Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

6020 The Journal of Neuroscience, April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027

Behavioral/Cognitive

Emotion Regulation and Trait Anxiety Are Predicted by the


Microstructure of Fibers between Amygdala and Prefrontal
Cortex
Annuschka Salima Eden,1 Jan Schreiber,4 X Alfred Anwander,4 X Katharina Keuper,1,3 Inga Laeger,2 Peter Zwanzger,2
Pienie Zwitserlood,5 X Harald Kugel,6 and Christian Dobel1,3
1

Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis and 2Department of Psychiatry, University Hospital, 48149 Munster, Germany, 3Otto Creutzfeldt Center
for Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience, 48149 Munster, Germany, 4Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig,
Germany, 5Westfalische Wilhelms-Universitat, 48149 Munster, Germany, and 6Center of Radiology, Faculty of Medicine, 48149 Munster, Germany

Diffusion tensor imaging revealed that trait anxiety predicts the microstructural properties of a prespecified fiber tract between the
amygdala and the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex. Besides this particular pathway, it is likely that other pathways are also affected.
We investigated white matter differences in persons featuring an anxious or a nonanxious personality, taking into account all potential
pathway connections between amygdala and anxiety-related regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Diffusion-weighted images, measures
of trait anxiety and of reappraisal use (an effective emotion-regulation style), were collected in 48 females. With probabilistic tractography, pathways between the amygdala and the dorsolateral PFC, dorsomedial PFC, ventromedial PFC, and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) were
delineated. The resulting network showed a direct ventral connection between amygdala and PFC and a second limbic connection
following the fornix and the anterior limb of the internal capsule. Reappraisal use predicted the microstructure of pathways to all
calculated PFC regions in the left hemisphere, indicating stronger pathways for persons with high reappraisal use. Trait anxiety predicted
the microstructure in pathways to the ventromedial PFC and OFC, indexing weaker connections in trait-anxious persons. These effects
appeared in the right hemisphere, supporting lateralization and top-down inhibition theories of emotion processing. Whereas a specific
microstructure is associated with an anxious personality, a different structure subserves emotion regulation. Both are part of a broad
fiber tract network between amygdala and PFC.
Key words: amygdala; DTI; PFC; reappraisal use; top-down inhibition; trait anxiety

Introduction
The interplay between amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) is
central to the regulation of emotions. Crucial components of this
interplay are top-down inhibition processes manifesting themselves in downregulation of the amygdala by medial PFC (mPFC;
Bishop, 2007). A greater functional connectivity between the
amygdala and the mPFC was reported in persons with low anxiety (Pezawas et al., 2005). Investigating fiber connections in white
matter, Kim and Whalen (2009) applied diffusion tensor imaging
(DTI) and found that trait anxiety, a stable tendency to respond
with anxiety, predicted the microstructure of a ventral amygdala
PFC pathway. Persons high in trait anxiety showed lower values
of fractional anisotropy (FA; Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996) on this

Received July 26, 2014; revised Jan. 31, 2015; accepted Feb. 3, 2015.
Author contributions: A.S.E., K.K., I.L., Pe.Z., Pi.Z., and C.D. designed research; A.S.E. and H.K. performed research;
J.S., A.A., H.K., and C.D. contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; A.S.E., J.S., and A.A. analyzed data; A.S.E.,
J.S., A.A., K.K., I.L., Pe.Z., Pi.Z., H.K., and C.D. wrote the paper.
This work was supported by the Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research (Grant Do3/021/10).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Correspondence should be addressed to Annuschka Salima Eden, Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis, Malmedyweg 15, 48143 Munster, Germany. E-mail: Annuschka.Eden@uni-muenster.de.
DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3659-14.2015
Copyright 2015 the authors 0270-6474/15/356020-08$15.00/0

tract to the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex, i.e., weaker connections. It is likely that a larger network of amygdalaPFC connections is affected in trait anxiety (for review, see Ray and Zald,
2012). Data by Kim and Whalen (2009) support the idea that
anxiety is associated with ineffective top-down inhibition. These
dysfunctional processes might be attributable to an attenuated
use of reappraisal, a coping strategy involving reinterpretation of
the emotional meaning of stimuli. Low reappraisal results in low
activity in PFC regions and in high amygdala activity (Ochsner et
al., 2002, 2004; Kim and Hamann, 2007). In women, the microstructural foundation for this interplay is related to the integrity
of white matter pathways between amygdala and PFC (Zuurbier
et al., 2013).
Anatomical knowledge about amygdalaPFC connections
mostly derives from primates. Because of close cytoarchitectural
homology between primates and humans (Petrides and Mackey,
2006; Jbabdi et al., 2013), a transfer of findings seems reasonable
but awaits further support. The mPFC and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) receive direct input from the amygdala (for an overview, see Barbas and Zikopoulos, 2006), whereas the lateral PFC
receives less, and mostly indirect, input via the cingulate or posterior OFC (Ray and Zald, 2012). Studies investigating amygdalaPFC connections in relation to anxiety have mostly focused on

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits

J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027 6021

Table 1. Means and SDs of demographic, behavioral, and fractional anisotropy (in target regions) data, separately for both anxiety groups
FA
Trait anxiety group Trait anxiety score Reappraisal score Age

Years of schooling IQ

Low

27.08 2.43

27.75 6.03

25.38 3.67 13 0

High

57.58 4.65

23.04 6.52

26.92 5.99 13 0

OFC

115.17 13.06 L: 0.533 0.017


R: 0.541 0.017
107.00 25.11 L: 0.522 0.030
R: 0.527 0.017

vmPFC

dmPFC

dlPFC

L: 0.544 0.015
R: 0.541 0.014
L: 0.488 0.152
R: 0.531 0.013

L: 0.553 0.015
R: 0.554 0.012
L: 0.505 0.157
R: 0.519 0.112

L: 0.545 0.019
R: 0.541 0.012
L: 0.471 0.183
R: 0.508 0.111

L, Left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere.

specific and direct connections, such as to the insula or via the


uncinate fasciculus. They neglected indirect routes via long association fibers (Modi et al., 2013; Baur et al., 2013a,b).
Our aim was to investigate white matter alterations in persons
with high and low trait anxiety, along multiple pathways connecting amygdala and PFC. We collected diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance images and applied probabilistic tractography
(Behrens et al., 2007) to reconstruct all potential pathways between the amygdala and PFC regions associated with anxietyrelated processes. A rather large body of literature established an
association of anxiety with right-hemispheric activity (Davidson
et al., 1990), and thus we hypothesized trait anxiety to be associated with weaker right-hemispheric connections. Mechanisms of
reappraisal are less investigated, but there is some evidence for
left-hemispheric lateralization of reappraisal (Ochsner et al.,
2002) as well as higher metabolic activity in left frontal regions in
reappraisal users (Kim et al., 2012). Consequently, reappraisal
was expected to be associated with stronger left-hemispheric connections brought forth by intensive use.

Materials and Methods


Subjects
Three hundred ten nonclinical volunteers completed an on-line version
of the Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1983). On
the basis of individual trait scores, right-handed (according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971) female participants were
selected if they scored 30 or 50. In a next step, they were matched for
age. This established two groups of participants (n 24 each), one with
low anxiety [LA; mean trait score, 27.08 (SD, 2.43); mean age, 25.38 (SD,
3.679)] and one with high anxiety [HA; mean trait score, 27.08 (SD,
2.43); mean age, 25.38 (SD, 3.67)], that did not differ for age (df 46,
T 1.414, p 0.166) nor years of schooling. All group members were
medically healthy and had no history of psychiatric medication or mental
disorders, as verified by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (version 5.0.0) of the Diagnostic Statistical ManualIV (Ackenheil
et al., 1999). The participants that were excluded from the two groups did
not fulfill one or more of the required inclusion criteria. All procedures
rztekammer Westfalenwere cleared by the ethical review board of the A
Lippe, and subjects gave informed consent for their participation.

Procedure
To control for intelligence differences between groups, the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale (German version of the WAIS-III; Von Aster et
al., 2006) was administered individually to every subject 2 d before image
acquisition. No significant differences were found between the HA and
LA groups (HA: mean, 107.00; SD, 25.11; LA: mean, 115.17; SD, 13.06;
T 1.414, p 0.164). Participants completed the German version of the
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross and John, 2003), which measures reappraisal use. The two groups displayed significant differences in
reappraisal use (HA: mean, 23.04; SD, 6.52; LA: mean, 27.75; SD, 6.03;
T 2.598, p 0.013; see Table 1 for means separated by group).

Image acquisition
Participants were instructed to lie still and stay awake throughout the
entire scanning procedure. Diffusion MRI (dMRI) and structural T1weighted images were acquired on a 3T MR scanner (Intera 3.0T; Philips
Medical Systems), with an inner bore diameter of 60 cm, equipped with

Quasar-Dual Gradients with two modes, either 40 mT/m gradient


strength and 200 mT/m/ms slew rate for general imaging or 80 mT/m
gradient strength and 100 mT/m/ms slew rate for diffusion imaging.
Diffusion MRI. High-angular resolution dMRI data were acquired using a six-channel head coil with a single-shot spin echo EPI sequence (TE,
55 ms; TR, 15,623 ms; image matrix, 128 128; FOV, 240 240 mm 2),
providing 60 diffusion-encoding gradient directions with a b value of
1000 s/mm 2 and a single measurement without a diffusion-weighting
gradient (b 0 s/mm 2). Seventy-eight axial slices with 1.88 mm thickness were acquired in an interleaved manner, covering the whole brain.
This resulted in cubic voxels of 1.88 mm edge length. Parallel image
acquisition was applied using SENSE with an acceleration factor of 2 and
a scan duration of 20 min.
Anatomical MRI. The T1-weighted images were acquired with a 3D
nonequilibrium gradient echo sequence (turbo field echo) with waterselective excitation. Contrast preparation of magnetization consisted of a
nonselective inversion pulse every 1020 ms. Imaging parameters were as
follows: TR, 9.3 ms; final TE, 4.4 ms; pulse angle, 9; FOV, 300 239
234 mm 3 (foot-head anterior-posterior right-left); cubic voxels of 1.17
mm edge length; SENSE acceleration factor 2 scan duration, 8.43 min.

dMRI preprocessing
Before data processing, dMRI volumes that were corrupted by movement of the participants were removed from the datasets. First, an automatic method was used to remove volumes of low quality. The algorithm
is based on the fact that motion eliminates the signal in a slice when
motion occurs during its acquisition. Usually, the average voxel intensity
of two consecutive slices does not change much. Only when motion
extinguishes the signal in parts of a slice does its average voxel intensity
differ greatly from its neighbors and indicate corruption of the volume.
In a following control step, visual inspection of the datasets ensured the
satisfactory quality of the remaining data. The cleaned dMRIs were interpolated to 1 mm isotropic resolution, aligned with the MNI template,
and corrected for motion and eddy-current effects in one step with a
single interpolation. The diffusion tensor and the FA maps were computed by use of the FSL software package (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.
uk/fsl/fsl-4.1.9; Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009; Jenkinson et al.,
2012).

Regions for connectivity analysis


Based on the literature, four regions in the PFC were defined: the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC; Buckholtz et al., 2008), the dorsomedial PFC
(dmPFC; Kim et al., 2011), the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC; Stein et al.,
2007), and the OFC (Zald and Kim, 1996a,b; Rauch et al., 1997; Sladky et
al., 2012). Since the precise anatomical locations and boundaries of these
areas have not yet been defined beyond controversy (Roy et al., 2012),
there was no suitable ready-to-use atlas that comprised all four PFC areas
for our purposes. Thus, we manually created an atlas that included the
dmPFC, dlPFC, OFC, and vmPFC based on the atlas introduced by Oishi
(type II WMPM; Oishi et al., 2009). The ventral and dorsal regions were
separated by an axial cutting plane at z 5 (MNI). The posterior end of
the ROIs was defined at y 0 (MNI). This atlas was morphed on the
individual brains using nonlinear registration (Avants et al., 2008) obtained from a registration of the FA template (FMRIB58_FA_1 mm supplied with FSL) onto the individual FA map. Figure 1 illustrates the
defined PFC areas applied in this study. The amygdala regions were
obtained by registration of the combined population maps of the
amygdala parcellation (Solano-Castiella et al., 2010) to each individual

6022 J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits

FA map, and they were thresholded at a value


of 3. The average volumes of the left and right
amygdala were 1478 and 1586 mm 3,
respectively.

Analysis of white matter microstructure


Probabilistic tractography based on the balland-stick model (Behrens et al., 2007) that is
implemented in the FSL software package was
used to create the connectivity maps between
amygdala and the PFC regions. These connectivity maps indicate for every voxel by how
many probabilistic tracks it has been crossed.
One thousand tracks were started in every
voxel of the amygdala, but only those that
reached one of the prefrontal regions were considered. Starting a fixed number of tracks in Figure 1. A, Illustration of the manually created atlas applied in this study. B, Axial slice of one subject with one exemplary
every voxel of the amygdala masks leads to a probabilistic track that connects the amygdala with the prefrontal cortex. Voxels selected for the analysis (red) are located within
different number of probabilistic tracks in ev- the probabilistic track (green) and the individual skeleton and have FA values higher than 0.4.
ery subject, because of different sizes of indiwe expected the factor trait anxiety to explain most of the variance.
vidual amygdalae. To compensate for this effect, all voxel values in the
Hence, trait anxiety was the first to enter this regression, followed by
connectivity maps were normalized by dividing them by the volume of
reappraisal use and, finally, IQ. As above, we also tested the alternative
the individual amygdala. Tracks were only seeded in the amygdala and
model with reappraisal use as the first factor.
not in the PFC regions, as pilot testing revealed that only a very small
fraction of the tracks seeded in the PFC areas reached the amygdala so
that the statistical power of this testing was not sufficient.
Results
To evaluate differences in white matter coherence along the connectAfter an anatomical description of the fiber pathway connections
ing pathways, values of FA were compared between the groups. This was
between seed and target regions, the results of the statistical analachieved by a region-based analysis method (Snook et al., 2007; Faria et
yses of the FA values of these pathways will be reported.
al., 2010) that considered only voxels on the center line of the white
matter within the pathways defined by the connectivity maps. The central
Figure 2 shows the calculated pathways between the amygdala
line was defined by computing the skeleton of each individuals FA map
and the four target regions of the prefrontal cortex: the OFC, the
as implemented in the tract-based spatial statistics software package
vmPFC, the dmPFC, and the dlPFC, (see Table 1 for FA means
(TBSS, http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/TBSS; Smith et al., 2006), takseparated by group). The calculated pathways of both hemiing into account only voxels with an FA value higher than 0.4 (Fig. 1B,
spheres did not show qualitative differences, so that the connecgreen). To define the target-specific ROI, the connectivity maps were
tions are exemplarily shown in the right hemisphere. The ventral
thresholded at a specific level for every target region. These levels were
PFC regions (Fig. 2 A, B) are connected with the amygdala via a
computed separately by averaging the 90% quantiles of all individual
direct ventral pathway following the uncinated fascicle. Addinormalized connectivity maps. The FA values on the skeleton within this
tionally, a strong indirect limbic connection was found. This
mask were averaged.

Amygdalaprefrontal pathways
The individual connectivity maps for each prefrontal connection were
normalized with the transformation obtained from the atlas registration,
averaged across participants, and thresholded at the target-specific level.
The anatomical locations of the fiber pathways were evaluated based on
the anatomical slices and a 3D volume rendering using the LIPSIA software (Lohmann et al., 2001).

Statistical analysis
To test the influence of trait anxiety and reappraisal use on the pathway
strength between amygdala (seed) and PFC regions (targets), multiple
hierarchical regressions were calculated individually for both hemispheres, with the factors trait anxiety (high trait anxiety vs low trait
anxiety, dummy coded), the individual reappraisal use score, and the
individual IQ score. The factor IQ was added to check the specificity of
the resultant pathways for emotion-related characteristics. There is evidence for a positive correlation between FA and IQ (Schmithorst et al.,
2005). If the delineated microstructure is emotion specific, IQ should not
explain additional variance.
The FA values on the calculated pathways between seed and targets
(dmPFC, dlPFC, vmPFC, and OFC, respectively) served as dependent
variables. Note that because of a priori knowledge of asymmetric hemispheric processing, we calculated hierarchical regressions and set up different hierarchies for both hemispheres. In the left hemisphere, the factor
reappraisal use was expected to explain most of the variance and thus was
the first variable to enter the regression, followed by the factor trait
anxiety and IQ as a third factor. To underline the hypothesis-driven
approach, we also tested the model where trait anxiety was the first factor.
Based on the literature, we expected no effects. In the right hemisphere,

pathway followed the fornix and the temporopulvinar tract to the


posterior thalamic nuclei, continuing through the anterior thalamic radiation to the frontal lobe. The latter connection was also
observed for the dorsal PFC regions (Fig. 2C,D). The dorsal regions and the vmPFC also showed a connection with the
amygdala via the cingulum.
Multiple hierarchical regressions
There are correlations between use of reappraisal and trait anxiety (r 0.377; p 0.008), but both of these variables did not
correlate with IQ (reappraisal: r 0.130, p 0.379; trait anxiety: r 0.182, p 0.216; all Pearson correlations). The intercorrelations between trait anxiety and reappraisal use stress the
use of methods that take this intercorrelation into account, such
as regression analysis.
The results of the multiple hierarchical regression analyses
with factor trait anxiety group as the first predictor (right hemisphere) will be reported first, followed by the results of the multiple hierarchical regression analyses with the factor reappraisal
use as the first predictor (left hemisphere).
The factor trait anxiety group (high trait anxiety vs low trait
anxiety) significantly predicted the FA values on the white matter
microstructure between the amygdala and the vmPFC (r 0.350,
df 47, F 6.441, T 2.538, p 0.015) and between the
amygdala and the OFC (r 0.395, df 47, F 8.520, T
2.919, p 0.005), both in the right hemisphere (see Table 2 for
a list of all statistical parameters for all multiple hierarchical re-

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits

J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027 6023

Figure 2. AD, Left, Three-dimensional sagittal and superior views of tracts between amygdala and PFC regions: A, orbitofrontal cortex; B, ventromedial PFC; C, dorsomedial PFC; D, dorsolateral
PFC. Renderings show the normalized number of tracts through each voxel (red). Seed and target regions are displayed in blue. For normalization, each voxel was divided by the amygdalas size. E,
Slices illustrating the tracts localization from medial to lateral. The distance between slices is 5 mm.

gressions). The FA values on the right-hemispheric white matter


microstructure between the amygdala and the dmPFC (r 0.215,
df 47, F 2.240, T 1,497, p 0.141) or the dlPFC (r
0.209, df 47, F 2.097, T 1,448, p 0.154) could not

significantly be predicted. In line with our hypotheses, the factor


reappraisal did not explain additional variance in this hemisphere
(vmPFC: r 0.377, df 47, F 3.731, T 1.009, p 0.318;
OFC: r 0.397, df 47, F 4.209, T 0.266, p 0.792;

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits

6024 J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027

Table 2. Significant relationships between trait anxiety, reappraisal use, IQ, and
fractional anisotropy values of pathway microstructure between amygdala and
prefrontal cortex regions
ROI

Model

R2

Cor. R 2

TA

Right dmPFC

TA
TA, R
TA, R, IQ
R
TA
TA, R
TA, R, IQ
R
TA
TA, R
TA, R, IQ
R
TA
TA, R
TA, R, IQ
R
R
R, TA
R, TA, IQ
TA
R
R, TA
R, TA, IQ
TA
R
R, TA
R, TA, IQ
TA
R
R, TA
R, TA, IQ
TA

0.05
0.06
0.06
0.03
0.04
0.06
0.06
0.04
0.12
0.14
0.18
0.07
0.16
0.16
0.17
0.01
0.09
0.11
0.11
0.05
0.11
0.14
0.15
0.08
0.11
0.13
0.14
0.06
0.09
0.11
0.15
0.05

0.03
0.02
0.00
0.01
0.02
0.02
0.00
0.02
0.10
0.10
0.13
0.05
0.14
0.12
0.11
0.01
0.07
0.07
0.05
0.03
0.09
0.10
0.09
0.06
0.09
0.09
0.08
0.04
0.07
0.07
0.09
0.03

2.24
1.41
0.95
1.56
2.10
1.53
1.01
2.06
6.44*
3.73*
3.31*
3.21
8.52**
4.21*
2.92*
0.54
4.74*
2.69
1.87
2.27
5.85*
3.73*
2.52
3.91
5.65*
3.36*
2.29
3.13
4.59*
2.68
2.51
2.44

0.22
0.17
0.18

Right dlPFC

Right vmPFC

Right OFC

Left dmPFC

Left dlPFC

Left vmPFC

Left OFC

0.21
0.16
0.16
0.35*
0.30
0.34*
0.40**
0.41**
0.39*
0.12
0.14
0.22
0.18
0.20
0.28
0.15
0.17
0.25
0.13
0.17
0.22

IQ

0.12
0.12
0.18

0.05

0.15
0.15
0.21

0.02

0.15
0.16
0.26

0.21

0.04
0.04
0.11
0.31*
0.26
0.27

0.08

0.34*
0.27
0.28

0.07

0.33*
0.28
0.28

0.07

0.30*
0.25
0.27

0.20

0.10

Model parameters for each predictor variable for hierarchical regression analyses are shown. Dependent variables
are fractional anisotropy values of pathway microstructure between amygdala and prefrontal cortex regions. ROI,
Region of interest; TA, trait anxiety; R, reappraisal use. *p 0.05; **p 0.01.

dmPFC: r 0.242, df 47, F 1.405, T 0.769, p 0.446;


dlPFC: r 0.252, df 47, F 1.530, T 0.982, p 0.331) and
IQ (vmPFC: r 0.429, df 47, F 3.308, T 1.502, p
0.140; OFC: r 0.408, df 47, F 2.923, T 0.674, p 0.504;
dmPFC: r 0.247, df 47, F 0.953, T 0.322, p 0.749;
dlPFC: r 0.253, df 47, F 1.005, T 0.148, p 0.883). As
expected, the alternative model with reappraisal as first factor did
not become significant in any of the four (right-hemispheric)
analyses.
The factor reappraisal significantly predicted the FA values
on the calculated white matter microstructure between the
amygdala and all four predefined PFC regions in the left hemisphere (dmPFC: r 0.306, df 47, F 4.741, T 2.177, p
0.035; dlPFC: r 0.336, df 47, F 5.854, T 2.419, p 0.020;
vmPFC: r 0.331, df 47, F 5.652, T 2.377, p 0.022;
OFC: r 0.301, df 47, F 4.591, T 2.143, p 0.037). The
addition of the factor anxiety group did not significantly increase
the explanation of variance in any of the regions (dmPFC: r
0.327, df 47, F 2.686, T 0.816, p 0.419; dlPFC: r
0.377, df 47, F 3.727, T 1.238, p 0.222; vmPFC:
r 0.361, df 47, F 3.364, T 1.033 p 0.307; OFC: r
0.326, df 47, F 2.678, T 0.887, p 0.380) or IQ (dmPFC:
r 0.336, df 47, F 1.870, T 0.564, p 0.576; dlPFC: r
0.383, df 47, F 2.521, T 0.485, p 0.630; vmPFC: r
0.367, df 47, F 2.285, T 0.491, p 0.626; OFC: r 0.382,

df 47, F 2.512, T 1.433, p 0.159). As hypothesized, the


alternative model with trait anxiety entering as first factor did not
reveal any effects.

Discussion
We investigated the microstructural properties of pathway connections between the amygdala and PFC regions deemed relevant
for anxiety and emotion regulation. The results provide direct
evidence for stronger connectivity between amygdala and
vmPFC and OFC of the right hemisphere in persons with low
trait anxiety. Emotion regulation was expressed as stronger connectivity between amygdala and vmPFC, OFC, dmPFC, and
dlPFC of the left hemisphere in persons with high reappraisal use.
We collected diffusion-weighted images (dMRI) and applied
probabilistic tractography to compute all tracks between
amygdala and PFC regions individually for both hemispheres.
The resulting white matter circuitry included (parts of) the uncinate fasciculus, cingulum, fornix, anterior thalamic radiation,
and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. Strong connectivity via
the uncinate fasciculus was observed for the pathways to vmPFC
and OFC, which was neither seen in the pathway network to
dlPFC nor to dmPFC. These connections, especially to vmPFC,
fit with findings from dMRI studies on humans and animals
(Croxson et al., 2005; Carlson et al., 2013; Jbabdi et al., 2013) and
with anatomical studies that reported extensive anatomical connections between the amygdala and the PFC. For instance, in rats
and monkeys, it was shown that the amygdala is connected to
prefrontal cortex by direct amygdalo-cortical projections (Nauta,
1961; Krettek and Price, 1974, 1977a), and indirectly via the thalamus (Nauta, 1962; Krettek and Price, 1974, 1977a,b). In rhesus
monkeys, the ventromedial region of PFC receives both direct
and indirect tracts stemming from the amygdala, but the dorsolateral PFC does not get such input (Porrino et al., 1981). These
findings suggested that the ventromedial region may be regarded
as the limbic portion of the frontal association cortex. The
longer, presumably indirect routes found in our study have not
been focused on and rarely have been addressed in human dMRI
studies on emotion processing or anxiety. Reviewing the role of
the PFC in emotion cognition interactions, Ray and Zald (2012)
emphasized that studies on nonhuman primates reveal large variability concerning direct projections between the amygdalae and
regions of the PFC and that direct projections between amygdala
and dlPFC are extremely weak. It may thus be surprising that
we find such strong connections between amygdala and prefrontal regions. However, there is ample evidence for indirect pathways, most notably through the thalamus, in both animals and
humans.
Recent research using tractography in humans provided further evidence for strong connections between hippocampus/
amygdala and thalamus. The amygdala connects with the
anterior thalamus via the fornix and uncinate fasciculus and with
the pulvinar via the temporopulvinar tract (Zarei et al., 2010).
Similarly, Linke et al. (2012) reported indirect pathways from the
amygdala to the OFC, namely via the anterior and posterior thalami. Note that these studies did not specifically target the connection between the amygdala and prefrontal regions.
With multiple hierarchical regressions, we analyzed the relationship of these determined fiber connections with trait anxiety
and reappraisal use by means of hypothesis-driven, hemispherespecific models. Given accumulating evidence for an asymmetric
involvement of the PFC, with a right-hemispheric dominance for
anxiety and anxiety-related processes (Stewart et al., 1988; Hermann et al., 1992; Hellige, 1993; OCarroll et al., 1993; Petruzzello

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits

and Landers, 1994; Lucey et al., 1995; Stapleton et al., 1997; Bremner et al., 1999; Nitschke et al., 1999; Wiedemann et al., 1999;
Davidson et al., 2000; Davidson, 2002; Pizzagalli et al., 2002; Smit
et al., 2007, Harmon-Jones et al., 2010), we expected our structural trait anxiety effects to be particularly visible in the right
hemisphere. Based on evidence for a left-hemispheric lateralization of reappraisal use (Ochsner et al., 2002; Jackson et al., 2003;
Kim and Bell, 2006) and recently observed left-sided biases of
metabolic activity in the superior frontal gyrus in frequent reappraisers (Kim et al., 2012), we expected reappraisal use effects to
be most prominent in the left hemisphere. We set up the hierarchies of the multiple regression analysis accordingly, with trait
anxiety as the first factor in the analyses of the right hemisphere
and with reappraisal use as the first factor for the left hemisphere.
The following methodological details are important to consider. First, we chose a hypothesis-based approach, analyzing the
microstructure of only those tracts to PFC regions that have consistently been associated with emotion-related processes. Different from prior studies, all tracks, including longer pathways
between the two structures, were considered. We calculated the
tracts for subsequent FA analysis on our sample, not relying on
data from thematically different experimental contexts. This is
more conservative than a whole-brain approach, where significant
regions are selected, analyzed, and interpreted post hoc. Second, we
also adopted the hypothesis-based approach in the statistical analysis. As mentioned above, anxiety and use of reappraisal is related to
hemisphere-specific networks. Whereas anxiety is primarily (but not
exclusively) related to processes of right-hemispheric amygdala and
PFC (Reiman et al., 1984; Heller et al., 1995), emotion regulation is
primarily associated with left-hemispheric processing in these
structures (Wheeler et al., 1993; Jackson et al., 2003; Kim and Bell,
2006; Kim et al., 2012). This differential hemispheric involvement in different aspects of emotion processing and regulation
supports the hypotheses by Davidson and colleagues (Davidson
et al., 1987, 1990, 2000; Davidson and Tomarken, 1989; Davidson, 1992, 1995, 2002; Sutton and Davidson, 1997). In low traitanxious persons, there was no higher FA in pathways to the
dorsomedial and the dorsolateral part of the PFC. One reason
might be that the amygdalar inhibition by these PFC regions
operates via general mechanisms, such as changes in the neurotransmitter system. Such mechanisms might not be reflected in
FA values of connecting pathways (Ray and Zald, 2012). An alternative explanation is that our atlas comprised comparably
wide boundaries around the dmPFC and the dlPFC. Although
this was deliberately done as not to miss potentially important
fibers, it could have led to an attenuation of the expected effect.
Some caveats and open questions should be addressed in future studies. First, we probabilistically tracked fiber pathways
from amygdala to PFC within each hemisphere, focusing on noncrossing fibers. However, animal studies have shown that a small
percentage of connecting fibers between the amygdala and the
PFC crosses the corpus callosum (Mascagni et al., 1993; McDonald and Mascagni, 1996). A second caveat concerns the origin of
the calculated fibers within the amygdala. We mapped the
amygdala in each participant and treated the entire amygdala as
seed region. Yet, the amygdala is a heterogeneous structure, with
various substructures and nuclei (Sah et al., 2003), each with
different functions and connections with different areas in the
brain. It is thus a challenge for future DTI studies to differentiate
connections between amygdala substructures and regions of the
PFC. Third, because women are especially prone to develop anxiety disorders (see Somers et al., 2006) and to avoid additional
variance attributable to existing brain-activation differences be-

J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027 6025

tween men and women (Gong et al., 2011), we included only


female persons. Studies revealed the sexually dimorphic nature of
the cortex, limbic structures, and connecting fibers and how such
sex differences might impact the development of affective disorders (for review, see Cahill, 2006). Zuurbier et al. (2013) observed
correlations between reappraisal and FA values in the left frontal
part of uncinate fasciculus in women, but not in men. Thus, it is
likely that at least some of the connections between amygdala and
PFC reported here cannot easily be transferred to men.
Although our results have to be interpreted within the constraints of the above-mentioned limitations, we believe that our
data make an important contribution to the understanding of the
neuroanatomical basis of anxiety and emotion regulation. Our
findings provide strong support for theories of and (fMRI) studies on top-down inhibition, by offering a structural network that
might underlie the so far mostly theoretically, behaviorally, and
functionally argued/established network.
Our data show that different structural networks are responsible for trait anxiety and reappraisal use. Neural models on the
generation and regulation of emotions stress the importance of
the interplay between automatic and voluntary processes (e.g.,
use of reappraisal; Ochsner and Gross, 2007; Phillips et al., 2008)
and emphasize the role of the amygdala and PFC regions (particularly OFC) for this interplay. The attempt to study voluntary
and automatic processes in separation may seem futile (Phillips et
al., 2008), especially in therapeutic settings. Nevertheless, we
identified a hemispherically specialized functional architecture
involved in the mediation of anxiety and reappraisal use, with
stronger connectivity between amygdala, OFC, and vmPFC in
each hemisphere. Given the correlational nature of these data, we
cannot make claims about the development of the relationship. It
is a challenge for future research to investigate how manipulating
one of these processes [e.g., using the anxiolytic effects of noninvasive brain stimulation (Zwanzger et al., 2009)] impacts on the
other, especially on the microstructure involved. Such investigations might help to develop new strategies for the treatment of
anxiety disorders and to identify persons at risk.

References
Ackenheil M, Stotz G, Dietz-Bauer R, Vossen A (1999) Deutsche Fassung
des Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview. Munchen: Psychiatrische Universitatsklinik Munchen.
Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC (2008) Symmetric diffeomorphic
image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of
elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal 12:26 41. CrossRef
Medline
Barbas H, Zikopoulos B (2006) Sequential and parallel circuits for emotional processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex. In: The orbitofrontal
cortex (Zald DH, Rauch SL, eds), pp 5791. Oxford: Oxford UP.
Basser PJ, Pierpaoli C (1996) Microstructural and physiological features of
tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J Magn Reson
111:209 219.
Baur V, Hanggi J, Langer N, Jancke L (2013a) Resting-state functional and
structural connectivity within an insulaamygdala route specifically index state and trait anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 73:8592. CrossRef Medline
Baur V, Bruhl AB, Herwig U, Eberle T, Rufer M, Delsignore A, Jancke L,
Hanggi J (2013b) Evidence of frontotemporal structural hypoconnectivity in social anxiety disorder: a quantitative fiber tractography study.
Hum Brain Mapp 34:437 446. CrossRef Medline
Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW (2007) Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can
we gain? Neuroimage 34:144 155. CrossRef Medline
Bishop SJ (2007) Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends Cogn Sci 11:307316. CrossRef Medline
Bremner JD, Staib LH, Kaloupek D, Southwick SM, Soufer R, Charney DS
(1999) Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic pictures and sound in
combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder: a posi-

6026 J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027


tron emission tomography study. Biol Psychiatry 45:806 816. CrossRef
Medline
Buckholtz JW, Callicott JH, Kolachana B, Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Genderson M, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, Meyer-Lindenberg A
(2008) Genetic variation in MAOA modulates ventromedial prefrontal
circuitry mediating individual differences in human personality. Mol Psychiatry 13:313324. CrossRef Medline
Cahill L (2006) Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:477
484. CrossRef Medline
Carlson JM, Cha J, Harmon-Jones E, Mujica-Parodi LR, Hajcak G (2013)
Influence of the BDNF genotype on amygdalo-prefrontal white matter
microstructure is linked to nonconscious attention bias to threat. Cereb
Cortex 24:2249 2257. CrossRef Medline
Croxson PL, Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Pinsk MA, Gross
CG, Richter W, Richter MC, Kastner S, Rushworth MF (2005) Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human
and macaque using probabilistic diffusion tractography. J Neurosci 25:
8854 8866. CrossRef Medline
Davidson RJ (1992) Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn 20:125151. CrossRef Medline
Davidson RJ (1995) Cerebral asymmetry, emotion and affective style. In:
Brain asymmetry (Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, eds), pp 361387. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Davidson RJ (2002) Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and
amygdala. Biol Psychiatry 51:68 80. CrossRef Medline
Davidson RJ, Tomarken AJ (1989) Laterality and emotion: an electrophysiological approach. Handb Neuropsychol 3:419 441.
Davidson RJ, Mednick D, Moss E, Saron C, Schaffer CE (1987) Ratings of
emotion in faces are influenced by the visual field to which stimuli are
presented. Brain Cogn 6:403 411. CrossRef Medline
Davidson RJ, Ekman P, Saron CD, Senulis JA, Friesen WV (1990)
Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression
and brain physiology: I. J Pers Soc Psychol 58:330 341. CrossRef Medline
Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Kalin NH (2000) Emotion, plasticity, context,
and regulation: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychol Bull
126:890 909. CrossRef Medline
Faria AV, Zhang J, Oishi K, Li X, Jiang H, Akhter K, Hermoye L, Lee SK, Hoon
A, Stashinko E, Miller MI, van Zijl PC, Mori S (2010) Atlas-based analysis of neurodevelopment from infancy to adulthood using diffusion tensor imaging and applications for automated abnormality detection.
Neuroimage 52:415 428. CrossRef Medline
Gong G, He Y, Evans AC (2011) Brain connectivity gender makes a difference. Neuroscientist 17:575591. CrossRef Medline
Gross JJ, John OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion regulation
processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc
Psychol 85:348 362. CrossRef Medline
Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA, Peterson CK (2010) The role of asymmetric
frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: a review and update. Biol Psychol 84:451 462. CrossRef Medline
Heller W, Etienne MA, Miller GA (1995) Patterns of perceptual asymmetry
in depression and anxiety: implications for neuropsychological models of
emotion and psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 104:327333. CrossRef
Medline
Hellige JB (1993) Hemispheric asymmetry: whats right and whats left.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP.
Hermann BP, Wyler AR, Blumer D, Richey ET (1992) Ictal fear: lateralizing
significance and implications for understanding the neurobiology of
pathological fear states. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol
5:205210.
Jackson DC, Mueller CJ, Dolski I, Dalton KM, Nitschke JB, Urry HL, Rosenkranz MA, Ryff CD, Singer BH, Davidson RJ (2003) Now you feel it,
now you dont: frontal brain electrical asymmetry and individual differences in emotion regulation. Psychol Sci 14:612 617. CrossRef Medline
Jbabdi S, Lehman JF, Haber SN, Behrens TE (2013) Human and monkey
ventral prefrontal fibers use the same organizational principles to reach
their targets: tracing versus tractography. J Neurosci 33:3190 3201.
CrossRef Medline
Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM (2012)
FSL. Neuroimage 62:782790. CrossRef Medline
Kim KJ, Bell MA (2006) Frontal EEG asymmetry and regulation during
childhood. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1094:308 312. CrossRef Medline
Kim MJ, Whalen PJ (2009) The structural integrity of an amygdalapre-

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits


frontal pathway predicts trait anxiety. J Neurosci 29:11614 11618.
CrossRef Medline
Kim MJ, Gee DG, Loucks RA, Davis FC, Whalen PJ (2010) Anxiety dissociates dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex functional connectivity
with the amygdala at rest. Cereb Cortex 21:16671673. CrossRef Medline
Kim SH, Hamann S (2007) Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. J Cogn Neurosci 19:776 798. CrossRef Medline
Kim SH, Cornwell B, Kim SE (2012) Individual differences in emotion regulation and hemispheric metabolic asymmetry. Biol Psychol 89:382386.
CrossRef Medline
Krettek JE, Price JL (1974) A direct input from the amygdala to the thalamus
and the cerebral cortex. Brain Res 67:169 174. CrossRef Medline
Krettek JE, Price JL (1977a) The cortical projections of the mediodorsal
nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol: 171:157
191. CrossRef Medline
Krettek JE, Price JL (1977b) Projections from the amygdaloid complex to
the cerebral cortex and thalamus in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol 172:
687722. CrossRef Medline
Linke J, Witt SH, King AV, Nieratschker V, Poupon C, Gass A, Hennerici MG,
Rietschel M, Wessa M (2012) Genome-wide supported risk variant for
bipolar disorder alters anatomical connectivity in the human brain. Neuroimage, 59:3288 3296. CrossRef Medline
Lohmann G, Mueller K, Bosch V, Mentzel H, Hessler S, Chen L, Zysset S, von
Cramon DY (2001) Lipsiaa new software system for the evaluation of
functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. Comput Med
Imaging Graph 25:449 457. CrossRef Medline
Lucey JV, Costa DC, Blanes T, Busatto GF, Pilowsky LS, Takei N, Marks IM,
Ell PJ, Kerwin RW (1995) Regional cerebral blood flow in obsessivecompulsive disordered patients at rest. Differential correlates with
obsessive-compulsive and anxious-avoidant dimensions. Br J Psychiatry
167:629 634. CrossRef Medline
Mascagni F, McDonald AJ, Coleman JR (1993) Corticoamygdaloid and corticocortical projections of the rat temporal cortex: a Phaseolus vulgaris
leucoagglutinin study. Neuroscience 57:697715. CrossRef Medline
McDonald AJ, Mascagni F (1996) Cortico-cortical and cortico-amygdaloid
projections of the rat occipital cortex: a Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin study. Neuroscience 71:3754. CrossRef Medline
Modi S, Trivedi R, Singh K, Kumar P, Rathore RK, Tripathi RP, Khushu S
(2013) Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white
matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fasciculus: preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study. Behav Brain Res 238:188
192. CrossRef Medline
Nauta WJ (1961) Fibre degeneration following lesions of the amygdaloid
complex in the monkey. J Anat 95:515531. Medline
Nauta WJ (1962) Neural associations of the amygdaloid complex in the
monkey. Brain 85:505520. CrossRef Medline
Nitschke JB, Heller W, Palmieri PA, Miller GA (1999) Contrasting patterns
of brain activity in anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology 36:628 637. CrossRef Medline
OCarroll RE, Moffoot AP, Van Beck M, Dougall N, Murray C, Ebmeier KP,
Goodwin GM (1993) The effect of anxiety induction on the regional
uptake of 99mTc-Exametazime in simple phobia as shown by single photon emission tomography (SPET). J Affect Disord 28:203210. CrossRef
Medline
Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2007) The neural architecture of emotion regulation.
Handbook Emot Regul 1:87109.
Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JDE (2002) Rethinking feelings:
an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J Neurosci 14:
12151229. CrossRef Medline
Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JD, Gross
JJ (2004) For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23:483
499. CrossRef Medline
Oishi K, Faria A, Jiang H, Li X, Akhter K, Zhang J, Hsu JT, Miller MI, van Zijl
PC, Albert M, Lyketsos CG, Woods R, Toga AW, Pike GB, Rosa-Neto P,
Evans A, Mazziotta J, Mori S (2009) Atlas-based whole brain white matter analysis using large deformation diffeomorphic metric mapping: application to normal elderly and Alzheimers disease participants.
Neuroimage 46:486 499. CrossRef Medline
Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97113. CrossRef Medline
Petrides M, Mackey S (2006) The orbitofrontal cortex: sulcal and gyral mor-

Eden et al. Fiber Microstructure Predicts Anxiety Traits


phology and architecture. In: The orbitofrontal cortex (Zald DH, Rauch
SL, eds), pp 19 37. Oxford: Oxford UP.
Petruzzello SJ, Landers DM (1994) State anxiety reduction and exercise:
does hemispheric activation reflect such changes? Med Sci Sports Exer
26:1028 1035. Medline
Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE,
Kolachana BS, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR (2005)
5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci
8:828 834. CrossRef Medline
Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC (2008) A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding
the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol
Psychiatry 13:833 857. CrossRef Medline
Pizzagalli DA, Nitschke JB, Oakes TR, Hendrick AM, Horras KA, Larson CL,
Abercrombie HC, Schaefer SM, Koger JV, Benca RM, Pascual-Marqui
RD, Davidson RJ (2002) Brain electrical tomography in depression: the
importance of symptom severity, anxiety and melancholic features. Biol
Psychiatry 52:73 85. CrossRef Medline
Porrino LJ, Crane AM, Goldman-Rakic PS (1981) Direct and indirect pathways from the amygdala to the frontal lobe in rhesus monkeys. J Comp
Neurol 198:121136. CrossRef Medline
Rauch SL, Savage CR, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ, Jenike MA (1997) The
functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a study of three disorders using
positron emission tomography and symptom provocation. Biol Psychiatry 42:446 452. CrossRef Medline
Ray RD, Zald DH (2012) Anatomical insights into the interaction of emotion and cognition in the prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:
479 501. CrossRef Medline
Reiman EM, Raichle ME, Butler FK, Herscovitch P, Robins E (1984) A focal
brain abnormality in panic disorder, a severe form of anxiety. Nature
310:683 685. CrossRef Medline
Roy M, Shohamy D, Wager TD (2012) Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical
systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn Sci 16:147
156. CrossRef Medline
Sah P, Faber ES, Lopez De Armentia M, Power J (2003) The amygdaloid
complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiol Rev 83:803 834. CrossRef
Medline
Schmithorst VJ, Wilke M, Dardzinski BJ, Holland SK (2005) Cognitive
functions correlate with white matter architecture in a normal pediatric
population: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 26:139 147.
CrossRef Medline
Sladky R, Hoflich A, Atanelov J, Kraus C, Baldinger P, Moser E, Lanzenberger
R, Windischberger C (2012) Increased neural habituation in the
amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder revealed by
fMRI. PLoS One 7:e50050. CrossRef Medline
Smit DJ, Posthuma D, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJ (2007) The relation between frontal EEG asymmetry and the risk for anxiety and depression.
Biol Psychol 74:26 33. CrossRef Medline
Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE,
Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, Luca M de, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy
RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, Stefano N de, Brady JM, Matthews PM
(2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and
implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 [Suppl 1]:S208 S219.
Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay
CE, Watkins KE, Ciccarelli O, Cader MZ, Matthews PM, Behrens TE
(2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject
diffusion data. Neuroimage 31:14871505. CrossRef Medline

J. Neurosci., April 15, 2015 35(15):6020 6027 6027


Snook L, Plewes C, Beaulieu C (2007) Voxel based versus region of interest
analysis in diffusion tensor imaging of neurodevelopment. Neuroimage
34:243252. CrossRef Medline
Solano-Castiella E, Anwander A, Lohmann G, Weiss M, Docherty C, Geyer S,
Reimer E, Friederici AD, Turner R (2010) Diffusion tensor imaging segments the human amygdala in vivo. Neuroimage, 49:2958 2965.
CrossRef Medline
Somers JM, Goldner EM, Waraich P, Hsu L (2006) Prevalence and incidence studies of anxiety disorders: a systematic review of the literature.
Can J Psychiatry 51:100 113. Medline
Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Russell S, Crane RS (1983) Assessment of anger:
the State-Trait Anger Scale. In: Advances in personality assessment, Vol 2
(Butcher JN, Spielberger CD, eds), pp 161185. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Stapleton JM, Morgan MJ, Liu X, Yung BC, Phillips RL, Wong DF, Shaya EK,
Dannals RF, London ED (1997) Cerebral glucose utilization is reduced
in second test session. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 17:704 712. Medline
Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP (2007) Increased
amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxietyprone subjects. Am J Psychiatry 164:318 327. CrossRef Medline
Stewart RS, Devous MD Sr, Rush AJ, Lane L, Bonte FJ (1988) Cerebral blood
flow changes during sodium-lactate-induced panic attacks. Am J Psychiatry 145:442 449. CrossRef Medline
Sutton SK, Davidson RJ (1997) Prefrontal brain asymmetry: a biological
substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychol Sci
8:204 210. CrossRef
Von Aster M, Neubauer A, Horn R (2006) Wechsler Intelligenztest fur
Erwachsene (WIE). Deutschsprachige Bearbeitung und Adaptation des
WAIS-III von David Wechsler. Frankfurt/Main, Germany: Harcourt Test
Services.
Wheeler RE, Davidson RJ, Tomarken AJ (1993) Frontal brain asymmetry
and emotional reactivity: a biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology 30:82 89. Medline
Wiedemann G, Pauli P, Dengler W, Lutzenberger W, Birbaumer N, Buchkremer G (1999) Frontal brain asymmetry as a biological substrate of emotions in patients with panic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:78 84.
CrossRef Medline
Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T,
Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2009) Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45:S173S186. CrossRef Medline
Zald DH, Kim SW (1996a) Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal
cortex: I. Anatomy, neurocircuitry, and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8:125138. CrossRef Medline
Zald DH, Kim SW (1996b) Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal
cortex, II: Function and relevance to obsessive-compulsive disorder.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 8:249 261. CrossRef Medline
Zarei M, Patenaude B, Damoiseaux J, Morgese C, Smith S, Matthews PM,
Barkhof F, Rombouts SA, Sanz-Arigita E, Jenkinson M (2010) Combining shape and connectivity analysis: an MRI study of thalamic degeneration in Alzheimers disease. Neuroimage 49:1 8. CrossRef Medline
Zuurbier LA, Nikolova YS, hs F, Hariri AR (2013) Uncinate fasciculus
fractional anisotropy correlates with typical use of reappraisal in women
but not men. Emotion 13:385390. CrossRef Medline
Zwanzger P, Fallgatter AJ, Zavorotnyy M, Padberg F (2009) Anxiolytic effects of transcranial magnetic stimulationan alternative treatment option in anxiety disorders? J Neural Transm 116:767775. CrossRef
Medline

Potrebbero piacerti anche