Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Reading

collection adapted from


Drones : The buzz of something new


THIS year, some predict, will be the year of the microdrone. Small, pilotless aircraftmost of them
helicopters with four or more sets of rotors and a payload slung between themare moving out of the
laboratory and into practical use. They are already employed for aerial photography and surveillance,
particularly in Europe. In Paris, earlier this month, drones flying around the Eiffel tower caused a security
scare. And in America, on March 19th, Amazon, a retailer, was given permission to test a drone designed to
deliver its goods.
These drones, though, rely on an operator on the ground. Indeed, this is often a legal requirement. But it is
also a constraint. If a world of microdrones really is to come about, then the craft will need to be able to
cut the surly bonds of Earth and fly unsupervised. For that, they are going to have to get a lot more
intelligent.
The problem is not navigation. The Global Positioning System and Google Earth can tell a drone where it is
and what large, permanent obstacles it might encounter, and it can be programmed with its course before
it lifts off. The problem, rather, is the unexpected: an unwary bird; an unmapped tree; a gust of wind. Part
of making drones able to fly by themselves will be to give them the senses they need to deal with such
hazards.

One approach is to ask how natural drones do it. The word, after all, referred originally to a male bee, and
bees and other insects rarely blunder into things or fall out of the sky. Copying their tricks makes sense.
And laboratories around the world, using bees, blowflies and hawk moths as their models, are trying to do
just that.
Joining the drones club
Ashutosh Natrajs idea is to give his drones vision. Dr Natraj, who works at Oxford University, drew his
inspiration from a bee he saw buzzing around his house one day. He wondered how the animal avoided the
many hazards a human dwelling presents.
The answer, he found after a few days perusing the apidological literature, is fairly straightforward, at least
in principle. Bees rely on optic flow. This is the perception, familiar to anyone who has looked out of a train
window, that nearby things are moving faster than distant ones.
To build optic-flow perception into a drone, Dr Natraj had first to fit it with an eye and a brain. The eye is a
video camera that weighs a mere 8g. This sends a stream of images, at a rate of 25 a second, to the brain.
At the moment this is a computer on the ground that is linked to the camera by Wi-Fi. Dr Natraj, though,
plans to replace it by a Raspberry Pia device the size of a credit cardon board the drone itself.
The computer, whether terrestrial or airborne, extracts from the incoming images features salient to optic
flow. In particular, it identifies objects edges and tracks them from frame to frame. This way, it can work
out how quickly the drone is approaching something and, if a collision is likely, how the drones path needs
to shift to avert it. It then uses this information to change the pitch of the rotors.
That sounds easy in principle, but collision-avoidance, especially when what is to be avoided is moving as
well, requires good manoeuvring skills. This is where the flies and the moths come in. Adjusted for size,
blowflies are better at manoeuvring than any fighter aircraft yet built. Hawk moths are superb at hovering.
Both insects use the same method: they combine vision with an inertial guidance system.
Inertial guidance relies on measuring the position of something that, because of its inertia, resists following
the object it is part of. Man-made systems use gyroscopes. Moths use their antennae. Flies use a pair of
tiny organs called halteres that have evolved from the animals hind wings and are shaped like balls on
sticks.
Several groups of researchers are looking into insect inertial guidance. Those studying blowflies are based
in London. Those studying moths are based in Baltimore. The London group, led by Holger Krapp of
Imperial College, has used micro-electrodes to follow the insects nerve impulses, and high-speed
photography and computed tomography (an advanced form of X-raying) to follow the movements of their

external body parts and their muscles. That, with the addition of a bit of computer modelling, has shown
them how dipteran inertial guidance works.
Flies do it using input from hundredspossibly thousandsof sensors. These are the elements of their
compound eyes, and also the many cells at the bases of their halteres. The signals from these, it turns out,
do not have to pass through the brain to be processed. Instead, they act as a series of reflexes controlling
the insects speed, attitude and heading directly. That is the opposite of most approaches to engineering
drone avionics. But it suggests that true drone manoeuvrability might be better created without trying to
imitate the functions of a brain. Dr Krapps colleague Mirko Kovac is now attempting to do this.
Hovercraft
One way manoeuvrability might be engineered into a drones airframe is shown by the work on hawk
moths. These insects, when hovering over flowers to drink nectar from them, employ a similar control
system to fliesthough in this case information from their antennae substitutes for that which flies get
from their halteres. Hawk moths are being studied independently by two groups at Johns Hopkins
Universityone led by Rajat Mittal and the other by Noah Cowan. They have found that the moths hold
their heads and thoraxes steady with respect to a flower by making minute changes to the orientation of
their abdomens.
Dr Cowan, indeed, has gone further than mere analysis. He has used knowledge garnered about how
moths hover to fit a drone with the equivalent of an abdomen. The drones battery pack hangs beneath it,
and is fitted with servo motors that adjust its position in the way that a moth moves its abdomen. That
stabilises the drone in mid air.
At Harvard, meanwhile, Robert Wood has taken a different approach to the problem of hovering. Though
referred to as microdrones, quadcopters and their kin are usually tens of centimetres across. Dr Woods
drones really are micro. They measure 3cm from wingtip to wingtip. Moreover, their wings flap like those
of real insects, rather than rotating.
Dr Wood has built simple eyes into his drones, and these act like occelli, which are small eye spots that
insects use to take bearings on the sun or the moon, so that they can fly at a constant angle to these
distant light sources and thus maintain a straight course. (Confusion of the ocelli is thought to be the
reason moths circle bright artificial lights at night.)
Dr Woods artificial eyes are pyramid-shaped and have a photosensor on each face. They are thus able, like
real ocelli, to track the sun. Dr Wood has not yet translated that ability into an on-board navigation system,
but it should not be too hard to do soso long, of course, as his drones do not come across any candles.

a) Vocabulary Work :
For each of the following either explain the following in your own words or give a synonym
or write a sentence using the word in context

1. Slung
2. scare
3. to come about
4. surly bonds
5. unwary
6. gust
7. blunder
8. buzzing
9. dwelling
10. straightforward
11. mere
12. salient
13. shift
14. moths
15. hovering
16. hind wings
17. Flies
18. hold something steady
19. garnered
20. kin
21. take bearings
22. track
23. candles

b) Written Work : Summary


Write a summary of this article and provide your reaction or connect the issue to a larger,
relevant context.

















c) Listening Work : MP3


Listen to the article using the MP3 provided and read at the same time.
Try to pronounce the vocabulary as you read.

Potrebbero piacerti anche