Sei sulla pagina 1di 13

http://nro.sagepub.

com
The Neuroscientist
DOI: 10.1177/1073858407299290
2007; 13; 280 Neuroscientist
Robert S. Blumenfeld and Charan Ranganath
Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging
Prefrontal Cortex and Long-Term Memory Encoding: An Integrative Review of Findings from
http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/13/3/280
The online version of this article can be found at:
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
can be found at: The Neuroscientist Additional services and information for
http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:
http://nro.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions:
http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/13/3/280
SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms):
(this article cites 124 articles hosted on the Citations
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Memorizing phone numbers, remembering past conversa-
tions, finding where you left your keysthese are the
kinds of daily activities that depend on the ability to form
coherent episodic memories. Recent research in neu-
ropsychology and neuroimaging has highlighted the
importance of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in promoting
successful episodic long-term memory (LTM) formation,
although its precise role remains poorly understood. Here,
we will review this evidence and present a theoretical
framework for understanding how different regions of the
PFC contribute to episodic memory encoding. Central to
this framework is the idea that different regions in the PFC
implement different control processes that augment mem-
ory by enhancing or attenuating certain aspects of a par-
ticular item or event.
Our review will have three main sections. First, we will
begin by discussing results from studies of memory in
patients with focal PFC lesions that converge on the idea
that specific deficits in cognitive control underlie the LTM
impairments in PFC patients. Next, we will review results
from neuroimaging studies of cognitive control, which
suggest that these different cognitive control processes
are performed by distinct PFC regions. Finally, we will
review the neuroimaging literature on episodic memory
encoding and consider how our framework can help to
clarify the role of the PFC in LTM encoding.
Effects of Prefrontal Lesions
on LTM Encoding
Clinicians have long noted that focal prefrontal lesions in
humans produce subtle but noticeable memory deficits,
and this impression accords well with results from neu-
ropsychological studies (Milner 1962; Stuss and Benson
1986; Shimamura 1995; Ranganath and Knight 2003).
In general, patients with PFC lesions show impairments
on a wide range of memory tasks that tax executive con-
trol during encoding and retrieval. For instance, PFC
patients exhibit impaired performance on unconstrained
memory tests such as free-recall (Jetter and others 1986;
McAndrews and Milner 1991; Eslinger and Grattan 1994;
Stuss and others 1994; Moscovitch and Winocur 1995;
Wheeler and others 1995; Dimitrov and others 1999; see
Box 1). In contrast to healthy control participants, PFC
patients tend not to spontaneously cluster or group recall
output according to the semantic relationships within a
categorized word list (della Rocchetta 1986; Hirst and
Volpe 1988; della Rocchetta and Milner 1993; Stuss and
others 1994; Gershberg and Shimamura 1995). When pre-
sented with several study-recall blocks of the same word
list, PFC patients show less consistency of recall output
order from trial to trial (i.e., subjective organization; see
Box 1) compared to controls (Stuss and others 1994;
Gershberg and Shimamura 1995; Alexander and others
Prefrontal Cortex and Long-Term Memory
Encoding: An Integrative Review of Findings
from Neuropsychology and Neuroimaging
ROBERT S. BLUMENFELD and CHARAN RANGANATH
Center for Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University of California at Davis
Recent findings have led to a growing appreciation of the role of the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) in
episodic long-term memory (LTM). Here, the authors will review results from neuropsychological and neu-
roimaging studies of humans and present a framework to explain how different regions of the PFC con-
tribute to successful LTM formation. Central to this framework is the idea that different regions within the
PFC implement different control processes that augment memory by enhancing or attenuating memory for
certain aspects of a particular item or event. Evidence reviewed here suggests that ventrolateral regions of
the PFC contribute to the ability to select goal-relevant item information, and that this processing strength-
ens the representation of goal-relevant features of items during LTM encoding. Dorsolateral regions of the
PFC may contribute to the ability to organize multiple pieces of information in working memory, thereby
enhancing memory for associations among items in LTM. Thus, dorsolateral and ventrolateral regions of the
PFC may implement different control processes that support LTM formation in a complementary fashion.
NEUROSCIENTIST 13(3):280291, 2007. DOI: 10.1177/1073858407299290
KEY WORDS Working, Organization, Selection, Frontal, Lobes, Associative, Episodic, Memory
280 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
This work was supported by PHS grant 1R01MH068721.
Address correspondence to: Robert S. Blumenfeld, UC Davis Center
for Neuroscience, 1544 Newton Ct, Davis, CA 95616 (e-mail: roblume
@ucdavis.edu).
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007
Copyright 2007 Sage Publications
ISSN 1073-8584
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 281
2003). In addition, patients with PFC lesions exhibit
impairments on tests of source memory (Janowsky and
others 1989b), memory for temporal order (Milner and
others 1985; Shimamura and others 1990; Kesner and oth-
ers 1994), recency (Milner and others 1991), frequency
(Stanhope and others 1998), and associative learning
(Shimamura and others 1995; Swick and Knight 1996;
Dimitrov and others 1999). Furthermore, PFC patients
often lack insight into their own memory problems and fail
to spontaneously use common memory strategies (Hirst
and Volpe 1988; Janowsky and others 1989a; Moscovitch
and Melo 1997; Vilkki and others 1998).
In contrast to these deficits, patients with prefrontal
lesions can often perform at near-normal levels when
given structured encoding tasks or tests that do not tax
strategic retrieval processes. For instance, PFC patients
perform better at cued-recall compared to free-recall and
can perform similarly to control subjects on item recogni-
tion (Kesner and others 1994; Stuss and others 1994;
Swick and Knight 1996; Dimitrov and others 1999;
Alexander and others 2003; but see Wheeler and others
1995). Moreover, patients can show marked improve-
ments on a variety of recall measures if given sufficient
practice or environmental support at encoding or retrieval
(Hirst and Volpe 1988; della Rocchetta and Milner 1993;
Stuss and others 1994; Gershberg and Shimamura 1995).
In general, theoretical accounts have stressed that LTM
impairments associated with PFC damage arise as a con-
sequence of deficits in control processing rather than from
a primary deficit in memory storage (Ranganath and
Knight 2003). Some theories emphasize the role of the
PFC in selection processes that direct attention toward
goal-relevant information and task-appropriate responses
(for reviews, see Milner and others 1985; Stuss and
Benson 1986; Miller and Cohen 2001). Thus, memory
deficits may occur in patients with prefrontal lesions
because they are unable to select relevant information or
inhibit distracting or interfering items or responses during
encoding or retrieval (Luria 1966, 1973; Perret 1974;
Shimamura and others 1995). One finding consistent with
this account comes from a study of paired-associate learn-
ing (see Box 1) in patients with focal PFC lesions and
matched controls (Shimamura and others 1995). In this
study, participants learned a list of word pairs (A-B) and
then learned an overlapping list of word pairs (A-C)
across several trials. Recall success on the second list
required subjects to inhibit the well-learned A-B pairing
and select the appropriate A-C pairing. Consistent with
the selection account, patients with PFC lesions showed a
significant and disproportionate decrement in recall per-
formance between the first trial of A-B learning compared
to the first trial of A-C learning (i.e., a measure of proac-
tive interference). Furthermore, during cued recall for the
A-C list, PFC patients recalled fewer appropriate targets
and recalled significantly more words from the A-B list.
These findings are consistent with the idea that PFC
lesions induce difficulties in resolving competition
between items in memory, possibly due to impairments of
controlled selection processes.
Other theories emphasize the role of the PFC in guiding
spontaneous organization of information during the
encoding and retrieval of memories (Petrides and Milner
1982; Milner and others 1985; della Rocchetta and Milner
1993; Gershberg and Shimamura 1995). Specifically,
organization refers to memory strategies that emphasize
comparison or transformation of relations among items
Box 1. Memory Measures in Behavioral Research
In behavioral studies, episodic memory is usually assessed by testing memory for lists of items such
as words (Ebbinghaus 1885/1964). With this approach, memory can be tested in a number of ways.
One is free-recall, in which participants are simply asked to recall as many items as they can remem-
ber from the study list. Organizational strategies strongly impact recall performance, and this impact
can be measured by analyzing the characteristics of items that are recalled and the order in which
they are recalled. For example, if participants are given lists of categorically related words to memo-
rize, they tend to recall semantically related items together even if these items were distributed across
the study list. This phenomenon, called semantic clustering, can provide a quantitative index of organ-
ization in memory. A related phenomenon, subjective organization, is seen in studies in which the
same list is repeatedly studied and tested. In this situation, participants will tend to recall sets of items
in the same order across different recall trials. This tendency is thought to reflect the fact that partic-
ipants form associations between items during encoding, and subsequently make use of these asso-
ciations to guide retrieval (Sternberg and Tulving 1977).
Another paradigm used in many memory studies is paired-associate learning. In these studies,
pairs of items are studied together, and then memory for these associations is tested. For example,
memory for a studied word pair (A-B) can be tested via cued recall (A-?) or associative recognition (A-
B or A-D?). Relevant to the studies discussed in our review, learning of a previous association (A-B)
makes it more difficult to learn a new, overlapping association (A-C) (Barnes and Underwood 1959).
This phenomenon is called proactive interference, and it may reflect competition between the pre-
viously learned association and the new association. Accordingly, overcoming proactive interference
requires the engagement of controlled selection processes to resolve this competition.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
that are to be learned (Bower 1970; Hunt and Einstein
1981). Some common organizational strategies during
encoding include categorizing words in a list according to
semantic features (Hunt and Einstein 1981), imagining
two or more items interacting (Bower 1970), or forming a
sentence out of two or more words. Organization during
encoding does not facilitate LTM by enhancing features
of specific items in memory, but instead promotes mem-
ory for associations among items (Bower 1970; Begg
1979; McGee 1980). Accordingly, a number of measures
can be used to estimate the impact of organization on
LTM performance (see Box 1).
In support of the notion that PFC is critical for organi-
zation in memory, studies have demonstrated that patients
with prefrontal lesions do not spontaneously organize
information or make use of effective organizational strate-
gies during encoding or retrieval (Hirst and Volpe 1988;
della Rocchetta and Milner 1993; Stuss and others 1994;
Gershberg and Shimamura 1995; Alexander and others
2003) and that they show deficits on many memory meas-
ures that are sensitive to organization. For instance,
Gershberg and Shimamura (1995) compared patients with
relatively focal lesions to the dorsolateral PFC and healthy
controls on learning of lists of words that were either
semantically related or unrelated. Relative to controls,
patients were less likely to exhibit subjective organization
or semantic clustering (see Box 1) during recall of lists of
semantically related words. Interestingly, recall and clus-
tering performance increased for semantically related
word lists when patients were explicitly asked to make a
category judgment during encoding, or when they were
provided with the category names at test, or both. In con-
trast, healthy controls performed at similar levels regard-
less of whether they were given encoding instructions or
retrieval cues. This pattern of results suggests that patients
were capable of using semantic information to guide
encoding and retrieval, but they lacked the ability to spon-
taneously use semantic organizational strategies. In con-
trast, controls were spontaneously using organizational
strategies during encoding and/or retrieval. These findings,
and others (Hirst and Volpe 1988; della Rocchetta and
Milner 1993; Stuss and others 1994; Alexander and others
2003), are consistent with the idea that LTM deficits fol-
lowing prefrontal lesions may result from a failure to
organize information during encoding and capitalize on
organizational structure during retrieval.
Many researchers have suggested that both selection
and organizational processes depend on the functioning of
the PFC, and there are several studies that reported find-
ings consistent with both the selection and organizational
accounts (Hirst and Volpe 1988; Stuss and others 1994;
Gershberg and Shimamura 1995; Shimamura and others
1995; Alexander and others 2003). One question that can-
not be addressed by the neuropsychological evidence is
whether selection and organization depend upon the same
regions of the PFC, because these studies typically used
subject groups that have significant heterogeneity in
lesion size and location. There are hints in the extant neu-
ropsychological literature to suggest that patients with
deficits in subjective organization and recall clustering
tend to have damage to the dorsolateral PFC (Eslinger and
Grattan 1994; Gershberg and Shimamura 1995; Alexander
and others 2003; but see Stuss and others 1994).
Nonetheless, the neuropsychological evidence cannot con-
clusively resolve this issue because studies have typically
used patient groups that have significant heterogeneity in
lesion size and location.
Functional Neuroimaging of
Cognitive Control
Several functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
studies have found support for the notion that selection
mechanisms and organizational control mechanisms are
processed by different regions of the PFC (DEsposito,
Postle, Ballard, and others 1999; Postle and others 1999;
Petrides 2000a; Wagner, Maril, and others 2001; Glahn
and others 2002; for review, see Fletcher and Henson
2001). Specifically, neuroimaging studies of working
memory (WM) and cognitive control have suggested that
regions within the ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC: BA
,44,45,47,47/12) and regions in the dorsolateral PFC
(DLPFC: BA 9,46) may implement control processes that
support memory encoding in a complementary fashion.
VLPFC and Selection Processes
Results from neuroimaging studies of proactive interfer-
ence resolution (Jonides and Nee 2006), response inhi-
bition (Aron and others 2004), verb generation (Buckner
2003; Thompson-Schill and others 2005), and semantic
and nonsemantic classification (Wagner and Davachi
2001; Badre and others 2005) are consistent with the
idea that VLPFC might be particularly important for
implementing selection processes that direct attention
toward goal-relevant information or inhibit the influence
of irrelevant information on behavior.
Some support for this view has come from studies that
used the recent-probes task (Monsell 1978) to examine
the role of VLPFC in resolving proactive interference. In
this task, participants must maintain a set of four letters
over a short delay and subsequently evaluate whether a
test probe was in the memory set. Proactive interference
is manipulated by varying the familiarity of the probes
across trials. For instance, on some trials, the probe letter
is not in the current memory set but was in the memory
set on the previous trial. To perform accurately on these
recent negative trials, participants must inhibit the high
familiarity of the probe to make the correct negative
response. Imaging studies have consistently shown that
activation in the left mid-VLPFC (BA 44, 45; see Fig. 1)
is increased on these trials, relative to trials with nonre-
cent negative or positive probes (e.g., Jonides and others
1998; DEsposito, Postle, Jonides, and others 1999;
Bunge and others 2001; Mecklinger and others 2003;
e.g., Nelson and others 2003; Postle and Brush 2004;
Postle and others 2004; Badre and Wagner 2005; see
Jonides and Nee 2006 for review).
Another line of evidence consistent with the selection
account has come from fMRI studies of verb generation and
semantic classification (Buckner 2003; Thompson-Schill
and others 2005). For instance, one study (Thompson-
Schill and others 1997) examined PFC activity associated
282 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 283
with selection processing across three different semantic
tasks. Within each task, selection demands were varied
within each of these conditions by increasing the competi-
tion among response alternatives. For example, in the high
selection condition of the comparison task, participants had
to compare a target noun (e.g., tooth) to two highly related
probe nouns (e.g., tongue and bone) and decide which probe
was most similar to the cue along a specific dimension (i.e.,
color). However, in the low-selection condition, participants
could make this comparison based on global similarity
between the target and probe words. Across all three tasks,
a common region of the left mid-VLPFC (BA 44, 45) exhib-
ited increased activation during blocks of high-selection tri-
als, suggesting a role for this region in the selection of
semantic features of items.
Results from some fMRI studies have shown that more
activation in relatively more anterior and ventral regions of
the VLPFC (BA 47/12, 45; see Fig. 1) is enhanced during
selection of semantic information, whereas activation in
the posterior and dorsal VLPFC regions (BA 44, 6; see Fig.
1) is enhanced during processing of semantic, phonologi-
cal, or orthographic attributes of words (e.g., Poldrack and
others 1999; Bokde and others 2001; Roskies and others
2001; Wagner, Pare-Blagoev, and others 2001; Dobbins
and others 2002; McDermott and others 2003; Badre and
others 2005; Bunge and others 2005; Dobbins and Wagner
2005; but see Otten and Rugg 2001b and Gold and
Buckner 2002). Not all studies, however, have observed
this type of material-specific dissociation within the PFC
(e.g., Barde and Thompson-Schill 2002). An alternative
view that has been proposed is that the extent and location
of VLPFC activity depends on the degree of controlled
selection that must be engaged (Buckner 2003). Research
consistent with this notion has found that posterior regions
of the VLPFC show recruitment during selection tasks that
require less control and that the anterior regions of the
VLPFC are additionally recruited during selection tasks
that require considerably more control (Gold and Buckner
2002; Gold and others 2005; but see Badre and others
2005). Interestingly, some studies have found that the acti-
vation of VLPFC subregions are influenced both by mate-
rials and by the amount of controlled selection (Badre and
others 2005; Gold and others 2005). This suggests that
although both of these accounts have some explanatory
power, more research will be required to find the best way
to characterize the functions of VLPFC subregions.
DLPFC and Organization
Unlike VLPFC, DLPFC is not robustly recruited during
tasks that solely require selection of task-relevant informa-
tion. Instead, evidence from neuroimaging studies suggests
that DLPFC is involved in organizationthat is, the com-
parison or transformation of relationships among items that
are active in memory (DEsposito, Postle, Ballard, and oth-
ers 1999; Postle and others 1999; Wagner, Maril, and oth-
ers 2001; Barde and Thompson-Schill 2002; Glahn and
others 2002; Bor and others 2003; Veltman and others
2003; Cannon and others 2005; Blumenfeld and Ranganath
2006; Crone and others 2006; Mohr and others 2006). For
example, DLPFC activation is reported in manipulation
tasks that involve sequencing of information that is being
maintained in WM (DEsposito, Postle, Ballard, and others
1999; Postle and others 1999; Postle and DEsposito 1999;
Wagner, Maril, and others 2001; Blumenfeld and
Ranganath 2006) or monitoring of previous responses
when selecting a future response (Petrides 2000a, 2000b;
Stern and others 2000). DLPFC activation has also been
reported in studies of chunking (Bor and others 2003;
Bor and others 2004), the process by which multiple sepa-
rate pieces of information are organized into fewer units of
information (Miller and others 1960).
One study of verbal WM highlights the selective role
of DLPFC in organization (Postle and others 1999). In
this study, participants were scanned during three tasks
that required maintenance of verbal information across
an eight-second delay period. In two conditions, partici-
pants had to maintain the identity and the serial position
of either two (forward 2 trials) or five (forward 5 tri-
als) letters. In the third condition, participants were
prompted to rearrange a set of five letters in alphabetical
order and to maintain the identity and serial position of
the rearranged set of letters (alphabetize 5 trials).
Whereas forward 5 and forward 2 trials differed prima-
rily in terms of the amount of information to be main-
tained, alphabetize 5 and forward 5 trials differed
primarily in the kind of processing that was engaged.
Specifically, alphabetize 5 trials placed greater demands
on organizational processing, because these trials
required participants to use the alphabetize rule to
transform the serial order relationships among the items.
Results from this study showed that, for each subject,
DLPFC activation was increased during the delay period
of alphabetize 5 trials, as compared with forward 5 tri-
als. DLPFC activation in this study could not be attrib-
uted to WM maintenance demands, because it did not
Fig. 1. Subregions of the human prefrontal cortex (PFC).
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (BA 9, 46) is
shown in green, and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex
(VLPFC) (BA 44, 45, and 47/12) is shown in red. The blue
dashed line indicates the approximate borders between
the DLPFC and VLPFC. White dashed lines indicate the
approximate borders between the subregions of the
VLPFC.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
significantly differ between forward 2 and forward 5 tri-
als. This pattern of results has been replicated in other
studies with verbal (DEsposito, Postle, Ballard, and oth-
ers 1999; Wagner, Maril, and others 2001; Barde and
Thompson-Schill 2002; Blumenfeld and Ranganath
2006) and spatial (Glahn and others 2002; Mohr and oth-
ers 2006) stimuli, suggesting that DLPFC activation is
more specifically increased during tasks that require
organization of information that is active in WM.
Additional evidence for the organizational account of
DLPFC function comes from neuroimaging studies of
chunking (Bor and others 2003; Bor and others 2004).
For example, in one study, participants were scanned
while memorizing a sequence of four spatial locations
within a 4 4 grid of red squares (Bor and others 2003).
Critically, on some trials, the spatial arrays exhibited a
coherent structure, in that an element in the sequence
was either on the same column, row, or diagonal as the
element preceding it. The resulting sequences therefore
appeared geometrically regular and were more readily
organized into single shapes. In contrast, on other trials,
the elements within the sequence did not exhibit any
geometric structure. Behavioral performance was higher
on structured trials, presumably because participants
were able to exploit the structure to organize, or chunk,
the spatial sequences. Critically, DLPFC activation was
increased on structured, as compared to unstructured,
trials even though the structured task was easier. This
finding suggests that, rather than simply reflecting non-
specific demands related to task difficulty, DLPFC acti-
vation is more specifically sensitive to demands for
organizational processing.
Functional Neuroimaging of LTM Encoding
We began this review by summarizing findings indicating
that PFC lesions may impair selection and organization
processes that facilitate LTM encoding. Neuroimaging
studies of cognitive control have suggested that VLPFC is
recruited during tasks that require the selection of specific
item information, whereas DLPFC is additionally recruited
during tasks that require organizational processing. As we
will describe below, these findings may provide critical
insights into the ways in which lateral prefrontal regions
contribute to successful LTM formation.
Subsequent Memory Effects and the PFC
Event-related fMRI studies have investigated LTM
encoding by identifying subsequent memory or dif-
ference due to memory effects (see Paller and Wagner
2002 for review). In these studies, activation during
encoding of a particular item or set of items is analyzed
as a function of later memory success or failure. Regions
where activation is increased during encoding of items
that are subsequently remembered (relative to activation
during encoding of items that are subsequently forgot-
ten) are thought to play a role in promoting successful
LTM formation.
As shown in Figure 2 and Table 1, studies using
the subsequent memory paradigm have demonstrated
significant prefrontal involvement in LTM encoding.
Inspection of the spatial distribution of activation peaks
(or local maxima) from these studies also suggests
that the degree of involvement seems to differ between
different PFC subregions. The data provide overwhelm-
ing support for the notion that the VLPFC contributes
to LTM formation. Out of 150 local maxima associated
with subsequent memory within the PFC, 132 fall within
the VLPFC. Furthermore, all but two studies that
284 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
Fig. 2. Prefrontal activations reported in 37 fMRI studies
of long-term memory (LTM) formation. Each green dot
represents an activation peak in an analysis that reported
increased activation during encoding of items that were
subsequently remembered, as compared with items that
were subsequently forgotten (i.e., a subsequent memory
effect). Each red dot represents an activation peak in an
analysis that reported increased activation during encod-
ing of items that were subsequently forgotten, as com-
pared with items that were subsequently remembered
(i.e., a subsequent forgetting effect). Of the 150 local
maxima associated with subsequent memory, only 18 fall
within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (BA 46
and 9) compared with 132 in the ventrolateral prefrontal
cortex (VLPFC) (BA 6, 44, 45, and 47). Furthermore, 10 of
the 11 local maxima associated with subsequent forget-
ting fall within the DLPFC. L = left; R = right.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 285
Table 1. Prefrontal Activation in Studies of LTM Encoding
A. Remember > Forgotten Contrasts
Activated
Prefrontal
Study Study Description Regions
Baker and others 2001 Semantic (abstract) vs. structural (uppercase) VLPFC
Blumenfeld and Ranganath 2006 Organizational processing VLPFC, DLPFC
Brassen and others 2006 Item memory and free-recall VLPFC, DLPFC
Buckner and others 2001 Encoding during retrieval VLPFC
Chee and others 2002 Word frequency VLPFC
Clark and Wagner 2003 Novel word learning VLPFC
Davachi and others 2001 Maintenance vs. elaborative rehearsal VLPFC
Dolcos and others 2004 Arousal, valence, and emotion VLPFC
Erk and others 2003 Emotional processing VLPFC
Fletcher and others 2003 Levels of processing VLPFC
Garoff and others 2005 Specific vs. general encoding VLPFC, DLPFC
Henson and others 1999 Recollection and familiarity VLPFC
Jackson and Schacter 2004 Associative memory VLPFC
Johnson and others 2004 Refreshing DLPFC
Kensinger and Schacter 2005 Emotion and reality monitoring DLPFC, VLPFC
Kirchhoff and others 2000 Novelty and content dependency VLPFC
Macrae and others 2004 Self-referential processing VLPFC
Maril and others 2003 Feeling of knowing VLPFC
Morcom and others 2003 Effect of aging VLPFC
Otten and Rugg 2001a Content dependency VLPFC
Otten and others 2001 Levels of processing VLPFC
Otten and others 2002 Item vs. task processing VLPFC
Prince and others 2005 Associative memory VLPFC
Ranganath and others 2004 Recollection and familiarity VLPFC
Raye and others 2002 Refreshing DLPFC
Reber and others 2002 Encoding effort VLPFC
Reynolds and others 2004 Item vs. task processing VLPFC
Sergerie and others 2005 Emotional processing of faces VLPFC, DLPFC
Sommer and others 2005 Object-location associations VLPFC, DLPFC
Sperling and others 2003 Face-name associations VLPFC
Staresina and Davachi 2006 Associative memory and free recall VLPFC, DLPFC
Summerfield and others 2006 Face-house associative binding VLPFC, DLPFC
Uncapher and Rugg 2005 Memory durability VLPFC
Wagner and others 1998 Semantic processing VLPFC
Weis and others 2004 Temporal lobe & cerebellum during encoding VLPFC
de Zubicaray and others 2005 Word frequency and strength effects VLPFC
B. Forgotten > Remember Contrasts
Prefrontal
Study Study Description Regions
Clark and Wagner 2003 Novel word learning DLPFC
Daselaar and others 2004 Forgetting VLPFC, DLPFC
Kensinger and Schacter 2005 Emotion and reality monitoring DLPFC
Otten and Rugg 2001b Content dependency DLPFC
Wagner and others 1998 Semantic processing DLPFC
A, Studies that revealed local maxima in prefrontal regions where activity during encoding was greater for items that were
subsequently remembered than for items that were subsequently forgotten. B, Studies that revealed local maxima in pre-
frontal regions where activity during encoding was greater for items that were subsequently forgotten than for items that
were subsequently remembered. LTM = long-term memory; VLPFC = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; DLPFC = dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
reported subsequent memory effects reported local max-
ima within the VLPFC. Given that the ability to select
relevant item information is essential for many forms of
goal-directed cognitive processing, including memory
encoding, it makes sense that the VLPFC activity should
be strongly linked to memory encoding in a wide variety
of behavioral contexts. These findings will be discussed
in more detail below.
The imaging literature seems to tell a different story
about the DLPFC. Out of 150 local maxima throughout
the PFC, only 18 fall within the DLPFC. Furthermore,
some studies have reported that DLPFC activation was
increased during encoding of items that were subse-
quently forgotten, as compared with those that were sub-
sequently remembered (i.e., a subsequent forgetting
effect). These findings do not seem to support the notion
that the DLPFC contributes to LTM encoding. However,
a close analysis of the studies summarized in Table 1
reveals that the majority of imaging studies have used
encoding and retrieval tasks that deemphasize organiza-
tional processing. As described below, DLPFC activa-
tion should be associated with successful LTM encoding
specifically under encoding and/or retrieval conditions
that emphasize organization.
VLPFC and Selection during LTM Encoding
As shown in Figure 2, there is overwhelming support for
the notion that the VLPFC supports LTM encoding.
Behavioral research suggests that engaging selection
processes at the time of encoding can strongly impact the
strength or distinctiveness of a memory for the relevant
item (Craik and Lockhart 1972; Hunt and Einstein 1981).
For instance, rehearsing an item (Naveh-Benjamin and
Jonides 1984; Greene 1987; Davachi and others 2001;
Raye and others 2002; Ranganath and others 2005) or
attending to its high-level or semantic features (Craik and
Lockhart 1972) can improve its subsequent memorability.
Given that most fMRI studies emphasize encoding and
retrieval of individual items, and that selection processes
figure so prominently in these contexts, it makes sense
that the VLPFC has been so consistently implicated in
successful memory formation.
As noted in the VLPFC and Selection Processes
section above, anterior regions of the VLPFC (BA
47/12, 45; see Fig. 1) have been implicated in tasks that
require high levels of control and/or selection of seman-
tic information (i.e., deep processing; cf. Craik and
Lockhart 1972), whereas more posterior regions of the
VLPFC (BA 44,6) have been implicated in tasks that
require less control during selection and/or selection of
phonological or orthographic information (i.e., shal-
low processing). Interestingly, some LTM encoding
studies have found corresponding patterns of subsequent
memory effects in the VLPFC. For instance, some stud-
ies have found that more anterior regions of the VLPFC
(BA 47/12, 45) showed subsequent memory effects for
items studied under deep encoding conditions (e.g.,
Wagner and others 1998; Baker and others 2001; Otten
and others 2001; Fletcher and others 2003; Ranganath
and others 2004), whereas more posterior regions of the
VLPFC (BA 44, 6) showed equivalent subsequent mem-
ory effects following both deep and shallow encoding
(Baker and others 2001; Fletcher and others 2003).
However, the literature is not consistent in this regard, as
several studies have failed to find such a dissociation in
the PFC (Baker and others 2001; Davachi and others
2001; Otten and others 2001; Otten and Rugg 2001a; but
see Fletcher and others 2003). Thus, more research is
needed to further characterize how VLPFC subregions
contribute to LTM encoding.
Until now, we have highlighted how selection processes,
putatively implemented by the VLPFC, may support suc-
cessful LTM formation in a very wide range of behav-
ioral contexts. However, there are some situations in
which selection processing may be deleterious to mem-
ory formation. For example, if selection processes direct
attention away from items that are to be encoded,
VLPFC activation can be negatively correlated with sub-
sequent memory performance (Otten and others 2002;
Reynolds and others 2004). In one such study, partici-
pants were scanned while performing several blocks of
encoding trials, and subsequent memory was tested out-
side of the scanner (Otten and others 2002). Consistent
with many previous studies, VLPFC activity was
increased during encoding of items that were later remem-
bered, as compared to those that were later forgotten.
However, in this report, a different analysis method was
used to examine sustained activity across each block of
trials that was not specifically related to item processing.
Interestingly, this sustained activity in the VLPFC across
each block of encoding trials was negatively correlated
with subsequent memory for the items in each block.
This suggests that when processing is directed away
from specific items, VLPFC activity will be negatively
correlated with subsequent memory performance.
DLPFC and Organization in LTM Encoding
As mentioned above, organizational processing during
encoding is thought to be an important determinant of
later memory (Bower 1970; Sternberg and Tulving 1977;
Hunt and Einstein 1981), and patients with lesions that
include the DLPFC are thought to have deficits in sponta-
neous usage of organizational strategies. Given that imag-
ing studies have implicated the DLPFC in control
processes that support organization in WM, it is surpris-
ing that few studies have reported DLPFC subsequent
memory effects and that some studies have reported sub-
sequent forgetting effects in the DLPFC. This pattern of
findings is consistent with at least two possibilities: 1) the
DLPFC implements processes that do not typically pro-
mote successful LTM formation or 2) most previous stud-
ies were insensitive to detect the way in which the DLPFC
contributes to LTM formation. Relevant to the latter pos-
sibility, most LTM encoding studies have used encoding
tasks that orient attention toward specific attributes of sin-
gle items and away from relationships between items. In
these studies, engaging in organizational processing dur-
ing encoding was irrelevant or possibly deleterious to later
memory performance (i.e., because allocating resources
toward processing the relationships among items might
286 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 287
take attentional resources away from processing the dis-
tinct features of the items themselves; Bower 1970; Begg
1979; McGee 1980). Accordingly, it is possible that the
encoding conditions in many previous imaging studies
were not conducive to revealing subsequent memory
effects in the DLPFC. The retrieval tests used in subse-
quent memory paradigms might also be a relevant factor.
Most imaging studies of encoding assess LTM with tests
of item recognition memory (see Box 1). However, organ-
ization facilitates memory by enhancing associations
among items (Bower 1970; Begg 1979; McGee 1980).
Thus, averaging encoding activation as a function of sub-
sequent item recognition performance might mask the
role of the DLPFC in successful LTM encoding.
If the DLPFC contributes to LTM encoding through its
role in organizational processing, the ability to detect this
contribution may depend on the kinds of encoding and
retrieval tasks that are used. Indeed, in studies that used
encoding tasks that encouraged formation of inter-item
associations (Sommer and others 2005; Summerfield and
Mangels 2005; Blumenfeld and Ranganath 2006), or
studies that used retrieval tests that are sensitive to mem-
ory for associations among items (Brassen and others
2006; Staresina and Davachi 2006), DLPFC activity dur-
ing encoding was positively correlated with subsequent
memory performance.
In one such study (Blumenfeld and Ranganath 2006),
we found evidence that DLPFC activation is related to
successful LTM encoding specifically under conditions
that emphasize organization. In this study, participants
were scanned while they performed two WM tasks: On
rehearse trials, participants were presented with a set of
three words and were required to maintain the set across
a 12-second delay period, in anticipation of a question
probing memory for the identity and serial position of the
items (see Fig. 3). On reorder trials, participants were
required to rearrange a set of three words based on the
weight of the object that each word referred to. They
maintained this information across a 12-second delay
period (see Fig. 3) in anticipation of a question probing
memory for serial order of the items in the rearranged set.
Although both rehearse and reorder trials required main-
tenance of the three-item set, reorder trials additionally
required participants to compare the items in the set and
transform their serial order. Consequently, reorder trials
required substantial organizational processing on each
Fig. 3. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity during working memory (WM) organization predicts successful
long-term memory (LTM) formation (Blumenfeld and Ranganath 2006). (a) Schematic depiction of the two tasks per-
formed during fMRI scanning. (b) Behavioral results, showing that participants recalled significantly more triplets from
each reorder trial (yellow) than would be expected based on the overall hit rate. This finding suggests that, on reorder
trials, memory performance was supported by associations among the items in the memory set. (c) fMRI data showing
that DLPFC (top) activation was increased during the delay period of reorder trials for which two to three items were
subsequently remembered (solid yellow), relative to trials in which zero to one items were remembered (dashed yellow).
No such effect is seen on rehearse trials (gray lines). At bottom, activation in a region of the posterior ventrolateral pre-
frontal cortex (pVLPFC) is plotted, showing that delay period activation in this region during both rehearse and reorder
trials was predictive of subsequent memory performance.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
288 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
trial, in addition to WM maintenance. Analyses of results
from a postscan recognition memory test showed that
there were significantly more reorder trials in which all
three items were recollected than would be expected
based on the overall item hit-rates alone (see Fig. 3). In
contrast, the proportion of rehearse trials on which all
three items were subsequently recollected was no differ-
ent than would be expected by the item hit-rates. These
findings suggest that active organization of the memory
set on reorder trials resulted in successful encoding of the
associations among the items in each set.
Consistent with the notion that the DLPFC is involved
in organization in WM, analyses of fMRI data revealed
that DLPFC activation was increased during reorder trials,
as compared with rehearse trials (Fig. 3). Furthermore,
DLPFC activation during reorder, but not rehearse, trials
was positively correlated with subsequent memory per-
formance. Specifically, DLPFC activation was increased
during the delay period of reorder trials for which two to
three items were later recollected, relative to trials for
which one or no items were later recollected. Critically,
no such relationship was evident during rehearse trials. In
contrast, activation in a posterior region of the left VLPFC
(BA 44, 6) was correlated with subsequent memory per-
formance on both rehearse and reorder trials. Thus, results
from this study suggest that DLPFC activation promotes
successful LTM formation through its role in organiza-
tional processing during encoding.
A study by Summerfield and colleagues (2006) pro-
vides further evidence for the role of the DLPFC in LTM
formation. In this study, participants learned associa-
tions between faces and houses by using a mnemonic
(he lives in this house). Results showed that DLPFC
and VLPFC activation was enhanced during encoding of
pairs that were subsequently remembered. In addition,
posterior regions thought to be involved in perception of
faces (the fusiform face area; Puce and others 1995;
Kanwisher and others 1997) and houses (the parahip-
pocampal place area; Aguirre and others 1998; Epstein
and Kanwisher 1998) showed subsequent memory
effects. Functional connectivity analyses demonstrated
that the correlation between the DLPFC, FFA, and the
PPA was increased on trials that led to successful mem-
ory for the face-house pairing. This suggests that the
DLPFC may promote memory for associations through
increased connectivity with regions that represent the
items that are to be associated.
Results from another recent study demonstrated the spe-
cific nature of DLPFC contributions to memory encoding
by comparing the relationship between activation and sub-
sequent performance on free recall and item recognition
memory tests (Staresina and Davachi 2006). As described
above, recognition tests are often insensitive to detecting
the presence of inter-item associations in LTM, whereas
recall performance is significantly influenced by organiza-
tion during encoding (see Box 1). Critically, DLPFC acti-
vation was specifically enhanced during encoding of items
that were recalled compared to those that were not.
DLPFC activation was not correlated with subsequent item
recognition performance. In contrast, encoding time acti-
vation in the VLPFC was positively correlated with subse-
quent memory performance on both the recall and the
recognition tests. These results are consistent with the idea
that DLPFC activation will contribute to subsequent mem-
ory performance specifically under retrieval conditions
that are sensitive to memory for associations among items.
Before concluding, it is important to address potential
challenges to our proposal that the DLPFC contributes to
LTM formation via its role in organization. A few studies
have reported increased DLPFC during processing of
items that were subsequently forgotten, relative to items
that were subsequently remembered. Importantly, many
of the studies that reported forgetting effects examined
encoding of single items and used encoding and retrieval
tasks that directed processing toward specific features of
the individual items (Otten and Rugg 2001b; Wagner and
Davachi 2001; Clark and Wagner 2003; Daselaar and oth-
ers 2004; Kensinger and Schacter 2005). As mentioned
above, these conditions deemphasize organizational pro-
cessing, which according to our proposal, will reduce the
contribution of the DLPFC in LTM formation. Moreover,
to the extent that organizational processing competes with
item encoding (Begg 1979; McGee 1980; but see Hockley
and Cristi 1996), DLPFC forgetting effects may reflect
task-irrelevant processing that interferes with successful
memory formation. Indeed, during item-specific encoding
conditions, organizational processing (and hence DLPFC
activity) may need to be inhibited to promote item mem-
ory (Daselaar and others 2004). Future research should
test this intriguing yet speculative proposal.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we have reviewed evidence suggesting
that different regions of the PFC implement cognitive
control processes that support successful LTM encoding.
Neuropsychological research has suggested that the PFC
supports LTM encoding by supporting controlled selec-
tion of goal-relevant item information as well as organi-
zation among items. Neuroimaging studies have
suggested that these cognitive control processes may
depend upon distinct regions of the PFC. For instance,
studies investigating resolution of proactive interference
and semantic categorization suggest that the VLPFC
may be disproportionately involved in controlled selec-
tion of item information. Studies investigating manipu-
lation, transformation, or chunking of multiple items in
WM provide evidence for the notion that the DLPFC
may be disproportionately involved in organizational
processing. Recent imaging studies suggest that the
roles of these prefrontal regions in cognitive control are
directly linked to their roles in LTM encoding. More
specifically, results from functional imaging studies
investigating LTM suggest that the VLPFC may support
LTM formation through its role in selecting relevant
item information, whereas the DLPFC may support
LTM by building associations among items that are
active in memory.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
References
Aguirre GK, Zarahn E, DEsposito M. 1998. Neural components of
topographical representation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:
83946.
Alexander MP, Stuss DT, Fansabedian N. 2003. California Verbal
Learning Test: performance by patients with focal frontal and non-
frontal lesions. Brain 126:1493503.
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. 2004. Inhibition and the right
inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 8:1707.
Badre D, Poldrack RA, Pare-Blagoev EJ, Insler RZ, Wagner AD. 2005.
Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mecha-
nisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 47:90718.
Badre D, Wagner AD. 2005. Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve
proactive interference. Cereb Cortex 15:200312.
Baker JT, Sanders AL, Maccotta L, Buckner RL. 2001. Neural corre-
lates of verbal memory encoding during semantic and structural
processing tasks. Neuroreport 12:12516.
Barde LH, Thompson-Schill SL. 2002. Models of functional organi-
zation of the lateral prefrontal cortex in verbal working memory:
evidence in favor of the process model. J Cogn Neurosci 14:
105463.
Barnes JM, Underwood BJ. 1959. Fate of first list associations in
transfer theory. J Exp Psychol 58:97105.
Begg I. 1979. Trace loss and the recognition failure of unrecalled
words. Mem Cognit 7:11323.
Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. 2006. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working
memory organization. J Neurosci 26:91625.
Bokde AL, Tagamets MA, Friedman RB, Horwitz B. 2001. Functional
interactions of the inferior frontal cortex during the processing of
words and word-like stimuli. Neuron 30:60917.
Bor D, Cumming N, Scott CE, Owen AM. 2004. Prefrontal cortical
involvement in verbal encoding strategies. Eur J Neurosci
19:336570.
Bor D, Duncan J, Wiseman RJ, Owen AM. 2003. Encoding strategies
dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand.
Neuron 37:3617.
Bower GH. 1970. Imagery as a relational organizer in associative
learning. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 9:52933.
Brassen S, Weber-Fahr W, Sommer T, Lehmbeck JT, Braus DF. 2006.
Hippocampal-prefrontal encoding activation predicts whether
words can be successfully recalled or only recognized. Behav
Brain Res 171:2718.
Buckner RL. 2003. Functional-anatomic correlates of control
processes in memory. J Neurosci 23:39994004.
Buckner RL, Wheeler ME, Sheridan MA. 2001. Encoding processes
during retrieval tasks. J Cogn Neurosci 13:40615.
Bunge SA, Ochsner KN, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. 2001.
Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of
mind. Brain 124:207486.
Bunge SA, Wendelken C, Badre D, Wagner AD. 2005. Analogical rea-
soning and prefrontal cortex: evidence for separable retrieval and
integration mechanisms. Cereb Cortex 15:23949.
Cannon TD, Glahn DC, Kim J, Van Erp TG, Karlsgodt K, Cohen MS,
and others. 2005. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during
maintenance and manipulation of information in working memory
in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:107180.
Chee MW, Westphal C, Goh J, Graham S, Song AW. 2003. Word fre-
quency and subsequent memory effects studied using event-related
fMRI. Neuroimage 20:1042-51.
Clark D, Wagner AD. 2003. Assembling and encoding word represen-
tations: fMRI subsequent memory effects implicate a role for
phonological control. Neuropsychologia 41:30417.
Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. 1972. Levels of processing: a framework for
memory research. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 12:599607.
Crone EA, Wendelken C, Donohue S, van Leijenhorst L, Bunge SA.
2006. Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate
information in working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
103:931520.
DEsposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. 1999. Maintenance ver-
sus manipulation of information held in working memory: an
event-related fMRI study. Brain Cogn 41:6686.
DEsposito M, Postle BR, Jonides J, Smith EE. 1999. The neural sub-
strate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working
memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 96:751419.
Daselaar SM, Prince SE, Cabeza R. 2004. When less means more:
deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory.
Neuroimage 23:9217.
Davachi L, Maril A, Wagner AD. 2001. When keeping in mind supports
later bringing to mind: neural markers of phonological rehearsal pre-
dict subsequent remembering. J Cogn Neurosci 13:105970.
de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL, Eastburn MM, Finnigan S,
Humphreys MS. 2005. fMRI evidence of word frequency and
strength effects during episodic memory encoding. Brain Res
Cogn Brain Res 22:43950.
della Rocchetta AI. 1986. Classification and recall of pictures after
unilateral frontal or temporal lobectomy. Cortex 22:189211.
della Rocchetta AI, Milner B. 1993. Strategic search and retrieval
inhibitionthe role of the frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia 31:
50324.
Dimitrov M, Granetz J, Peterson M, Hollnagel C, Alexander G,
Grafman J. 1999. Associative learning impairments in patients
with frontal lobe damage. Brain Cogn 41:21330.
Dobbins IG, Foley H, Schacter DL, Wagner AD. 2002. Executive con-
trol during episodic retrieval: multiple prefrontal processes sub-
serve source memory. Neuron 35:98996.
Dobbins IG, Wagner AD. 2005. Domain-general and domain-sensitive
prefrontal mechanisms for recollecting events and detecting nov-
elty. Cereb Cortex 15:176878.
Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. 2004. Dissociable effects of arousal and
valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and
subsequent memory: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage
23:6474.
Ebbinghaus H. 1885/1964. Memory: a contribution to experimental
psychology. New York: Dover.
Epstein R, Kanwisher N. 1998. A cortical representation of the local
visual environment. Nature 392:598601.
Erk S, Kiefer M, Grothe J, Wunderlich AP, Spitzer M. 2003. Emotional
context modulates subsequent memory effect. Neuroimage 18:
43947.
Eslinger PJ, Grattan LM. 1994. Altered serial position learning after
frontal lobe lesion. Neuropsychologia 32:72939.
Fletcher PC, Henson RN. 2001. Frontal lobes and human memory:
insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain 124:84981.
Fletcher PC, Stephenson CM, Carpenter TA, Donovan T, Bullmorel
ET. 2003. Regional brain activations predicting subsequent mem-
ory success: an event-related fMRI study of the influence of encod-
ing tasks. Cortex 39:100926.
Garoff RJ, Slotnick SD, Schacter DL. 2005. The neural origins of spe-
cific and general memory: the role of the fusiform cortex.
Neuropsychologia 43:84759.
Gershberg FB, Shimamura AP. 1995. Impaired use of organizational
strategies in free-recall following frontal lope damage.
Neuropsychologia 33:130533.
Glahn DC, Kim J, Cohen MS, Poutanen VP, Therman S, Bava S, and oth-
ers. 2002. Maintenance and manipulation in spatial working memory:
dissociations in the prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 17:20113.
Gold BT, Balota DA, Kirchhoff BA, Buckner RL. 2005. Common and
dissociable activation patterns associated with controlled semantic
and phonological processing: evidence from fMRI adaptation.
Cereb Cortex 15:143850.
Gold BT, Buckner RL. 2002. Common prefrontal regions coactivate
with dissociable posterior regions during controlled semantic and
phonological tasks. Neuron 35:80312.
Greene RL. 1987. Effects of maintenance rehearsal on human-memory.
Psychol Bull 102:40313.
Henson RNA, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ. 1999.
Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related
functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 19:
396272.
Hirst W, Volpe BT. 1988. Memory strategies with brain damage. Brain
Cogn 8:379408.
Hockley WE, Cristi C. 1996. Tests of encoding tradeoffs between item
and associative information. Mem Cognit 24:20216.
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 289
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
290 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Prefrontal Cortex and Memory Encoding
Hunt RR, Einstein GO. 1981. Relational and item-specific information
in memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 20:497514.
Jackson O 3rd, Schacter DL. 2004. Encoding activity in anterior
medial temporal lobe supports subsequent associative recognition.
Neuroimage 21:45662.
Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Squire LR. 1989a. Memory and
metamemory: comparisons between patients with frontal lobe
lesions and amnesic patients. Psychobiology 17:311.
Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Squire LR. 1989b. Source memory
impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia
27:104356.
Jetter W, Poser U, Freeman RB Jr, Markowitsch HJ. 1986. A verbal
long term memory deficit in frontal lobe damaged patients. Cortex
22:22942.
Johnson MK, Mitchell KJ, Raye CL, Greene EJ. 2004. An age-related
deficit in prefrontal cortical function associated with refreshing
information. Psychol Sci 15:12732.
Jonides J, Nee DE. 2006. Brain mechanisms of proactive interference
in working memory. Neuroscience 139:18193.
Jonides J, Smith EE, Marshuetz C, Koeppe RA, Reuter-Lorenz PA.
1998. Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain acti-
vation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:841013.
Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. 1997. The fusiform face area:
a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face percep-
tion. J Neurosci 17:430211.
Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. 2005. Emotional content and reality-
monitoring ability: fMRI evidence for the influences of encoding
processes. Neuropsychologia 43:142943.
Kesner RP, Hopkins RO, Fineman B. 1994. Item and order dissociation
in humans with prefrontal cortex damage. Neuropsychologia
32:88191.
Kirchhoff BA, Wagner AD, Maril A, Stern CE. 2000. Prefrontal-temporal
circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J Neurosci
20:617380.
Luria AR. 1966. Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic
Books.
Luria AR. 1973. The working brain. New York: Basic Books.
Macrae CN, Moran JM, Heatherton TF, Banfield JF, Kelley WM.
2004. Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cereb
Cortex 14:64754.
Maril A, Simons JS, Mitchell JP, Schwartz BL, Schacter DL. 2003.
Feeling-of-knowing in episodic memory: an event-related fMRI
study. Neuroimage 18:82736.
McAndrews MP, Milner B. 1991. The frontal cortex and memory for
temporal order. Neuropsychologia 29:84959.
McDermott KB, Petersen SE, Watson JM, Ojemann JG. 2003. A pro-
cedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention
to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic
resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41:293303.
McGee R. 1980. Imagery and recognition memory: the effects of rela-
tional organization. Mem Cognit 8:3949.
Mecklinger A, Weber K, Gunter TC, Engle RW. 2003. Dissociable
brain mechanisms for inhibitory control: effects of interference
content and working memory capacity. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res
18:2638.
Miller EK, Cohen JD. 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex
function. Annu Rev Neurosci 24:167202.
Miller GA, Galanter E, Pribram KH. 1960. Plans and the structure of
behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Milner B. 1962. Some cognitive effects of frontal lobe lesions in man.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 296:21126.
Milner B, Petrides M, Smith ML. 1985. Frontal lobes and the tempo-
ral organization of memory. Hum Neurobiol 4:13742.
Milner B, Corsi P, Leonard G. 1991. Frontal-lobe contribution to
recency judgements. Neuropsychologia 29:60118.
Mohr HM, Goebel R, Linden DE. 2006. Content- and task-specific
dissociations of frontal activity during maintenance and manipula-
tion in visual working memory. J Neurosci 26:446571.
Monsell S. 1978. Recency, immediate recognition, and reaction time.
Cogn Psychol 10:465501.
Morcom AM, Good CD, Frackowiak RS, Rugg MD. 2003. Age effects
on the neural correlates of successful memory encoding. Brain
126:21329.
Moscovitch M, Melo B. 1997. Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes:
evidence from confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsychologia
35:101734.
Moscovitch M, Winocur G. 1995. Frontal lobes, memory, and aging.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 769:11950.
Naveh-Benjamin M, Jonides J. 1984. Maintenance rehearsal: a two-
component analysis. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 10:36985.
Nelson JK, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Sylvester CY, Jonides J, Smith EE.
2003. Dissociable neural mechanisms underlying response-based
and familiarity-based conflict in working memory. Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 100:111715.
Otten LJ, Henson RN, Rugg MD. 2001. Depth of processing effects
on neural correlates of memory encoding: relationship between find-
ings from across- and within-task comparisons. Brain 124:399412.
Otten LJ, Henson RN, Rugg MD. 2002. State-related and item-related
neural correlates of successful memory encoding. Nat Neurosci
5:133944.
Otten LJ, Rugg MD. 2001a. Task-dependency of the neural correlates of
episodic encoding as measured by fMRI. Cereb Cortex 11:115060.
Otten LJ, Rugg MD. 2001b. When more means less: neural activity
related to unsuccessful memory encoding. Curr Biol 11:152830.
Paller KA, Wagner AD. 2002. Observing the transformation of experi-
ence into memory. Trends Cogn Sci 6:93102.
Perret E. 1974. The left frontal lobe and the suppression of habitual
responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia
12:32330.
Petrides M. 2000a. Dissociable roles of mid-dorsolateral prefrontal
and anterior inferotemporal cortex in visual working memory. J
Neurosci 20:7496503.
Petrides M. 2000b. The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
in working memory. Exp Brain Res 133:4454.
Petrides M, Milner B. 1982. Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal-
and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 20:24962.
Poldrack RA, Wagner AD, Prull MW, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli
JD. 1999. Functional specialization for semantic and phonological
processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 10:1535.
Postle BR, Berger JS, DEsposito M. 1999. Functional neuroanatomi-
cal double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control
processes contributing to working memory performance. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A 96:1295964.
Postle BR, Brush LN. 2004. The neural bases of the effects of item-
nonspecific proactive interference in working memory. Cogn
Affect Behav Neurosci 4:37992.
Postle BR, Brush LN, Nick AM. 2004. Prefrontal cortex and the medi-
ation of proactive interference in working memory. Cogn Affect
Behav Neurosci 4:6008.
Postle BR, DEsposito M. 1999. What-Then-Where in visual working
memory: an event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 11:58597.
Prince SE, Daselaar SM, Cabeza R. 2005. Neural correlates of rela-
tional memory: successful encoding and retrieval of semantic and
perceptual associations. J Neurosci 25:120310.
Puce A, Allison T, Gore JC, McCarthy G. 1995. Face-sensitive regions
in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. J
Neurophysiol 74:11929.
Ranganath C, Cohen MX, Brozinsky CJ. 2005. Working memory
maintenance contributes to long-term memory formation: neural
and behavioral evidence. J Cogn Neurosci 17:9941010.
Ranganath C, Knight RT. 2003. Prefrontal cortex and episodic mem-
ory: integrating findings from neuropsychology and event-related
functional neuroimaging. In: Parker A, Wilding EL, Bussey TJ,
editors. Memory encoding and retrieval: a cognitive neuroscience
perspective. New York: Psychology Press. p 8399.
Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, Cohen MX, Dy CJ, Tom SM, DEsposito
M. 2004. Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity
within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 42:213.
Raye CL, Johnson MK, Mitchell KJ, Reeder JA, Greene EJ. 2002.
Neuroimaging a single thought: dorsolateral PFC activity associated
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from
Volume 13, Number 3, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 291
with refreshing just-activated information. Neuroimage 15:
44753.
Reber PJ, Wong EC, Buxton RB. 2002. Encoding activity in the medial
temporal lobe examined with anatomically constrained fMRI analy-
sis. Hippocampus 12:36376.
Reynolds JR, Donaldson DI, Wagner AD, Braver TS. 2004. Item- and
task-level processes in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: positive
and negative correlates of encoding. Neuroimage 21:147283.
Roskies AL, Fiez JA, Balota DA, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. 2001.
Task-dependent modulation of regions in the left inferior frontal
cortex during semantic processing. J Cogn Neurosci 13:82943.
Sergerie K, Lepage M, Armony JL. 2005. A face to remember: emo-
tional expression modulates prefrontal activity during memory for-
mation. Neuroimage 24:5805.
Shimamura AP. 1995. Memory and the prefrontal cortex. Ann N Y
Acad Sci 769:1519.
Shimamura AP, Janowsky JS, Squire LR. 1990. Memory for the tempo-
ral order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic
patients. Neuropsychologia 28:80313.
Shimamura AP, Jurica PJ, Mangels JA, Gershberg FB, Knight RT.
1995. Susceptibility to memory interference effects following
frontal lobe damage: findings from tests of paired-associate learn-
ing. J Cogn Neurosci 7:14452.
Sommer T, Rose M, Weiller C, Bchel C. 2005. Contributions of
occipital, parietal and parahippocampal cortex to encoding of
object-location associations. Neuropsychologia 43:73243.
Sperling R, Chua E, Cocchiarella A, Rand-Giovannetti E, Poldrack R,
Schacter DL, and others. 2003. Putting names to faces: successful
encoding of associative memories activates the anterior hippocam-
pal formation. Neuroimage 20:140010.
Stanhope N, Guinan E, Kopelman MD. 1998. Frequency judgements
of abstract designs by patients with diencephalic, temporal lobe or
frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 36:138796.
Staresina BP, Davachi L. 2006. Differential encoding mechanisms for
subsequent associative recognition and free recall. J Neurosci
26:916272.
Stern CE, Owen AM, Tracey I, Look RB, Rosen BR, Petrides M. 2000.
Activity in ventrolateral and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dur-
ing nonspatial visual working memory processing: evidence from
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 11:3929.
Sternberg RJ, Tulving E. 1977. Measurement of subjective organiza-
tion in free-recall. Psychol Bull 84:53956.
Stuss D, Benson DF, eds. 1986. The frontal lobes. New York: Raven.
Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Palumbo CL, Buckle L, Sayer L, Pogue J. 1994.
Organizational strategies of patients with unilateral or bilateral frontal
lobe injury in word list learning tasks. Neuropsychology 8:35573.
Summerfield C, Greene M, Wager T, Egner T, Hirsch J, Mangels J.
2006. Neocortical connectivity during episodic memory formation.
PLoS Biol 4:e128.
Summerfield C, Mangels JA. 2005. Functional coupling between
frontal and parietal lobes during recognition memory. Neuroreport
16:11722.
Swick D, Knight RT. 1996. Is prefrontal cortex involved in cued recall?
A neuropsychological test of PET findings. Neuropsychologia
34:101928.
Thompson-Schill SL, Bedny M, Goldberg RF. 2005. The frontal lobes
and the regulation of mental activity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:
21924.
Thompson-Schill SL, DEsposito M, Aguirre GK, Farah MJ. 1997.
Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic
knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:147927.
Uncapher MR, Rugg MD. 2005. Encoding and the durability of
episodic memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
J Neurosci 25:72607.
Veltman DJ, Rombouts SA, Dolan RJ. 2003. Maintenance versus
manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage 18:24756.
Vilkki J, Servo A, Surma-aho O. 1998. Word list learning and prediction
of recall after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology 12:26877.
Wagner AD, Davachi L. 2001. Cognitive neuroscience: forgetting of
things past. Curr Biol 11:R9647.
Wagner AD, Maril A, Bjork RA, Schacter DL. 2001. Prefrontal con-
tributions to executive control: fMRI evidence for functional dis-
tinctions within lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 14:133747.
Wagner AD, Pare-Blagoev EJ, Clark J, Poldrack RA. 2001.
Recovering meaning: left prefrontal cortex guides controlled
semantic retrieval. Neuron 31:32938.
Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM,
and others. 1998. Building memories: remembering and forgetting
of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281:
118891.
Weis S, Klaver P, Reul J, Elger CE, Fernandez G. 2004. Temporal and
cerebellar brain regions that support both declarative memory for-
mation and retrieval. Cereb Cortex 14:25667.
Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. 1995. Frontal lobe damage produces
episodic memory impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1:52536.
2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
by Favio Vega on November 18, 2007 http://nro.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Potrebbero piacerti anche