Sei sulla pagina 1di 4

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1559–1562

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp

FlashReports

On the belief in God: Towards an understanding of the emotional


substrates of compensatory control
Kristin Laurin, Aaron C. Kay *, David A. Moscovitch
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ont., Canada N2L 3G1

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We suggest that beliefs in a controlling God originate, at least in part, from the desire to avoid the
Received 6 June 2008 emotionally uncomfortable experience of perceiving the world as random and chaotic. Forty-seven par-
Revised 11 July 2008 ticipants engaged in an anxiety-provoking visualization procedure. For half, the procedure included a
Available online 5 August 2008
manipulation designed to temporarily lower beliefs in personal control. As predicted, it was only among
those participants whose sense of personal control was threatened—i.e., participants in need of an alter-
Keywords: nate means for protecting their belief in a non-random world—that subjective anxiety led to increased
Belief in God
subsequent beliefs in the existence of a controlling God. Wide-ranging implications are discussed.
Compensatory control
Anxiety
Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Arousal
Religion
System justification

Introduction be other resources upon which individuals can draw in response to


the threat of chaos and randomness in their environment.
There is no doubt that religious beliefs are of great consequence One especially powerful resource, we submit, is the belief in a
to human civilization. Yet, the psychological roots of such beliefs controlling God. The groundwork for this hypothesis has been laid
are still far from understood. Here, we empirically investigate the across several theoretical perspectives. At a broad level, we join a
possibility that these kinds of beliefs—in particular, the belief in a number of other researchers in suggesting that religious beliefs
controlling God—originate, at least in part, from a desire to defend emerge from ordinary cognitive processes (Boyer, 2003; Guthrie,
against the aversive emotional experience of perceiving the social 1993) and could be motivated by the desire to down-regulate
world as random and chaotic. threat (McGregor, 2006; McGregor, Gailliot, Vasquez, & Nash,
2007; McGregor & Haji, 2007) and avoid various negative psycho-
God as a resource of compensatory control logical states, such as stress (e.g., McIntosh, 1995; Pargament,
1997; Park, 2005), social alienation (Epley, Akalis, Waytz, & Caciop-
It has long been believed that human beings are motivated to po, 2008), or existential dilemmas (Atran & Norenzayan, 2004;
avoid the perception of randomness (Antonovsky, 1979; Kruglan- Jonas & Fischer, 2006; Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006; Osarchuk &
ski, 1989), particularly because this experience can be psychologi- Tatz, 1973). Moreover, recent research on system justification the-
cally stressful, even traumatic (e.g., Janoff-Bulman, 1992; ory (Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004; Kay et al., 2007) has explicitly
Pennebaker & Stone, 2004). Traditionally, the primary means noted the palliative function of endorsing broad ideological belief
through which people have been assumed to avoid this perception systems under times of threat. Thus, there is precedence for think-
is by affirming their belief in personal control (Kelly, 1955; Perkins, ing that religious beliefs might naturally be affected by defensive,
1968; Presson & Benassi, 1996; Seligman, 1975, 1976; Skinner, motivated processes.
1995; White, 1959). However, given recent observations that not Inspired by this work, Kay, Gaucher, Napier, Callan, and Laurin
all individuals, and indeed not all cultures, place such a strong (2008) recently tested the specific hypothesis that beliefs in per-
emphasis on personal control (e.g., Ji, Peng, & Nisbett, 2000; Pepi- sonal control and beliefs in the existence of external agents of con-
tone & Saffiotti, 1997; Rodin, Rennert, & Solomon, 1980), there must trol, such as a controlling God, can serve as substitutable
psychological resources in service of the motivation to avoid per-
ceptions of randomness and chaos (also, see Rothbaum, Weisz, &
Snyder, 1982). According to Kay et al., 2008, individuals can rely
* Corresponding author.
upon both external systems of control as well as internal beliefs
E-mail addresses: ackay@uwaterloo.ca (C. Kay), dmosco@uwaterloo.ca
(A. Moscovitch).
of personal control, in varying ratios, to help them cope with the

0022-1031/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.007
1560 K. Laurin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1559–1562

psychological stress associated with the perception of randomness; ical details). Participants were asked to sit quietly for three min-
that is, to affirm the general belief that things are under control utes, while SCLbasline was assessed. Following this, they were
(Antonovsky, 1979).1 Although there is evidence supporting ele- randomly assigned to one of two conditions and asked to vividly
ments of this theory (Kay et al., 2008), to date there has been no imagine the following scenario, played from an audio recording
experimental test of the main thrust of this idea: that the emotional (SCLmanipulation was recorded during this visualization procedure):
discomfort produced by the experience of lowered levels of per-
You have just gotten off work and begin to head home. It is a 15
sonal control is predictive of increased beliefs in the existence of
minute walk and you pass by a man, begging for money on the
a controlling God.
street. He asks you for some spare change, so you reach into
your pockets to look for some. Your pockets are empty so you
Overview of the present study
tell the man that you don’t have any. He doesn’t believe you
and begins to come towards you in a threatening manner.
In the present research, we seek direct evidence that endorsing
Again, you tell him that you have no change. He calls you a liar
the existence of a controlling God can be a response to the psycho-
and pulls out a knife from beneath his jacket. You frantically
logical discomfort associated with decreased perceptions of per-
look around for help, but there is no one. The street is deserted.
sonal control. In particular, we examine whether the experience
Following your instincts, you turn and run. He begins to chase
of anxiety—specifically in the context of low personal control—
you, screaming at you for money. All you can hear is the pound-
leads to increased belief in the existence of a controlling God.
ing of your heart and feet on the cement. You do not know
It is important to note the nuance of our prediction. We are not
where you are going, but if you stop he will catch you. You keep
suggesting that the mere experience of anxiety should necessarily
running; your life depends on it.
lead to an increased belief in a controlling God. Rather, we hypoth-
esize that when anxiety is coupled specifically with a decreased At this point, the two conditions diverged. In the Low Personal
sense of personal control, people will be driven to increase their Control (LPC) condition, the scenario continued as follows:
faith in external providers of order and structure, one of which
You are being chased. You scan your surroundings as you run.
could be a controlling God.
You turn around to see how close he is. He is right behind
All participants were asked to visualize a hypothetical scenario
you; you can almost feel his breath on your neck. . . Suddenly,
designed to induce anxiety; however, for a random half of the
the police come out of nowhere and intercept him. They have
participants, the visualization included a manipulation designed
saved you!
to lower beliefs in personal control, while for the other half, the
same visualization included a manipulation designed to affirm In the High Personal Control (HPC) condition, the scenario in-
beliefs in personal control. We then measured participants’ self- stead continued as follows:
reported levels of subjective anxiety. Skin conductance levels
You remember that you have your cell phone with you, so you
(SCL) were measured continuously throughout the study, with
dial 9–1–1. You tell the operator that you are being chased.
the onset and termination of periods of interest defined using
You tell her where you are and she tells you that the police
an event marker that was engaged manually by the experimenter
are on the way. You turn around to see how close he is. He
at the appropriate times. Finally, following the assessments of
is right behind you; you can almost feel his breath on your
anxiety and arousal, we asked participants to report the extent
neck. . . Suddenly, the police come out of nowhere and inter-
to which they believed in the existence of a controlling God. In
cept him. They have responded to your phone call! You have
line with Kay et al. (2008), we assume these beliefs can fluctuate
saved yourself!
situationally, and not just act as stable, trait-like individual
differences.
This manipulation was shown during pilot testing prior to the
Both of our visualizations were designed to be highly anxiety-
study to successfully produce differing perceptions of personal
provoking and so both were expected to be effective in boosting
control: participants exposed to the LPC manipulation rated items
participants’ levels of anxiety. However, as mentioned above, we
tapped to measure feelings of personal control (‘‘My life is deter-
expected the consequences of this anxiety to differ between the
mined exclusively by my own actions”, and ‘‘The events in my life
two experimental conditions. Specifically, we expected that anxi-
are mainly determined by my own actions”) significantly lower
ety—as measured by either SCL, self-report, or both—would lead
(M = 4.35, SD = 1.47) than those exposed to the HPC manipulation
to increased beliefs in the existence of a controlling God, but only
(M = 5.12, SD = 1.12), t(44) = 2.34, p < .03. In addition, and as ex-
when participants experienced this anxiety in the context of low-
pected, the manipulation did not lead to differences in subjective
ered beliefs in personal control.
anxiety (MLPC = 44.1, SDLPC = 25.2; MHPC = 44.3, SDHPC = 23.1),
t(44) < 1, ns, presumably because both manipulations led to similar
Method increases in anxiety, compared to baseline.
Immediately after the manipulation, participants were asked to
Participants complete a standard explicit anxiety measure, an ‘‘anxiety ther-
mometer” (Houtman & Bakker, 1989), on a scale from 0 (‘‘not at
Forty-seven students (29 females, 10 males, 8 unspecified; 17– all anxious or nervous) to 100 (‘‘extremely anxious or nervous”).
28 years of age) participated. Next, they answered the two questions about their belief in a con-
trolling God employed by Kay et al., 2008, using a seven-point scale
Procedure from 1 (‘‘tremendously doubtful”) to 7 (‘‘very likely”): ‘‘To what ex-
tent do you think it is feasible that God, or some type of non-hu-
Participants arrived in the study room and SCL electrodes were man entity, is in control, at least in part, of the events within our
attached (see Hofmann, Moscovitch, & Kim, 2006 for methodolog- universe?” and ‘‘To what extent do you think that the events that
occur in this world unfold according to God’s, or some other non-
1
It should be noted, however, that not all external sources of control are presumed human entity’s, plan?” Responses to the two items used here were
to be equivalently effective in helping to alleviate the stress of perceived randomness. very highly correlated, r = .91, p < .001, and therefore were aver-
In particular, Kay et al. (2008) noted that external systems perceived as being
aged into a single index of belief in a controlling God.
benevolent and/or efficacious are most effective in this regard.
K. Laurin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1559–1562 1561

Results visualization procedure, increased levels of self-reported anxiety


were associated with increased subsequent beliefs in the likelihood
Consistent with the pretesting data, the visualization sce- of the existence of a controlling God. In contrast, when personal
narios led to increased mean levels of arousal compared to control was not threatened by an otherwise identical anxiety-pro-
baseline (M SCL Baseline = 4.69, SDSCL Baseline = 1.94; MSCL Scenario = voking visualization, participants’ anxiety was unrelated to their
5.53, SDSCL Scenario = 2.24; F(46) = 36.15, p < .001), and this effect beliefs in a controlling God.
was not moderated by condition, F < 1, ns, indicating that the two It was interesting to note that while these effects were quite ro-
conditions were equally successful in generating anxious arousal. bust using self-reported anxiety, they were virtually non-existent
Data representing the relationship between anxiety and belief using physiologically-indexed anxiety—i.e., SCL. Although we had
in a controlling God across the two conditions were analyzed using no a priori predictions as to which anxiety measure would confirm
hierarchical regression. In the first step, we entered condition our predictions, we can now offer some speculation. Physiological
[0 = Low Personal Control (LPC), 1 = High Personal Control (HPC)], arousal and subjective negative affect are considered separate
SCLscenario (continuous; centered), and self-reported anxiety (con- components of anxiety (e.g., Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow, 1998;
tinuous; centered). In the second step, we entered the interactions Clark & Watson, 1991). While SCL was a precise measure of the
between the predictor variables: condition by self-reported anxi- arousal component, only the anxiety thermometer captured the
ety and condition by SCLscenario. subjective affective component. Perhaps, then, it is specifically
The first step of the regression explained 15.3% of the variance the experience of subjective emotional discomfort, in the context
in participants0 belief in a controlling God, with participants’ level of perceived threats to personal control, that leads people to rein-
of self-reported anxiety significantly associated with their belief in force their beliefs in a controlling God.
the existence of a controlling God, t(47) = 2.68, p = .010, b = .38. In Related to this, it is worth noting that this study involved a
the second step, which increased the explained variance to 25.3%, blend of experimental and correlational methodologies. Because
the interaction between condition and SCLscenario failed to reach of this, issues of directionality within our key experimental condi-
statistical significance, t(47) < 1, ns, b = .22. However, a significant tion must be addressed. Importantly, because participants’ beliefs
interaction emerged between condition and self-reported anxiety, in a controlling God represented our conceptual outcome variable,
t(47) = 2.37, p = .022, b = .50, completely qualifying the main we measured these beliefs after the anxiety-inducing visualization
effect of anxiety reported above. The condition  SCL interaction task, and employed a measure that has shown situational variance
was non-significant, (t(47) < 1, ns, b = .07). (Kay et al., 2008). For these reasons, we find our account—that anx-
The condition  self-reported anxiety interaction is illustrated iety induced in the context of threat to beliefs in personal control
in Fig. 1. As predicted, among participants whose personal control caused increased beliefs in a controlling God—quite plausible. An
was threatened, the more anxiety they reported, the more strongly alternative account of our data would suggest that that we in fact
they subsequently endorsed the existence of a controlling God, measured stable beliefs in a controlling God, and that these led
t(23) = 3.73, p = .001, b = .630. In contrast, for participants exposed participants to feel more anxious during the earlier threat to per-
to the same anxiety-provoking scenario but without a threat to sonal control. However, we are aware of no theoretical perspective
their personal control, levels of self-reported anxiety were not sig- that would explicitly support the prediction that stable beliefs in
nificantly predictive of beliefs in a controlling God, t(23) < 1, ns, God should lead to increased anxiety in the context of a threat to
b = .12. personal control.
Another methodological issue to consider relates to the way in
Discussion which we threatened personal control. It might be argued that the
low personal control manipulation was actually a high external
In the present study, we attempted to provide novel empirical control manipulation, in which the ever-watchful police rescue
evidence for one of the presumed psychological functions of the participant from an unpleasant fate. If this were the case, we
believing in God. We hypothesized that people who experience might have effectively primed participants in the low personal
anxiety in the context of perceived threats to personal control control condition to think about sources of external control in gen-
would be driven to increase their faith in a controlling God as a eral, including a controlling God. Such a possibility, however, is un-
compensatory strategy to restore order and defend against the per- likely for two reasons. First, both conditions in fact possessed
ception of randomness and chaos. Indeed, we found that when per- elements of high external control: the police ultimately played
sonal control was threatened during an anxiety-provoking the same role in both cases. Second, no difference was observed be-
tween conditions in terms of overall beliefs in a controlling God,
which would have been expected under such a priming interpreta-
tion. Thus, it is reasonable to argue that the two conditions differed
primarily in the extent to which they emphasized participants’
personal control over an equivalent outcome.

Concluding remarks

There is little doubt that religious beliefs drastically shape peo-


ple’s everyday lives and the broader political and social landscape.
Ultimately, the goal of the theoretical perspective presented here
(also see Kay et al., 2008) is to illuminate the contextual factors,
both permanent and fleeting, that produce increased or decreased
attachment to external agents of control, such as a controlling God.
One intriguing implication of this model is that those socio-cul-
tural contexts that strip individuals of (a) feelings of personal con-
Fig. 1. Mean belief in a controlling God as a function of self-reported anxiety and
trol (through, for example, poverty and other barriers to social
condition (LPC: low personal control; HPC: high personal control). mobility), as well as (b) legitimate and concrete sources of external
1562 K. Laurin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 1559–1562

control (such as stable governments), may be particularly likely to Jonas, E., & Fischer, P. (2006). Terror management and religion—Evidence that
intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality
breed reliance upon religious sources of comfort, insofar as such
salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 553–567.
sources are the only reasonable means through which people can Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justiifcation
maintain their faith in an orderly and non-random world. Of research: Accumulated evidence of consicous and unconscious bolstering of the
course, given the breadth and magnitude of religious beliefs, there status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919.
Kay, A. C., Jost, J. T., Mandisodza, A. N., Sherman, S. J., Petrocelli, J. V., Johnson, A. L.
are undoubtedly many causes to their widespread adoption (Atran (2007). Panglossian ideology in the service of system justification: How
& Norenzayan, 2004; Boyer, 2003; Epley et al., 2008; Guthrie, complementary stereotypes help us to rationalize inequality. In M. P. Zanna
1993; Jonas & Fischer, 2006; McIntosh, 1995; Norenzayan & Han- (Ed.), Experimental social psychology, Vol. 38, pp. 305–358.
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). God and the
sen, 2006; Osarchuk & Tatz, 1973; Pargament, 1997; Park, 2005). government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of
A complete understanding of their roots and causes, therefore, will external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 18–35.
most certainly require several different perspectives. It is our hope Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York, NY: Norton.
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and
that the precise model of compensatory control we have tested motivational bases. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
here and elsewhere (Kay et al., 2008) will contribute to our theo- McGregor, I. (2006). Zeal appeal: The allure of moral extremes. Basic and Applied
retical and empirical footing in this very important domain. Social Psychology, 28, 343–348.
McGregor, I., Gailliot, M. T., Vasquez, N. A., & Nash, K. A. (2007). Ideological and
personal zeal reactions to threat among people with high self-esteem:
Acknowledgments Motivated promotion focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
1587–1599.
McGregor, I., & Haji, R. (2007). Religious extremes and the uncertain self: The double-
This research was undertaken, in part, thanks to funding from a
edged sword of defensive zeal. York University: Toronto, Canada.
Social Science and Humanities Research Council of Canada re- McIntosh, D. N. (1995). Religion-as-schema, with implications for the relation
search grant to the second author, and the Canada Research Chairs between religion and coping. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion,
Program to the third author. Special thanks to Renée Chan and 5, 1–16.
Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of
Maureen Merrifield for their assistance with data collection. death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 174–187.
Osarchuk, M., & Tatz, S. J. (1973). Effect of induced fear of death on belief in afterlife.
References Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 256–260.
Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research,
practice. New York, NY: Guilford Publications.
Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health stress and coping. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Park, C. L. (2005). Religion as a meaning-making framework in coping with life
Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: stress. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 707–729.
Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioural and
Pennebaker, J. W., & Stone, L. D. (2004). Translating traumatic experiences into
Brain Sciences, 27, 713–770. language: Implications for child abuse and long-term health. In L. J. Koenig, L. S.
Boyer, P. (2003). Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function.
Doll, A. O’Leary, & W. Pequegnat (Eds.), From child sexual abuse to adult sexual
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 119–124. risk: Trauma, revictimization, and intervention (pp. 201–216). Washington, DC:
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Structural relationships among American Psychological Association.
dimensions of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and the dimensions of
Pepitone, A., & Saffiotti, L. (1997). The selectivity of nonmaterial beliefs in
negative affect, positive affect, and autonomic arousal. Journal of Abnormal interpreting life events. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 23–35.
Psychology, 107, 179–192.
Perkins, C. C. (1968). An analysis of the concept of reinforcement. Psychological
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Review, 75, 155–172.
Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Presson, P. K., & Benassi, V. A. (1996). Illusion of control: A meta-analytic review.
Psychology, 100, 316–336. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 493–510.
Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social connection Rodin, J., Rennert, K., & Solomon, S. K. (1980). Intrinsic motivation for control: Fact
through inferential reproduction: Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets,
or fiction. In A. Baum & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology:
gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, 114–120. Vol. 2. Applications of personal control. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Guthrie, S. E. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. New York, NY: Rothbaum, R., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing
Oxford University Press. the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and
Hofmann, S. G., Moscovitch, D. A., & Kim, H.-J. (2006). Autonomic correlates of social Social Psychology, 42, 5–37.
anxiety and embarrassment in shy and non-shy individuals. International
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San
Journal of Psychophysiology, 61, 134–142. Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Houtman, I. L. D., & Bakker, F. C. (1989). The anxiety thermometer: A validation
Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness and depression in animals and men.
study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 575–582. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, & coping. Thousand Oaks, CA:
trauma. New York, NY: Free Press. Sage Publications, Inc..
Ji, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of White, R. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence.
relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychological Review, 66, 297–330.
78, 943–955.

Potrebbero piacerti anche