Sei sulla pagina 1di 21

Page 1 of 21

Motor Control

BDNF Val66Met is associated with performance in a


computerized visual-motor tracking test in healthy adults

Yeimy Gonzlez-Giraldo, BSc 1,7; Johana Rojas, BSc 2,7; Shane T. Mueller, PhD 3;
Brian J. Piper, PhD 4; Ana Adan, PhD 5,6; Diego A. Forero, MD, PhD 1,*

r
Fo

Laboratory of NeuroPsychiatric Genetics, Biomedical Sciences Research Group, School of Medicine, Universidad

Antonio Nario. Bogot, Colombia.

School of Health Sciences, Universidad Colegio Mayor de Cundinamarca. Bogot, Colombia.

Department of Cognitive and Learning Sciences, Michigan Technological University, Houghton, Michigan, USA.

School of Pharmacy, Husson University, Bangor, Maine, USA.

Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.

Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior (IR3C), Barcelona, Spain

These authors contributed equally to this work

Correspondence: Prof. Dr. Diego Forero, MD, PhD. Laboratory of NeuroPsychiatric Genetics, Biomedical Sciences

er

Pe

Re

Research Group, School of Medicine, Universidad Antonio Nario. Bogot, Colombia. Phone: + 57 313 2610427.

Word Count in Body of Manuscript: 2514

ew

Word Count in Abstract: 120

vi

Email: diego.forero@uan.edu.co

Tables: 2, Figures: 1, Supplementary Files: 1

Keywords: Neurogenetics, Endophenotypes, Latin America, Motor Learning, Candidate


Genes, Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor.

Running Title: BDNF gene and visual-motor tracking performance

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 2 of 21

Abstract
Brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is known to play an important role in
neuroplasticity and cognitive processes. We explored the association of BDNF Val66Met
polymorphism with performance in a visual-motor tracking test. One hundred and sixty
seven young healthy Colombian adults completed a computerized version of the Pursuit
Rotor Task, using the Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) platform. DNA

r
Fo

genotyping was performed by allele-specific PCR. We found that BDNF Val/Met and
Met/Met subjects had a better performance in the pursuit rotor task (p value: 0.03). Our

Pe

findings suggest that BDNF gene is key to understand differences in motor performance
in healthy participants in different populations. This approach could be useful for future

er

fine mapping of genetic modifiers for neuropsychiatric diseases.

ew

vi

Re
Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 3 of 21

Motor Control

Introduction
Adequate functioning of the mechanisms underlying motor learning and performance
are important for daily activities in normal subjects and for pathophysiological
mechanisms of neuropsychiatric diseases and related processes for rehabilitation
(Chang, 2014; Dayan & Cohen, 2011). Neural plasticity in a number of brain regions and
neurophysiological networks (such as the primary motor cortex, premotor cortex and the
supplementary motor area) is known to be correlated with these processes (Dayan &

r
Fo

Cohen, 2011). Several experimental paradigms, such as finger tapping, have been used
for an initial exploration of the basis of motor learning and performance in humans

er

Pe

(Dayan & Cohen, 2011).

In recent years, explorations of genetic factors have started to provide evidence of


specific molecular mechanisms associated with motor learning and performance in

Re

healthy subjects (Kleim et al., 2006; Smolders, Rijpkema, Franke, & Fernandez, 2012).

vi

Given the important role of proteins involved in synaptic plasticity, the brain derived

ew

neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene has been a key candidate (Kleim et al., 2006).

BDNF is a commonly studied neurotrophin that plays an important role in several basic
neural processes such as synaptic plasticity and long-term potentiation and cognitive
phenomena, such as learning and memory processes (Forero, Benitez, et al., 2006).
This protein of 267 amino acids is encoded by the BDNF gene, which is located on
chromosome 11 and has 8 exons (gene size: 65 kb) (Liu et al., 2005). A change of
Valine to Methionine in codon 66 (Val66Met) constitutes a functional Single Nucleotide
Polymorphism (SNP) in BDNF (dbSNP ID: rs6265); this variation affects intracellular

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 4 of 21

traffic and activity-dependent release of BDNF in an allele-specific manner (Egan et al.,


2003).

BDNF Val66Met polymorphism has been studied as a genetic candidate for differences
in performance in cognitive tests in healthy subjects, showing that Val/Val carriers had
better declarative memory and hippocampal activation (Kambeitz et al., 2012). Previous
studies have used relatively simple behavioral paradigms, such as finger tapping, to

r
Fo

study the possible association of BDNF gene and motor learning and performance
(Kleim et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2013; McHughen et al., 2010) (Table 1, for further

Pe

details). In general, the tasks requiring placing and moving manual skills (Piper, 2011)
show no associations with BNDF genotype; with a few cases using visual-motor tracking

er

tasks in which Val/Met subject exhibited poorer performance after relatively long-term
learning.

Re

vi

The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that healthy carriers of the BDNF Met

ew

allele could show differences in performance in a computerized visual motor tracking


task, using the Pursuit Rotor Test (PRT) of the Psychology Experiment Building
Language battery. The PRT has long been used as a measure of both perceptual-motor
skill, and of motor adaptation and learning (R. Ammons, 1955; R. B. Ammons, 1947).
More recent research has suggested that such motor learning is governed by two
complementary processes (Smith, Ghazizadeh, & Shadmehr, 2006; Trewartha, Garcia,
Wolpert, & Flanagan, 2014): a fast process linked to declarative memory that adapts
and decays quickly (Keisler & Shadmehr, 2010) and a slower process that adapts and
decays more gradually. We hypothesized that our results could differ from these

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 5 of 21

Motor Control

previous results for two reasons. First, the majority of null results in Table 1 involved fine
motor movement and placing, whose motor control differs from smooth pursuit (Wolpert,
Diedrichsen, & Flanagan, 2011). Second, we tested participants from a South American
population (whereas previous studies were carried out in samples of European and
Asian descent) and our sample size is several times larger than previous studies
(median sample for previous studies was 38 participants). Environmental or epigenetic
factors (Forero, Velez-van-Meerbeke, Deshpande, Nicolini, & Perry, 2014) could lead to

r
Fo

this population having different performance advantages. We thus expected that a test
as such as the PEBL PRT, that hypothetically requires fast learning and adaptation

motor learning.

Materials and methods

Re

Subjects

er

Pe

linked to declarative memory systems, could identify allele-specific effects of BDNF on

vi

One hundred and sixty eight unrelated subjects were recruited at a University in Bogot,

ew

Colombia. The population living in Bogot is composed of a European genetic


background with some historical admixture with Amerindians (Forero, Arboleda, Yunis,
Pardo, & Arboleda, 2006; Ojeda et al., 2013). Participants were only included if they
were young university students whose four grandparents were born in Colombia.
Participants were excluded if they had personal history of neuropsychiatric diseases,
were taking medications acting on nervous system, or if they have incomplete
phenotypic or genotypic data. All participants provided written informed consent
(Beskow et al., 2001) and the study was approved by the institutional ethics committee.

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 6 of 21

Phenotypic Evaluations
All participants completed a self-administered questionnaire. The questionnaire was
used to collect socio-demographic variables (age, sex, personal and familial history of
neuropsychiatric disorders). We applied to all participants a computerized task, the
PEBL Pursuit Rotor Test, for analyzing quantitative and objective measures of motor
performance, using the Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) platform,
which is freely available at http://pebl.sourceforge.net/ and has been described

r
Fo

previously (Mueller & Piper, 2014). Pursuit rotor test consists on following, with the
mouse pointer, a small red circle that rotates clockwise at a constant speed around a

Pe

larger circle (Piper, 2011) (Figure 1). Subjects received verbal and visual instructions
before the start of the testing phase, where they had 30 seconds to complete two rounds

er

of the test (each one with a duration of 15 seconds) Source code was modified to allow
its application in the Spanish language, using a laptop (Hewlett-Packard Company, Palo

Re

Alto, CA, USA) running a Windows 7 operating system. Performance scores were given

vi

by the total time on target for each individual. The PEBL Pursuit Rotor task has been

ew

shown to be sensitive to a number of neurologically and genetic factors, including


development, gender, and handedness (Piper, 2011), tremor (Walecki, Laso, Kunc, &
Gorzelaczyk, 2013), synaptic plasticity deficits related to Tourrette's Syndrome (Brandt
et al., 2014), and relatively unimpacted by time of day (which nevertheless did impact an
attention task) or extensive practice (which did improve performance a dynamic
compensatory tracking task), see (Ahonen, Dunham, Getty, & Kosmowski, 2012).
Neuroimaging studies have identified a possible role of circuits in the prefrontal cortex
and the presupplementary motor area related to the pursuit rotor task (Hatakenaka,
Miyai, Mihara, Sakoda, & Kubota, 2007).

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 7 of 21

Motor Control

Genotyping
A sample of peripheral blood from each participant was taken for genomic DNA
extraction, using a salting out method (Miller, Dykes, & Polesky, 1988; Morales et al.,
2009). Genotyping of BDNF Val66Met was performed by tetra-primer allele specific
Polymerase Chain Reaction (PCR) in a Labnet MultiGene 96-well thermal cycler (Labnet
International Inc, Edison, NJ, USA). PCR products were separated in a 2% agarose gel

r
Fo

at 140V and visualized with ultraviolet light after SYBR-safe staining (Invitrogen,
Carlsbad, CA, USA). We used 4 primers: (P1: CCT ACA GTT CCA CCA GGT GAG

Pe

AAG AGT G, P2: TCA TGG ACA TGT TTG CAG CAT CTA GGT A, P3: CTG GTC CTC
ATC CAA CAG CTC TTC TAT AAC and P4: ATC ATT GGC TGA CAC TTT

er

CGA ACC CA) (Sheikh, Hayden, Kryski, Smith, & Singh, 2010). The PCR reactions
included 2 l of genomic DNA (~50 ng), 1.5 mM MgCl 2, 10X reaction buffer, 1 M of

Re

each primers, 1 mM of dNTPs, Betaine 1 M and 0.75 U of Taq polymerase (Bioline,

vi

London, United Kingdom) in a final volume of 20 l. PCR program consisted of

ew

a 3 -min denaturation step at 94C (1 cycle), 94C for 20s, 63C for 25s, 72C for 40s
(30 cycles), and 72C for 5 minutes (1 cycle). Alleles and genotypes were scored as
follows: An allele as 401 and 204 bp bands and G allele as 401 and 253 bp bands. A
random subsample (10% of subjects) was reanalyzed for both polymorphisms to assure
consistency in the genotyping results. All genotypes were checked by two different
investigators in order to confirm and validate the results (Benitez et al., 2010; Ojeda,
Nino, et al., 2014). Additionally, genotyping data were validated by a method based in
qPCR, which combines high resolution melting and allele-specific PCR (Ojeda, Lopez-

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 8 of 21

Leon, & Forero, 2014), in a CFX96 Touch Real-Time PCR Detection System (BioRad,
Hercules, California).

Statistical analyses
X2 test was used for calculation of HardyWeinberg equilibrium and estimation of
genotype and allele frequencies was carried out with the SNPStats program (Perea et
al., 2014; Sole, Guino, Valls, Iniesta, & Moreno, 2006). Performance scores were given

r
Fo

by the total time on target (values from 0 to 15.000 milliseconds) for the second round of
the test: Higher values for time on target corresponded to better performances. A linear

Pe

regression model was used to analyze the association of BDNF genotypes with
quantitative measures of motor learning, correcting for sex and age (Piper, 2011). A p

er

value < 0.05 was considered as statistically significant.

Re

Results

vi

In total 167 subjects participated in this study, 118 were women and 49 men, with ages

ew

from 18 to 34 years (mean: 21.2, SD: 2.9). Scores for pursuit rotor task ranged from
1344 to 14058 time-on-target (in milliseconds) with a theoretical maximum of 15000
milliseconds (average time-on-target of 8599 milliseconds; SE: 220.9). There were no
significant differences in mean scores between female and male subjects (p value:
0.37).

Minor allele frequency for the Val66Met polymorphism in BDNF was 0.13 (A/Met allele),
the distribution of genotypes was: Val/Val (G/G) n=128 (77%), Met/Val (A/G) n=34 (20
%) and Met/Met (A/A) n= 5 (3%). Observed genotypes were in Hardy-Weinberg

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 9 of 21

Motor Control

equilibrium (p value: 0.17). Mean performance scores (time-on-target in milliseconds) for


the G/G, A/G and A/A subjects were 8342.7, 9657.0 and 7956.6, respectively (Figure
S1).

A linear regression model was used to assess the association between BDNF Val66Met
genotypes and pursuit rotor task scores, correcting by sex and age. We found a
significant association, under a dominant model, where Met carrier subjects (Val/Met

r
Fo

and Met/Met) had a better performance in comparison with Val/Val homozygous


subjects (p value 0.03) (Table 2).

er

Pe

Discussion

Previous studies on BDNF and motor learning in healthy subjects have been carried out
with a variety of tests, such as finger tapping, mainly in populations of European descent

Re

(Table 1) (Cirillo, Hughes, Ridding, Thomas, & Semmler, 2012; Freundlieb et al., 2012;

vi

Joundi et al., 2012; Kleim et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2013; Li Voti et al., 2011; McHughen &

ew

Cramer, 2013; McHughen et al., 2010; Smolders et al., 2012; Witte et al., 2012). It is
important to highlight that, in comparison with previous studies, our work is the largest
(in terms of sample size) in this field and that our study is the first work that uses the
computerized PEBL PRT as a quantitative endophenotype of fine motor function, an
automated brief visuomotor tracking test, for exploration of genetic correlates. As shown
in Table 1, most previous studies of the impact of BDNF Val66Met on performance of
manual tasks have involved tasks requiring tapping and manipulating and longer-term
visuomotor learning, whereas the Pursuit Rotor test is a very brief assessment of
visuomotor tracking. Research has indicated that pursuit motor control behaves as a

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 10 of 21

10

series of ballistic (Craik, 1948) Fitts-law movements (Zhang & Hornof, 2012), with
corrective movements involving a tight perceptual-motor cycle. This type of motor control
is fundamentally different from simple keypress motor responses whose motor control
and decision properties obey the Hick-Hyman law (Seow, 2005). Furthermore,
adaptation on the PRT may primarily involve a fast motor learning process linked to
declarative memory, which (Kambeitz et al., 2012) showed was associated with BNDF
genotype). In addition, other works have used tests that are possibly more demanding

r
Fo

cognitively or involve a slower motor learning process (Cirillo et al., 2012; Joundi et al.,
2012; McHughen & Cramer, 2013; Smolders et al., 2012); and although one used a

Pe

visuomotor tracking task (Cirillo et al., 2012) it is different from the task we used in this
work (lines that moved down the screen while making left and right movements that

er

were unpredictable). It is possible that its small sample size (only 29 subjects in total, 17
Met carriers vs 12 non Met carriers) and differences in genetic background could also

Re

explain the differences in findings. Joundi et al (Joundi et al., 2012) used a task that

vi

was oriented to test visuomotor adaptation (a 60 degree deviation) and Smolders et al

ew

(Smolders et al., 2012) used a task oriented to assess bimanual motor control.

In this study, we found that the subjects that are carriers of the Met allele (Val/Met and
Met/Met genotypes) showed a better performance on the pursuit rotor task. Our current
findings are in agreement with previous neurophysiological and imaging studies that
found differences in brain activation and functioning in key areas between BDNF
genotypes (evidence of motor cortex plasticity), following experimental paradigms that
used motor training or external stimuli (Kleim et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2013; McHughen et
al., 2010). Our findings of a positive association of BDNF and the PEBL pursuit rotor test

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 11 of 21

Motor Control

11

is compatible with current theories of motor learning that suggest that the initial stage of
motor learning (fast stage) is related to the function of brain circuits involved in superior
cognitive functions, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Dayan & Cohen, 2011;
Wolpert et al., 2011), considering the role of BDNF dependent plasticity on those neural
circuits (Hong, Liou, & Tsai, 2011). A recent animal model of chronic stroke showed that
Met/Met genotype carriers showed a better motor and neurophysiological functioning
(Qin et al., 2014). There is evidence from previous studies that the alleles associated

r
Fo

with better cognitive performance in other ethnicities are different from those found in
samples of European descent (Gonzalez-Giraldo et al., 2014; Solis-Ortiz, Perez-Luque,

er

Pe

Morado-Crespo, & Gutierrez-Munoz, 2010; Wang et al., 2013).

Analysis of different populations is a key factor in genetic analysis of human brain


endophenotypes (Forero et al., 2014). Frequencies of risk alleles, patterns of linkage

Re

disequilibrium and interactions with environmental factors are known to vary between

vi

populations (Gatt, Burton, Williams, & Schofield, 2015). Limitations of the current study

ew

includes: Use of a single task for the analysis of motor function and study of a single
functional SNP in BDNF gene. Analysis of genetic and epigenetic variants in other
candidate molecules involved in synaptic plasticity mechanisms will be fundamental for
a better understanding of motor learning and performance in healthy subjects (Forero,
Arboleda, Vasquez, & Arboleda, 2009; Hernandez, Tse, Pang, Arboleda, & Forero,
2013; Strazisar et al., 2014).

In the context of molecular genetics of cognitive endophenotypes, the use of an


automated neuropsychological battery (such as PEBL), has several advantages,

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 12 of 21

12

allowing a quantitative assessment (in real time) of multiple cognitive domains for a
large number of participants (Gonzalez-Giraldo et al., 2014; Ness et al., 2011; Tumkaya
et al., 2013). In addition, as PEBL is freely available, it facilitates testing in different
economic backgrounds, leading to the possibility of sampling a greater genetic diversity
in different countries and ethnicities (Mueller & Piper, 2014). Our analysis of genetic
factors associated with motor learning endophenotypes could be also useful in the
context of the fine mapping of genetic modifiers for neurological and psychiatric

r
Fo

diseases in patients (Alaerts et al., 2009; Castro et al., 2013; Galvez et al., 2014; He,
Zhang, Yung, Zhu, & Wang, 2013).

er

Pe

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from Universidad Antonio Nario (VCTI-UAN,
Project # 20131079). YGG was supported by a fellowship from VCTI-UAN (Young

ew

vi

Re

Scientists Program).

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 13 of 21

Motor Control

13

References

r
Fo

Ahonen, B, Dunham, C, Getty, E, & Kosmowski, KJ. (2012). The effects of time of day and practice on
cognitive abilities: the PEBL pursuit rotor, compensatory tracking, match-to-sample, and TOAV
tasks. PEBL Techn Rep Ser, 2.
Alaerts, M., Ceulemans, S., Forero, D., Moens, L. N., De Zutter, S., Heyrman, L., . . . Del-Favero, J. (2009).
Detailed analysis of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) shows no association with bipolar
disorder in the Northern Swedish population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 150B(4),
585-592. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30853
Ammons, RB. (1955). Rotary pursuit apparatus: I. Survey of variables. Psychological bulletin, 52(1), 69.
Ammons, Robert B. (1947). Acquisition of motor skill: II. Rotary pursuit performance with continuous
practice before and after a single rest. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37(5), 393.
Benitez, B. A., Forero, D. A., Arboleda, G. H., Granados, L. A., Yunis, J. J., Fernandez, W., & Arboleda, H.
(2010). Exploration of genetic susceptibility factors for Parkinson's disease in a South American
sample. J Genet, 89(2), 229-232.
Beskow, L. M., Burke, W., Merz, J. F., Barr, P. A., Terry, S., Penchaszadeh, V. B., . . . Khoury, M. J. (2001).
Informed consent for population-based research involving genetics. JAMA, 286(18), 2315-2321.
Brandt, V. C., Niessen, E., Ganos, C., Kahl, U., Baumer, T., & Munchau, A. (2014). Altered synaptic
plasticity in Tourette's syndrome and its relationship to motor skill learning. PLoS One, 9(5),
e98417. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098417
Castro, T., Mateus, H. E., Fonseca, D. J., Forero, D., Restrepo, C. M., Talero, C., . . . Laissue, P. (2013).
Sequence analysis of the ADRA2A coding region in children affected by attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Neurol Sci, 34(12), 2219-2222. doi: 10.1007/s10072-013-1569-4
Cirillo, J., Hughes, J., Ridding, M., Thomas, P. Q., & Semmler, J. G. (2012). Differential modulation of
motor cortex excitability in BDNF Met allele carriers following experimentally induced and usedependent plasticity. Eur J Neurosci, 36(5), 2640-2649. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08177.x
Craik, Kenneth JW. (1948). Theory of the human operator in control systems. British Journal of
Psychology. General Section, 38(3), 142-148.
Chang, Y. (2014). Reorganization and plastic changes of the human brain associated with skill learning
and expertise. Front Hum Neurosci, 8, 35. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00035
Dayan, E., & Cohen, L. G. (2011). Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron, 72(3), 443-454.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008
Egan, M. F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J. H., Goldberg, T. E., Kolachana, B. S., Bertolino, A., . . . Weinberger, D.
R. (2003). The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and
human memory and hippocampal function. Cell, 112(2), 257-269.
Forero, D. A., Arboleda, G. H., Vasquez, R., & Arboleda, H. (2009). Candidate genes involved in neural
plasticity and the risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of 8 common
variants. J Psychiatry Neurosci, 34(5), 361-366.
Forero, D. A., Arboleda, G., Yunis, J. J., Pardo, R., & Arboleda, H. (2006). Association study of
polymorphisms in LRP1, tau and 5-HTT genes and Alzheimer's disease in a sample of Colombian
patients. J Neural Transm, 113(9), 1253-1262. doi: 10.1007/s00702-005-0388-z
Forero, D. A., Benitez, B., Arboleda, G., Yunis, J. J., Pardo, R., & Arboleda, H. (2006). Analysis of functional
polymorphisms in three synaptic plasticity-related genes (BDNF, COMT AND UCHL1) in
Alzheimer's
disease
in
Colombia.
Neurosci
Res,
55(3),
334-341.
doi:
10.1016/j.neures.2006.04.006
Forero, D. A., Velez-van-Meerbeke, A., Deshpande, S. N., Nicolini, H., & Perry, G. (2014). Neuropsychiatric
genetics in developing countries: Current challenges. World J Psychiatry, 4(4), 69-71. doi:
10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.69

er

Pe

ew

vi

Re

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 14 of 21

14

r
Fo

Freundlieb, N., Philipp, S., Schneider, S. A., Bruggemann, N., Klein, C., Gerloff, C., & Hummel, F. C. (2012).
No association of the BDNF val66met polymorphism with implicit associative vocabulary and
motor learning. PLoS One, 7(11), e48327. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048327
Galvez, J. M., Forero, D. A., Fonseca, D. J., Mateus, H. E., Talero-Gutierrez, C., & Velez-van-Meerbeke, A.
(2014). Evidence of association between SNAP25 gene and attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder in a Latin American sample. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord, 6(1), 19-23. doi:
10.1007/s12402-013-0123-9
Gatt, J. M., Burton, K. L., Williams, L. M., & Schofield, P. R. (2015). Specific and common genes implicated
across major mental disorders: A review of meta-analysis studies. J Psychiatr Res, 60C, 1-13. doi:
10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.09.014
Gonzalez-Giraldo, Y., Rojas, J., Novoa, P., Mueller, S. T., Piper, B. J., Adan, A., & Forero, D. A. (2014).
Functional Polymorphisms in BDNF and COMT Genes Are Associated with Objective Differences
in Arithmetical Functioning in a Sample of Young Adults. Neuropsychobiology, 70(3), 152-157.
doi: 10.1159/000366483
Hatakenaka, M., Miyai, I., Mihara, M., Sakoda, S., & Kubota, K. (2007). Frontal regions involved in
learning of motor skill--A functional NIRS study. Neuroimage, 34(1), 109-116. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.014
He, Y. Y., Zhang, X. Y., Yung, W. H., Zhu, J. N., & Wang, J. J. (2013). Role of BDNF in central motor
structures and motor diseases. Mol Neurobiol, 48(3), 783-793. doi: 10.1007/s12035-013-8466-y
Hernandez, H. G., Tse, M. Y., Pang, S. C., Arboleda, H., & Forero, D. A. (2013). Optimizing methodologies
for PCR-based DNA methylation analysis. Biotechniques, 55(4), 181-197. doi: 10.2144/000114087
Hong, C. J., Liou, Y. J., & Tsai, S. J. (2011). Effects of BDNF polymorphisms on brain function and behavior
in health and disease. Brain Res Bull, 86(5-6), 287-297. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.08.019
Joundi, R. A., Lopez-Alonso, V., Lago, A., Brittain, J. S., Fernandez-Del-Olmo, M., Gomez-Garre, P., . . .
Brown, P. (2012). The effect of BDNF val66met polymorphism on visuomotor adaptation. Exp
Brain Res, 223(1), 43-50. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3239-9
Kambeitz, J. P., Bhattacharyya, S., Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L. M., Valli, I., Collier, D. A., & McGuire, P. (2012).
Effect of BDNF val(66)met polymorphism on declarative memory and its neural substrate: a
meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 36(9), 2165-2177. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.002
Keisler, A., & Shadmehr, R. (2010). A shared resource between declarative memory and motor memory. J
Neurosci, 30(44), 14817-14823. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4160-10.2010
Kleim, J. A., Chan, S., Pringle, E., Schallert, K., Procaccio, V., Jimenez, R., & Cramer, S. C. (2006). BDNF
val66met polymorphism is associated with modified experience-dependent plasticity in human
motor cortex. Nat Neurosci, 9(6), 735-737. doi: 10.1038/nn1699
Lee, M., Kim, S. E., Kim, W. S., Lee, J., Yoo, H. K., Park, K. D., . . . Lee, H. W. (2013). Interaction of motor
training and intermittent theta burst stimulation in modulating motor cortical plasticity:
influence of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism. PLoS One, 8(2), e57690. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0057690
Li Voti, P., Conte, A., Suppa, A., Iezzi, E., Bologna, M., Aniello, M. S., . . . Berardelli, A. (2011). Correlation
between cortical plasticity, motor learning and BDNF genotype in healthy subjects. Exp Brain
Res, 212(1), 91-99. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2700-5
Liu, Q. R., Walther, D., Drgon, T., Polesskaya, O., Lesnick, T. G., Strain, K. J., . . . Uhl, G. R. (2005). Human
brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) genes, splicing patterns, and assessments of
associations with substance abuse and Parkinson's Disease. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr
Genet, 134B(1), 93-103. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30109
McHughen, S. A., & Cramer, S. C. (2013). The BDNF val(66)met polymorphism is not related to motor
function or short-term cortical plasticity in elderly subjects. Brain Res, 1495, 1-10. doi:
10.1016/j.brainres.2012.12.004

er

Pe

ew

vi

Re

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 15 of 21

Motor Control

15

r
Fo

McHughen, S. A., Rodriguez, P. F., Kleim, J. A., Kleim, E. D., Marchal Crespo, L., Procaccio, V., & Cramer, S.
C. (2010). BDNF val66met polymorphism influences motor system function in the human brain.
Cereb Cortex, 20(5), 1254-1262. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp189
Miller, S. A., Dykes, D. D., & Polesky, H. F. (1988). A simple salting out procedure for extracting DNA from
human nucleated cells. Nucleic Acids Res, 16(3), 1215.
Morales, L. C., Arboleda, G., Rodriguez, Y., Forero, D. A., Ramirez, N., Yunis, J. J., & Arboleda, H. (2009).
Absence of Lamin A/C gene mutations in four Wiedemann-Rautenstrauch syndrome patients.
Am J Med Genet A, 149A(12), 2695-2699. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.33090
Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test
Battery. J Neurosci Methods, 222, 250-259. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024
Ness, V., Arning, L., Niesert, H. E., Stuttgen, M. C., Epplen, J. T., & Beste, C. (2011). Variations in the
GRIN2B gene are associated with risky decision-making. Neuropharmacology, 61(5-6), 950-956.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.06.023
Ojeda, D. A., Lopez-Leon, S., & Forero, D. A. (2014). A novel cost-effective assay based on real-time PCR
for
COMT
Val158Met
genotyping.
Biomarkers,
19(7),
567-570.
doi:
10.3109/1354750X.2014.948069
Ojeda, D. A., Nino, C. L., Lopez-Leon, S., Camargo, A., Adan, A., & Forero, D. A. (2014). A functional
polymorphism in the promoter region of MAOA gene is associated with daytime sleepiness in
healthy subjects. J Neurol Sci, 337(1-2), 176-179. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.12.005
Ojeda, D. A., Perea, C. S., Nino, C. L., Gutierrez, R. M., Lopez-Leon, S., Arboleda, H., . . . Forero, D. A.
(2013). A novel association of two non-synonymous polymorphisms in PER2 and PER3 genes with
specific
diurnal
preference
subscales.
Neurosci
Lett,
553,
52-56.
doi:
10.1016/j.neulet.2013.08.016
Perea, C. S., Nino, C. L., Lopez-Leon, S., Gutierrez, R., Ojeda, D., Arboleda, H., . . . Forero, D. A. (2014).
Study of a Functional Polymorphism in the PER3 Gene and Diurnal Preference in a Colombian
Sample. Open Neurol J, 8, 7-10. doi: 10.2174/1874205X01408010007
Piper, B. J. (2011). Age, handedness, and sex contribute to fine motor behavior in children. J Neurosci
Methods, 195(1), 88-91. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.11.018
Qin, L., Jing, D., Parauda, S., Carmel, J., Ratan, R. R., Lee, F. S., & Cho, S. (2014). An adaptive role for BDNF
Val66Met polymorphism in motor recovery in chronic stroke. J Neurosci, 34(7), 2493-2502. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4140-13.2014
Seow, Steven C. (2005). Information theoretic models of HCI: a comparison of the Hick-Hyman law and
Fitts' law. Human-Computer Interaction, 20(3), 315-352.
Sheikh, H. I., Hayden, E. P., Kryski, K. R., Smith, H. J., & Singh, S. M. (2010). Genotyping the BDNF rs6265
(val66met) polymorphism by one-step amplified refractory mutation system PCR. Psychiatr
Genet, 20(3), 109-112. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32833a2038
Smith, M. A., Ghazizadeh, A., & Shadmehr, R. (2006). Interacting adaptive processes with different
timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol, 4(6), e179. doi:
10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179
Smolders, R., Rijpkema, M., Franke, B., & Fernandez, G. (2012). BDNF Val66Met polymorphism interacts
with sex to influence bimanual motor control in healthy humans. Brain Behav, 2(6), 726-731. doi:
10.1002/brb3.83
Sole, X., Guino, E., Valls, J., Iniesta, R., & Moreno, V. (2006). SNPStats: a web tool for the analysis of
association studies. Bioinformatics, 22(15), 1928-1929. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btl268
Solis-Ortiz, S., Perez-Luque, E., Morado-Crespo, L., & Gutierrez-Munoz, M. (2010). Executive functions
and selective attention are favored in middle-aged healthy women carriers of the Val/Val
genotype of the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene: a behavioral genetic study. Behav Brain
Funct, 6, 67. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-67

er

Pe

ew

vi

Re

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 16 of 21

16

r
Fo

Strazisar, M., Cammaerts, S., van der Ven, K., Forero, D. A., Lenaerts, A. S., Nordin, A., . . . Del-Favero, J.
(2014). MIR137 variants identified in psychiatric patients affect synaptogenesis and neuronal
transmission gene sets. Mol Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.53
Trewartha, K. M., Garcia, A., Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2014). Fast but fleeting: adaptive motor
learning processes associated with aging and cognitive decline. J Neurosci, 34(40), 13411-13421.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1489-14.2014
Tumkaya, S., Karadag, F., Mueller, S. T., Ugurlu, T. T., Oguzhanoglu, N. K., Ozdel, O., . . . Bayraktutan, M.
(2013). Situation awareness in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res, 209(3), 579-588.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.009
Walecki, Piotr, Laso, Wojciech, Kunc, Marek, & Gorzelaczyk, Edward J. (2013). Analysis of tremor in
motor learning task. Bio-Algorithms and Med-Systems, 9(1), 45-52.
Wang, Y., Li, J., Chen, C., Chen, C., Zhu, B., Moysis, R. K., . . . Dong, Q. (2013). COMT rs4680 Met is not
always the 'smart allele': Val allele is associated with better working memory and larger
hippocampal volume in healthy Chinese. Genes Brain Behav, 12(3), 323-329. doi:
10.1111/gbb.12022
Witte, A. V., Kurten, J., Jansen, S., Schirmacher, A., Brand, E., Sommer, J., & Floel, A. (2012). Interaction of
BDNF and COMT polymorphisms on paired-associative stimulation-induced cortical plasticity. J
Neurosci, 32(13), 4553-4561. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6010-11.2012
Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev
Neurosci, 12(12), 739-751. doi: 10.1038/nrn3112
Zhang, Yunfeng, & Hornof, Anthony J. (2012). A Discrete Movement Model For Cursor Tracking Validated
in the Context of a Dual-Task Experiment. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting.

er

Pe

ew

vi

Re
Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 17 of 21

Motor Control

Table 1. Overview of published studies on allele-specific effects of BDNF Val66Met


and performance in fine-motor tasks

Country

Sample size

Age

Kleim, 2006

USA

26 (14 males)

18-29

McHughen,
2010a

USA

24 (14 males)

18-30

Finger tapping,
Pegboard, Pinch grip

No significant differences

McHughen,
2010b

USA

29 (18 males)

18-30

Driving-Based Motor
Learning Task

Val/Met subjects showed more


driving errors and poorer retention
over 4 days

Li Voti, 2011

Italy

38 (22 males)

28 +/- 5.6

No significant differences

Smolders,
2012

Netherlands

69 (35 males)

18-35

Digital versin of
Preilowskis Task

Joundi, 2012

Spain

r
Fo

Index Finger movements

42 (32 males)

20.6

Joystick-controlled
visuomotor adaptation
task

Cirillo, 2012

Australia

29 (17 males)

18-39

Freundlieb,
2012

Germany

38 (12 males)

22-27

Serial Reaction Time


Task

No significant differences

Witte, 2012

Germany

32 (0 males)

20-49

Serial Reaction Time


Task

No significant differences

73.3

Peg board, Finger


tapping, Pinch grip,
Speed of reaction time,
Driving-based motor
learning task

No significant differences in elderly


subjects

Finger tapping

No significant basal differences.


Met/Met subjects did not show
increase in performance after
intermittent theta burst stimulation

Association
No significant differences

Women that were Met carriers


showed a poorer performance on
difficult angles
Val/Met subjects showed slower
rates of adaptation to a visuomotor
deviation during initial learning and
for 24-h retention
No significant differences in motor
performance or motor learning

20-40

ew

82 (42 male)

vi

Korea

38 (14 males)

Index finger abduction


acceleration,
Visuomotor tracking task

Re

Lee, 2013

USA

Tests
Finger tapping,
Pegboard, Pinch grip

er

McHughen,
2013

Pe

Author, Year

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 18 of 21

Table 2. Association between BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and performance on


PEBL pursuit rotor task.

Time on Target a

p value b

Val/Val (G/G)
Val/Met and Met/Met (A/G and A/A)

128
39

8342.7 (260.4)
9439.0 (380.1)

0.03

r
Fo

Genotype Groups

Pe

a. Time on target (in milliseconds), presented as means (SE).

er

b. Linear regression test, correcting by sex and age, performed in SNPstats.

ew

vi

Re
Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 19 of 21

Motor Control

Figure 1. Overview of the PEBL Pursuit Rotor Test


Pursuit rotor test consists on following, with the mouse pointer of the computer, a small
red circle that rotates clockwise at a constant speed around a larger circle presented on
the computer screen. Performance scores for each individual were given by the total

r
Fo

time on target (in milliseconds).

er

Pe
ew

vi

Re
Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Motor Control

Page 20 of 21

r
Fo
er

Pe
ew

vi

Re
81x81mm (150 x 150 DPI)

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Page 21 of 21

Motor Control

r
Fo
er

Pe
vi

Re
ew

Figure S1. Scatter plot showing the distribution scores for the PEBL pursuit rotor task
(Time-on-target, in milliseconds) according to the BDNF Val66Met genotypes.

Human Kinetics, 1607 N Market St, Champaign, IL 61825

Potrebbero piacerti anche