of a
primary prevention program on classroom levels of peer-relational and physical
victimization. Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 397418.
Linder, J. R., Crick, N. R., & Collins, W. A. (2002). Relational aggression and victimization
in young adults romantic relationships: Associations with perceptions of parent,
peer, and romantic relationship quality. Social Development, 11(1), 6986.
Madsen, K. C. (1996). Differing perceptions of bullying and their practical implications.
Educational and Child Psychology, 13(2), 1422.
Merrell, K. W., Gueldner, B. A., Ross, S. W., & Isava, D. M. (2008). How effective are
school bullying intervention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention research.
School Psychology Quarterly, 23(1), 2642.
Monks, C. P., & Smith, P. K. (2006). Denitions of bullying: Age differences in understanding of the term, and the role of experience. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 24, 801821.
Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J., Simon-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001).
Bullying behaviors among U.S. youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(16), 20942100.
OConnell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and
challenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence 22 473-452.
Ofce of Educational Research & Improvements (2010). National Center for Education
Statistics. Core of Common Data. Retrieved from: National Center for Education

92

K.P. Allen / Studies in Educational Evaluation 36 (2010) 8392

Statistics, Ofce of Educational Research & Improvement: http://nces.ed.gov/ccd/


schoolsearch/school_detail.asp?Search+1&SchoolID=3623160032 41
Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools: Bullies and whipping boys. Washington, DC:
Hemisphere Publishing.
Olweus, D. (1991). Bully/victim problems among schoolchildren: Basic facts and effects
of a school based intervention program. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The
development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 411448). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Cambridge,
MA: Blackwell.
Olweus, D. (1996). Revised Olweus bully/victim questionnaire. Bergen, Norway: Research
Center for Health Promotion, University of Bergen.
Orpinas, P., Horne, A. M., & Staniszewski, D. (2003). School bullying: Changing the
problem by changing the school. School Psychology Review, 32(3), 431444.
Pepler, D. J., Craig, W. M., Ziegler, S., & Charach, A. (1994). An evaluation of an antibullying intervention in Toronto schools. Canadian Journal of Community Mental
Health, 13(2), 95110.
Pepler, D., Jiang, D., Craig, W., & Connolly, J. (2008). Developmental trajectories of
bullying and associated factors. Child Development, 79(2), 325338.
Porhola, M., Karhunen, S., & Rainivaara, S. (2006). Bullying at school and in the
workplace: A challenge for communication research. In C. S. Beck (Ed.), Communication Yearbook 30 (pp. 249301). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rahey, L., & Craig, W. M. (2002). Evaluation of an ecological program to reduce bullying
in schools. Canadian Journal of Counselling, 36(4), 281296.
Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1991). Bullying among Australian school children: Reported
behavior and attitudes towards victims. The Journal of Social Psychology, 131(5),
615627.
Rosenthal, A. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (revised). Newbury
Park, CA: Sage.
Salmivalli, C., & Kaukiainen, A. (2004). Female aggression revisited: Variable- and
person-centered approaches to studying gender differences in different types of
aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 158163.
Salmivalli, C., & Nieminen, E. (2002). Proactive and reactive aggression among school
bullies, victims, and bully-victims. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 3044.
Smith, P. K. (2010). Bullying in primary and secondary schools: Psychological and
organizational comparisons. In S. R. Jimerson, S. M. Swearer, & D. L. Espelage (Eds.),
Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective (pp. 137150). New
York: Routledge.

Smith, P. K., Madsen, K. C., & Moody, J. C. (1999). What causes the age decline in reports
of being bullied at school? Towards a developmental analysis of risks of being
bullied. Educational Research, 41(3), 267285.
Smith, P. K., Ananiadou, K., & Cowie, H. (2003). Interventions to reduce school bullying.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48(9), 591599.
Smith, J. D., Schneider, B. H., Smith, P. K., & Ananiadou, K. (2004). The effectiveness of
whole-school anti-bullying programs: A synthesis of evaluation research. School
Psychology Review, 33(4), 547560.
Solberg, M. E., & Olweus, D. (2003). Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the
Olweus bully/victim questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 239268.
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Social cognition and bullying: Social
inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
17, 435450.
Swearer, S. M., & Espelage, D. L. (2004). Introduction: A social-ecological framework of
bullying among youth. In D. L. Espelage & S. M. Swearer (Eds.), Bullying in schools: A
social-ecological perspective on prevention and intervention (pp. 112). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Swearer, S. M., Espelage, D. L., Vaillancourt, R., & Hymel, S. (2010). What can be done
about school bullying?: Linking research to educational practice. Educational
Researcher, 39(1), 3847.
Tto, M. M., Farrington, D. P., & Baldry, A. C. (2008). Effectiveness of programmes to
reduce school bullying. Stockholm: Swedish Council for Crime Prevention, Information, and Publications.
Vossekuil, B., Fein, R., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002). The nal report and
ndings of the safe school initiative: Implications for the prevention of school attacks in
the United States. Washington, DC: United States Secret Service and United States
Department of Education.
Vreeman, R. C., & Carroll, A. E. (2007). A systematic review of school-based interventions to prevent bullying. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 161, 7888.
Whitney, I., & Smith, P. K. (1993). A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in junior/
middle and secondary schools. Educational Research, 35, 325.
Yoon, J. S., Barton, E., & Taiariol, J. (2004). Relational aggression in middle school:
Educational implications of developmental research. Journal of Early Adolescence,
24(3), 303318.
Kathleen P. Allen is a doctoral candidate in the Human Development Program at the
University of Rochester, Margaret Warner Graduate School of Education and Human
Development. Her area of interest is bullying in schools.

Potrebbero piacerti anche