Sei sulla pagina 1di 18

The inhibitory role of top-down alpha-band

oscillations in the working memory context



State of the art essay (II) MSc in Neuroimaging

Mario B. Perez

4/4/2014






State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
1


The inhibitory role of top-down alpha-band oscillations in
the working memory context






Index


I. Introduction : Establishing inhibition



II. The roles of alpha inhibitory activity: Enabling, suppressing and rejecting

- Top-down alpha during pre-encoding, stimulus onset, and encoding of items
- Dynamic encoding: Selective processing as a result of instructions
- Inhibiting task-irrelevant areas: Default or on-demand inhibition?
- Alpha inhibition during maintenance: Rejecting distractors and keeping items
uncorrupted.





III. Critical considerations & Future Directions

- Cueing and management of temporal expectations
- Setting up tasks: the importance of tasks properties
- Detaching roles and measuring sources
- Future directions; the interplay between phase and amplitude









IV. Conclusions


V. References








State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
2


I. Introduction : Establishing inhibition
Alpha oscillatory activity was the firstly discovered (Millet, 2001) and is the most prominent
neural rhythm recorded in the human brain (Klimesch et al, 1999). Despite his remarkably antique
origin, the number and extent of its competencies is object of a lively and vigorous discussion, and the
study of alpha oscillatory activity has gained great popularity over the last decade (Payne and
Sekuler, 2013).
As it is widely known, alpha oscillations were initially thought to reflect cortical idling (Adrian
and Matthews, 1934). However, this view was later began to be questioned (Pfurtscheller et al, 1996),
and some authors reported the appearance of alpha activity during internal processing which were
supposed to not to require attention from the environment (Ray and Cole, 1985), reflecting the first
observations of alpha activity over disengaged areas. Posterior research by Pfurtscheller (2001),
Klimesch (1996) and many others described in more detail the appearance of alpha rhythm event-
related desynchronization (ERD) in occipital areas during visual information processing, whereas
event-related synchronization (ERS) appears in inactive areas.
The current stage in literature was in part established by a second series of articles which
started to talk about these relations in a generalized way, and which benefited from the accumulated
evidence (e.g Jensen et al, 2002; Sauseng et al 2005) in support of alpha oscillatory activity as a
mechanism to engage and disengage areas from activity (Klimesch et al, 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri,
2010). In particular, the landmark study by Klimesch et al (2007) pointed out that upper-alpha band
frequency power disappearance is particularly coupled with active cognitive processing. Additionally,
both articles stressed that alpha activity can be modulated in a top-down fashion, representing
functional inhibition.
This alpha inhibitory framework has posteriorly gained substantial empirical support which
has increased over the last years (Hanslmayr et al., 2007; van Dijk et al., 2008; Sauseng et al, 2009;
Handel et al 2011; Haegens et al 2011 & 2012; Roux et al, 2012; Klimesch et al, 2012). Many of these
studies used working memory paradigms (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010), showing how alpha activity
disappeared in engaged areas and reinforced in pertinent ones, or suddenly reappeared when task-
irrelevant stimuli appeared.
However, although evidence appears to be extensive, literature regarding alpha oscillations
has been in occasions chaotic and unclear in the range of processes in which alpha inhibition takes
place. It is frequent to find a lack of agreement in to what extent this phenomenon is present in
working memory or attentional tasks (Palva and Palva, 2007). This may be in part caused by the lack
of replicability in some aspects of the framework described by Klimesch (2007), perhaps due to a
certain tendency in finding new contexts and applications instead of securing the grounds.
In the following sections we will try to present a coherent and fluid view of the alpha inhibitory
role in a WM environment. WM tasks normally follow a structured set of processes; the information
needs to be adequately perceived and encoded, then the internal copy of the items presented needs
to be maintained and finally these items will be used for a comparison or perhaps a manipulation task
(Haenschel and Linden, 2011). The relatively developed stage of WM theory (Baddeley,2003) and its
position as binding point between perceptual and cognitive domains makes of WM an excellent
framework to make the most of EEG and MEG measurements, and current electrophysiological
models of WM are being developed (Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014).

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
3

Apart from its theoretical usability, WM proves to be worth of studying as its impairment have
been related with deficits in inhibitory function (Park and Reuter-Lorentz 2009) and failures in
inhibition are considered to be key symptoms of Alzheimers disease (Amieva et al, 2004; Belleville et
al 2006) schizophrenia (Haenschel and Linden, 2011) or even related to normal ageing (Zanto and
Gazzaley, 2009; Belanger et al, 2010).

II. The roles of alpha inhibitory activity: Enabling, suppressing and
rejecting
Once this general inhibitory character of alpha activity has been established, its significance
can vary in function of when these oscillations appear along the timeline (see graph below) and in
which localizations, as these sites may be differently relevant for the WM task (Klimesh et al, 2007).
Hence, alpha power modulations might reflect enabling/disabling of processing at the beginning of a
task as a product of cueing (pre-stimulus, Klimesch et al, 2007 or Payne and Sekuler, 2013), selective
suppression of processing in situations in which irrelevant and relevant information appear together
(dynamic encoding, e.g Freunberger, 2009 or Horschig et al, 2014) or perhaps protection of the held
items against distractors in the maintenance phase (rejection of intruders or distractors, Bonnefond
and Jensen, 2012).

Figure 1: The below graph provides an overview of the following sections. Image (left) from Payne and Sekuler
(2013), which provides some examples of pre-stimulus alpha power modulation in somatosensory, ortographic, auditory or
visual tasks. Image (right) from Freunberger et al, (2009) will be discussed in the following. Flow graph created by author.
Pre-encoding
Enabling/disabling
Klimesch et al 2007
Stimulus
onset
Readiness
Payne and Sekuler,
2013
Encoding
Selective
Processing
Freunberger et al
2009
Retention
Rejecting
intruders
Bonnefond and Jensen 2012
Timeline
CUEING Looking at alpha inhibition in the WM context (Sternberg)

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
4




So far, the vast majority of up to date literature has considered alpha power or alpha
amplitude as default elements (Palva and Palva, 2007) and we will do so as well. Therefore, unless
specified we will be always referring to alpha activity as amplitude modulation, although in the closing
sections we will pay attention to other components such as phase coupling or locking.

Top-down alpha during pre-encoding, stimulus onset, and encoding of items


Enhancement of alpha activity over task-relevant areas may appear at the beginning of a task
when the appearance of an irrelevant item is announced by cueing (Rohenkohl and Nobre,2012;
Payne and Sekuler, 2013 for a good review) and it is not required to be processed, normally known as
suppression of processing by alpha ERS. Conversely, alpha power can drop as a result of cued
relevant items appearance, therefore enabling processing through alpha ERD in relevant places for
the task (Klimesch et al, 2007).
This modulatory role extends from pre-encoding to encoding phases, as it can happen either
in the middle of a task or at the beginning of a task when explicit cueing is provided. More importantly,
this process reflects the control of the subject upon its attentional resources (Wilsch et al, 2014) and
can be understood as an electrophysiological correlate of attentional top-down management
(Klimesch, 2012). Impairments in this process would entail deficiencies in WM tasks as suppression is
a key skill in working memory functioning (BelanVogel et al 2005). Clear evidence of this role can be
found at Haegens et al (2011) where a correlation between pre-stimulus alpha suppression and
performance in a somatosensory discriminatory were found in the somatosensory cortex for the
attended side, in which stimuli appearance was expected. As Payne and Sekuler (2013) state, correct
discrimination increases as pre-stimulus alpha decreases, and accuracy in the task and speed of
response were both correlated with the alpha degree. Additionally, alpha presence varied when the
reliability of the cueing was modified, suggesting a strong top-down modulation.

More sophisticated evidence of cueings and temporal expectations relevance can be found at
the study by Wilsch et al (2014) in which three different types of cueing were used (early,
neutral/uninformative and late) in an auditory delayed-to-match working memory task. This task
consisted in the comparison of two syllables, identifying if the first consonant used was the same in
both cases or different. Items were embedded in a sort of background noise, providing a complex
presentation, perhaps resembling a conversation in a noisy environment.
The authors successfully reasoned that the noise would increase the need of functional
inhibition, thus boosting the presence of alpha rhythm. Critically, temporal expectancy manipulated by
cueing effectively modulated alpha power, which strategically dropped when the task-relevant
information (syllable) was about to be presented knowing when to listen (figures A and B in the
below illustrations). Trials in which a long cueing was provided revealed the higher drops in alpha
inhibition and yielded the best performances (also in Mazaheri et al,2010 and Hanslmayr et al, 2007,
or Obleser and Weisz, 2012 for an auditory example) Moreover, and as can be seen in the illustration
below (figure C), alpha modulations (defined by the difference in power between neutral and cued)
predicted accuracy in the task.


State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
5



Figure 2: Wilsch et al, 2014.



Figure 3: Wilsch et al, 2014.


Furthermore, Sauseng et al (2009) conducted an experiment using a WM task which
consisted in two ensembles of differently coloured squares (varying from two to six), one in each hemi
field and which colours have to be memorized . A cue informed the participant of which of the two
arrays have to be ignored and which one has to be remembered. The authors reasoned that if alpha
activity is related with the suppression of irrelevant stimuli's processing, and successful ignoring of
irrelevant information is required for efficient WM performance, then lateralized alpha (difference of
alpha amplitude between hemispheres) must grow as the amount of irrelevant information does.
Illustration B below shows how amplitude increases in the ipsilateral hemisphere to relevant
information while decreasing in the contralateral one (processing relevant items). Illustration C
shows how lateralized alpha predicts capacity to retain items based on the capacity to ignore
irrelevant information as alpha grows. Handel et al (2011) and Mathewson et al (2009) have found
similar results relating successful ignoring and performance.

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
6





Figure 4: Sauseng et al, 2009

Figure 5 Sauseng et al, 2009


Additionally, Sauseng et al (2009) used rTMS in an attempt to boost retention capacity by
inducing alpha (10Hz) activity over the task-irrelevant sites. They also applied entrained alpha activity
over task-relevant locations. Results showed a significant increase of short memory capacity when
rTMS was applied over task-irrelevant areas, while a decrease took part when applied over task-
relevant ones (maintaining items). Cleverly, authors delivered also activity of a diverse range of
frequencies, finding these effects specific for induced alpha rhythm, as can be seen in the illustration
below. Enhancement in memory capacity for items was specific of alpha rhythm at 10Hz. Romei et al
(2010) found similar results using TMS while inducing impairment for stimulis detection using
entrainment of alpha waves in an attentional task, inducing blindness to targets when oscillations
were delivered over task-irrelevant visual areas.


Figure 6: Sauseng et al, 2009

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
7


Dynamic encoding: Selective encoding and cognitive cherry picking as result of
instructions

In a logic extension of the previous role, alpha activity suppression of processing can also be
observed in more dynamic, fast-changing environments taking advantage of MEG/EEG temporal
resolution. The main difference with the previous section is the intermixed, alternative manner in
which the relevant/irrelevant information is presented. In this sense, alpha activity plays a critical role
in successful encoding (Rihs et al 2007; Haegens et al 2010 ; Zanto et al, 2011 ;Haegens et al 2011
;Roux et al 2012; Payne et al 2013 ;Horschig et al 2014). Also, task-relevant and task-irrelevant items
might appear according some instructions given at the beginning and without one-to-one cueing, or
perhaps without explicit cueing at all (Horschig et al,2014). Relevantly, speed in discrimination has
been related with alpha activity (vanRullen et al, 2011).
Irrelevant information in a constantly changing environment can be presented in a very
diverse sort of situations and under different mixes with relevant items. Therefore, alpha activity has
to adapt to the perceptual scene For example, irrelevant information can appear in combination with
relevant information but segregated in space (visually, Sauseng et al, 2009,; tactile presentation in
different points, Haegens et al, 2012) or in the same spatial location but segregated in time
(Freunberger et al, 2009; Payne et al, 2013). It can also be the case that two different features are
present in the same stimuli/items, but only one of them is relevant for further processing, making the
inhibition work only on some aspects of what looks a unitary scene (Snyder and Foxe, 2010 e.g
remember colours and not shapes).Finally, items can appear in different sensorial modalities at the
same time (as exemplified in Klimesch et al, 2012). All of these situations represent a challenge while
picking the important information and forgetting or not processing information. This cognitive cherry
picking selects the relevant information out from the stimuli stream, and it is key any further cognitive
functions. Alpha is therefore related with the internal control of attention once again (Klimesch et al,
2007). Given the remarkably storage-limited capacity of WM (Cowan, 2010), this suppression
character of alpha activity acquires a critical importance, and efficient performance at WM tasks has
been directly related with the successful inhibition of distracting stimuli (Gazzaley and Nobre,2012;
Freunberger et al, 2011). Intrusion of task-irrelevant information is a major cause of failures in WM
(Hasher and Zacks,1988 as quoted in Freunberger et al, 2009; Zanto and Gazzaley, 2009).
Freunberger et al (2009) designed an experiment in the context of a Sternberg WM task. In
this study, a set of 16 items per trial were presented to participants, 8 of them have to be
remembered. After this trial, a probing item was displayed and the subject should indicate if it is an old
(previously presented) or new (not previously presented) item. Activity of alpha waves were expected
in parieto-occipital sites, supposedly related with blocking/enabling memory processing.
Enhancement of alpha power especially of in its upper frequencies was visible during to-be-
remembered items exposition, while functional inhibition took place during the presentation of
irrelevant items evidencing a strong top-down regulation (Leiberg et al, 2006, found similar results for
auditory Sternberg task) the illustration below shows this last effect.
State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
8


Figure 7: Freunberger et al, 2009

In a more complex way, the remarkable study by Horschig et al (2014) holds an special
importance among the alpha modulatory activity literature. In this experiment the subjects were
exposed to a task in which the to-be-attended hemi field will change without explicit cueing of it.
However, with each trial in which the attended hemi field remained the same the implicit probabilities
of a change were higher, so the subjects were dedicating an increasing amount of covert attention to
the opposite hemi field. This top-down covert attention was operatized by the amount of anticipatory
posterior alpha, which demonstrated to be correlated with performance.

Inhibiting task-irrelevant areas: Default or on-demand inhibition?

The definition of what it is a task-irrelevant area is slippery. A task-irrelevant area can be so
because the stimulus that is being presented in the perceptual scene is non-pertinent to the task, and
thus, its processing must by inhibited. As an example of this apart from the Sauseng et al (2009),
Haegens et al (2012) showed how subjects who performed better in a somatosensory WM task and
were less affected by the distractors described stronger alpha power ipsilateral to the side in which
relevant stimulation is presented.
However, task-irrelevant areas are also all those locations in which processing is not
expected to happen. Alpha power in these areas has been object of controversy, as some authors
consider that such is only possible as a result of a distractors appearance, or in case that those areas
are inhibited by default, like if the mere spatial location will be a distractor itself (Klimesch et al, 2007;
Payne et al 2013). In this line, Klimesch et al (2012) state that alpha ERS in irrelevant areas reflect
protection against potential interfering processes.
Worden et al (2000, as quoted in Horschig et al, 2014) has nonetheless demonstrated the
incensement in irrelevant areas and the decrease in relevant ones without presence of distractors.
Additionally, Haegens et al. (2010) described that alpha activity over occipital sites during the
retention period of a somatosensory WM task was observed and correlated with performance,
constituting initial evidence for the necessity of alpha waves over task irrelevant locations in
distractors absence (see illustration below).

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
9

Tuladhar et al (2007) and Scheeringa et al in a joint EEG/fMRI study (2009) have shown the
same pattern of alpha activity over posterior sites during WM maintenance, and alpha during the
retention period and without the presence of an obvious distractor has been correlated with the
number of items to be remembered (Jensen et al, 2002; Sauseng et al, 2009; Wilsch et al 2014).

Figure 8: Haegens et al, 2010

Some researchers have attached to this activity an additional role, ensuring the correct
distribution of resources (Tuladhar et al, 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Payne and Sekuler,
2013). Additionally, Jensen and Mazaheri (2010) proposed a model of brain communication and
information routing based on the presence of these oscillations, which in theory must help to distribute
the information across different areas and subsystems. Evidence in support of this model can at best
be qualified of circumstantial.

Alpha inhibitory activity during Maintenance Rejecting distractors and
keeping items uncorrupted.

As was mentioned in the previous section, alpha activity has been also observed during WM
retention and maintenance (Jensen et al 2002; Jokisch and Jensen, 2007; Haegens et al, 2010;
Bonnefond and Jensen 2012; Payne et al 2013 are some examples). Although there is some debate
concerning the role that alpha might be accomplishing, that we will examine in the last sections, there
are some good examples of the relevance of alpha oscillations in this stage.
Thus, Bonnefond and Jensen (2012) used the Sternberg WM memory task paradigm to test
the impact of two sets of distracting/intrusion items during the retention phase, one of them labelled
as strong, with high resemblance to memorized items, and weak with low resemblance.
Appearance of the distracter was expected by explicit cueing. Alpha oscillations increased previously
to distracters appearance and predicted individual performance in the task, furthermore, differences
in alpha power correlated negatively with divergences in reaction times. Consequently, subjects that
showed an increased alpha power before strong distracters appearance were also the subjects with
quickest response times during memory probing.

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
10

In a thoughtful study, Payne, Guillroy and Sekuler (2013) carried out an experiment to
evaluate the role of alpha oscillations in the protection of retained stimuli using a short-memory task
with Gabor patches. Participants have to remember the spatial frequency of a Gabor patch to later
reproduce it. Before the reproduction test, other Gabor patch with different spatial frequency was
presented to subjects that were instructed to ignore this item. In the test in which the target Gabor
patch has to be reproduced, subjects used a software which allow them to manipulate the spatial
frequency until participant thinks that matches the target stimuli. This allowed the authors to have a
exact measurement of the distortion induced by the presentation of task-irrelevant material over the
target one. Participants with higher alpha activity when the distractor stimulus was presented also
maintained a less corrupted version of the stimuli to be remembered. The authors interpreted this as
evidence that alpha activity acts as a filter for irrelevant information. The illustration below shows the
non-target effect influence of distractor over the target Gabor patch in pre-stimulus and onset of the
distractor during WM maintenance. Note the value of correlation coefficients.


Figure 9: Payne, Guillroy and Sekuler, 2013

Therefore, alpha activity is also key for retaining and maintaining the fidelity of the items in
the WM retention against the possible intrusions. This property is capital as intrusion of task-irrelevant
items is a major cause of failures in WM, particularly in the aged population (Zanto and Gazzaley,
2009).
III. Critical considerations & Future Directions
Cueing and management of temporal expectations
One of the fundamental problems concerning literature about alpha inhibition in pre-encoding
and dynamic encoding roles is how to establish an appropriate cueing to avoid possible confounds.
Alpha suppression or alpha ERD can appear due to incoming activity in a bottom-up fashion as an
stimulus in being processed, as the stimulus simply appear (Ray and Cole, 1985; Klimesch et al,
1999). Thus, cue processing (or stimulus appearance, for that matter) and cues top-down effect can
mix up if cueing and stimulus onset are too close. According to Klimesch et al (2012) alpha
suppression is expected between 200 500 ms after stimulus onset whereas Rihs et al (2007) says
that alpha ERD as a result of cueing is expected to appear at 400-600 ms after stimulus onset,
revealing an overlap between the two modalities.


State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
11

Conversely, the opposite problem might also appear. Late or very early cueing would not
lead to an immediate appearance of alpha suppression while when short cueing was provided alpha
suppression appeared almost instantly. Payne et al (2013) varied from 300 to 900 ms the cueing of
the distractor stimulus, when cueing was presented 300ms modulation of alpha began straight away,
whereas 900 ms cueing resulted in a modulatory effect several hundred milliseconds after onset. A
possible reason for this delay, as we are talking about top-down modulations, would be that the
participant is aware of the incoming appearance of a stimulus, but does not expect it in the immediate
term and therefore delays the action of attention focusing. This could be solved setting intervals of
appearance or jittering like in the study by Wilsch et al (2014). These issues are especially relevant
when we consider the use of time-frequency instead of evoked-response analysis in the field, as
expectancy changes in alpha amplitude might be diffuse over time.

Setting up tasks: the importance of tasks properties

The nature of the WM is a common topic of discussion. While there is a trend to use
competitive visual stimuli (for example two arrays of different visual targets e.g Sauseng et al, 2009 or
Handel et al 2011) there is some controversy about how of appropriated these stimuli are. Thus,
modulations in alpha power can be found over the left hemisphere but in occasions not over the right
one, as in Horschig et al (2014). There is some consistency about a differential behaviour between
the two hemispheres, which states that left parietal regions process right hemi field visual stimuli while
right parietal regions process a certain amount of both hem fields output (Sack, 2009 as quoted in
Zanto et al, 2014). A similar effect has been claimed in somatosensory tasks (Haegens et al 2011).
Task properties are also relevant when it comes to ensure that inhibition or suppression has
actually taken place. In the experiment used to exemplify dynamic encoding (Freunberger et al 2009)
the reader will note that there is actually no way to ensure that irrelevant items have in fact being
ignored and not entered the WM retention system.
A way to overcome this problem can be seen in other of the examples, this time by Payne et
al (2013) in which Gabor patches were used. In that case, the impact of the task-irrelevant information
is made explicit by making a discrete (remember/no remember) category quantifiable in terms of the
percentage of the spatial frequency change due to distractors influence in the reproduction made by
the subject. Although witty, this task proved to be very easy for participants, in especial when the
task-irrelevant item was presented in first place and the task-relevant patch in the second.

Detaching roles and measuring sources

The tasks inherent properties are perhaps especially important regarding WM maintenance
and retention studies. It is important to notice that caution is advisable while approaching this kind of
experiments as alpha activity can be easily misunderstood if the experimental setting is not correctly
settled. When the stimuli that has to be ignored and the stimuli that has to be memorized are shown
at the same time, and especially if both stimuli are of the same modality (like two visual figures) to
disentangle between the possible contributions of alpha activity can be complicated.
This confusion arises from the potential role that alpha might be accomplishing, if intervening
directly in items maintenance (Palva and Palva, 2007, Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014) or suppressing and
rejecting possible intrusions (Freunberger et al 2009; Bonnefond and Jensen, 2012; Payne and
Sekuler, 2013). Normally, this distinction is drawn based on where the activity is located, if in task-
relevant or in task-irrelevant sites. Noteworthy, this distinction is often difficult to be established as
extensive parts of the current body of evidence supporting the inhibitory role of alpha oscillations are
based in EEG recordings, which involve a considerable spatial smearing (Palva and Palva, 2007).

State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
12

Is in this framework in which studies involving joint EEG/fMRI recordings have the best
opportunity to make a unique contribution in finding an answer for this dilemma. Furthermore, BOLD
signal has been found to correlate negatively with alpha activity (Scheeringa et al, 2009; look at
Jensen and Mazaheri 2010 for more examples).
There is other issue when recording alpha-band activity regarding the place of its origin.
Alpha activity is thought to appear from cortico-cortico sources or cortico-thalamic sources (Nicolelis
and Fanselow, 2002) and it is thought to accomplish a open/closing function of the thalamic gates
thus enabling or disabling processing. It is less clear how efficiently alpha activity can be measured
from such sources, again being here relevant complementary studies with fMRI or perhaps using
invasive recordings in animal models.

Future directions; the interplay between phase and amplitude

Most of the research to date has focused on amplitude measurements to assess the role of
alpha oscillations. However, a significant but yet a reduced part of the research community have
claimed that other components might have something to add to this ongoing conversation, like phase
or perhaps temporal envelopes (Palva and Palva, 2007; Mazaheri and Jensen, 2009; Payne et al,
2013).
More specifically, Palva and Palva (2007) have claimed that it is phase rather than amplitude
the component that carries on with the functional significance of alpha oscillations, attributing to
alpha-band rhythmicity a key role in attention and consciousness and against the widely accepted
alpha amplitude thesis. They also pointed out that alpha phase reordering can take place in the
absence of alpha amplitude changes (supported by Hanslmayr et al,2005), and even during alpha
amplitude suppression. Palva and Palva base their claims of an active role for alpha phase coherence
mainly in studies in which an increase in phase synchrony is spotted around frontal areas (mainly
mental calculations Palva et al, 2005, Halgren et al, 2002 or perceptual selection Mima et al 2001)
although authors intended a broader generalization.
Perhaps more interestingly, Freunberger et al (2009) proposes that alpha phase serves as a
mechanism relevant for neural activity timing and in the binding of memory processes, showing how
increase in alpha phase locking in low alpha frequencies (7-12Hz) takes place for items that have to
be remembered later on in a WM task. Crucially, in this study the amplitude component was also
examined, and authors observed an increase in phase coupling around stimulus onset together with a
ERD for to-be-remembered items. The increase in phase-locking would be then a marker of
information encoding, similarly to the way in which was originally proposed by Klimesch et al (2007),
and possibly a marker of phase resetting. In a later article, Freunberger et al (2011) developed that
the role played by phase could reside in the segregation of items, while keeping them in a coherent
and integrated whole.
This series of articles by Fruenberger et al (2009; 2011) can provide powerful enriched
framework that might improve our understanding of alpha oscillations in WM tasks as seen in the
below graph







State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
13


Amplitude Phase

To-be-remembered
amplitude (a) (PLI*)
(7-12Hz)


Not-to-be-remembered
amplitude (a) (PLI*)
(7-12Hz)

*Phase Locking Index --
Figure 10: Adapted from Freunberger et al, 2009, made by author.

Klimesch et al (2012) shaped further the Fruenbergers framework of amplitude and phase
suggesting that amplitude manages the width of the encoding slots. This function of amplitude will be
however a question of grading because while in a moderate manner amplitude will serve as referred,
amplitude in the higher extent would inhibit any processing. Is in this sense, phase angle will be
relevant at the start of the information encoding in its interplay with the P1 component.

Figure 11: Klimesch et al, 2012

This hypothesis presented by Klimesch et al (2012) has yet to find solid experimental support,
to the best of the authors knowledge. However, Zanto et al (2014) found promising results when
embracing this framework in an experiment using a WM task context. Briefly, they found improved
performance while TMS pulses were delivered during the peak of the alpha oscillation compared to
when it were delivered in a trough before stimulus onset (illustration above provides a short rationale,
peak must coincide with P1 component appearance). Additional evidence that have taken advantage
of phase can be found in Roux et al (2012), in which interareal phase synchronization was found to
be relevant for WM maintenance.
Perhaps, some of the studies that lack of coherence within the alpha inhibition theory will find
some more sense when phase components are also take into account. Phase measurements might
also prove to be profitable in their integration with cross-frequency models and nested oscillatory
paradigms. To discuss these models will be well beyond the objective of this essay, although the
interested reader could find useful Roux and Uhlhaas (2014) in the WM domain. In a last remark,
studies combining MRS spectroscopy and EEG are suggestive, as alpha is as well thought to be
evoked with the help of inhibitory interneurons (Buzsaki and Chrobak, 1995).
State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
14

Potential manipulations over GABA levels looking for a correlate in alpha inhibitory activity and
WM performance in studies similar to the one by Freunberger et al (2009) could complement existent
TMS evidence (Sauseng et al 2009; Romei et al, 2010) about the role of alpha oscillations.
IV. Conclusions
There are several ways in which alpha oscillations contribute to successful performance in
WM tasks by exerting its inhibitory role. The suppression of irrelevant information in early stages of
processing saves valuable resources needed for the correct encoding of pertinent items and posterior
operations. Also, alpha activity supports the rejection of distracters during retention and maintenance
stages protecting relevant items from external disruption, function which impairment has
demonstrated to be critical for WM deficits in several diseases.
All in all, the exact mechanisms underpinning alpha inhibition are yet to be clarified, and the
emergence of other components like phase coupling promises to shed light upon this matter. The
subsequent shaping of alpha-band activity by measuring phase and amplitude in a coherent
framework, together with further studies that could provide finer spatial specificity and the addition of
alpha activity to studies seeking to define cross-frequency networks sets an exciting moment for the
research community. This scenario promises to generate powerful tools for the understanding of
perceptual and cognitive abilities, which applications will have a predictable application in disease
models.
V. References

Adrian, E. D., & Matthews, B. H. (1934). The Berger rhythm: potential changes from the occipital lobes in
man. Brain, 57(4), 355-385.
Amieva, H., Phillips, L. H., Della Sala, S., & Henry, J. D. (2004). Inhibitory functioning in Alzheimers
disease. Brain, 127(5), 949-964.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10),
829-839.
Blanger, S., Belleville, S., & Gauthier, S. (2010). Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive
impairment and healthy aging: Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 581-
590.
Belleville, S., Rouleau, N., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Use of the Hayling task to measure inhibition of
prepotent responses in normal aging and Alzheimers disease. Brain and cognition, 62(2), 113-119.
Bonnefond, M., & Jensen, O. (2012). Alpha oscillations serve to protect working memory maintenance against
anticipated distracters. Current biology, 22(20), 1969-1974.
Buzski, G., & Chrobak, J. J. (1995). Temporal structure in spatially organized neuronal ensembles: a role for
interneuronal networks. Current opinion in neurobiology, 5(4), 504-510.
Cowan, N. (2010). The Magical Mystery Four How Is Working Memory Capacity Limited, and Why?. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 51-57.
Foxe, J. J., & Snyder, A. C. (2011). The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism
during selective attention. Frontiers in psychology, 2.
State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
15

Freunberger, R., Fellinger, R., Sauseng, P., Gruber, W., & Klimesch, W. (2009). Dissociation between phase
locked and nonphaselocked alpha oscillations in a working memory task. Human brain mapping, 30(10), 3417-
3425.
Freunberger, R., Werkle-Bergner, M., Griesmayr, B., Lindenberger, U., & Klimesch, W. (2011). Brain oscillatory
correlates of working memory constraints. Brain research, 1375, 93-102.
Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working
memory. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16(2), 129-135.
Haegens, S., Osipova, D., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2010). Somatosensory working memory performance in
humans depends on both engagement and disengagement of regions in a distributed network. Human brain
mapping, 31(1), 26-35.
Haegens, S., Ncher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R., & Jensen, O. (2011). -Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor
network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 108(48), 19377-19382.
Haegens, S., Luther, L., & Jensen, O. (2012). Somatosensory anticipatory alpha activity increases to suppress
distracting input. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 24(3), 677-685.
Haenschel, C., & Linden, D. (2011). Exploring intermediate phenotypes with EEG: working memory dysfunction
in schizophrenia. Behavioural brain research, 216(2), 481-495.
Halgren, E., Dhond, R. P., Christensen, N., Van Petten, C., Marinkovic, K., Lewine, J. D., & Dale, A. M. (2002).
N400-like magnetoencephalography responses modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical
class in sentences. Neuroimage, 17(3), 1101-1116.
Hndel, B. F., Haarmeier, T., & Jensen, O. (2011). Alpha oscillations correlate with the successful inhibition of
unattended stimuli. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 23(9), 2494-2502.
Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Doppelmayr, M., Schabus, M., & Klimesch, W. (2005). Increasing individual upper
alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects. Applied psychophysiology
and biofeedback, 30(1), 1-10.
Hanslmayr, S., Aslan, A., Staudigl, T., Klimesch, W., Herrmann, C. S., & Buml, K. H. (2007). Prestimulus
oscillations predict visual perception performance between and within subjects. Neuroimage, 37(4), 1465-
1473.
Horschig, J. M., Jensen, O., Van Schouwenburg, M. R., Cools, R., & Bonnefond, M. (2014). Alpha activity reflects
individual abilities to adapt to the environment. NeuroImage.
Jensen, O., & Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working
memory task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(8), 1395-1399.
Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by
inhibition. Frontiers in human neuroscience,4.
Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 24(1), 61-100.
Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and
analysis. Brain research reviews, 29(2), 169-195.
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibitiontiming
hypothesis. Brain research reviews, 53(1), 63-88.
State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
16

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., Hanslmayr, S., Gruber, W., & Freunberger, R. (2007). Event-related phase
reorganization may explain evoked neural dynamics. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(7), 1003-1016.
Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in
cognitive sciences, 16(12), 606-617.
Leiberg, S., Lutzenberger, W., & Kaiser, J. (2006). Effects of memory load on cortical oscillatory activity during
auditory pattern working memory. Brain research, 1120(1), 131-140.
Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., & Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: prestimulus
phase predicts visual awareness. The Journal of neuroscience, 29(9), 2725-2732.
Mazaheri, A., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., van Dijk, H., & Jensen, O. (2009). Prestimulus alpha and mu activity predicts
failure to inhibit motor responses. Human brain mapping, 30(6), 1791-1800.
Mazaheri, A., & Jensen, O. (2010). Rhythmic pulsing: linking ongoing brain activity with evoked
responses. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 4(0).
Millett, D. (2001). Hans Berger: From psychic energy to the EEG. Perspectives in biology and medicine, 44(4),
522-542.
Mima, T., Oluwatimilehin, T., Hiraoka, T., & Hallett, M. (2001). Transient interhemispheric neuronal synchrony
correlates with object recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(11), 3942-3948.
Nicolelis, M. A., & Fanselow, E. E. (2002). Dynamic shifting in thalamocortical processing during different
behavioural states. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological
Sciences, 357(1428), 1753-1758.
Obleser, J., & Weisz, N. (2012). Suppressed alpha oscillations predict intelligibility of speech and its acoustic
details. Cerebral cortex, 22(11), 2466-2477.
Palva, J. M., Palva, S., & Kaila, K. (2005). Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the human cortex. The
Journal of Neuroscience, 25(15), 3962-3972.
Palva, S., & Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for -frequency band oscillations.Trends in neurosciences, 30(4),
150-158.
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual
review of psychology, 60, 173.
Payne, L., Guillory, S., & Sekuler, R. (2013). Attention-modulated alpha-band oscillations protect against
intrusion of irrelevant information. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 25(9), 1463-1476.
Payne, L., & Sekuler, R. The importance of ignoring: Alpha oscillations protect selectivity.
Pfurtscheller, G., Stancak Jr, A., & Neuper, C. (1996). Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha bandan
electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(1), 39-
46.
Pfurtscheller, G., & Neuper, C. (2001). Motor imagery and direct brain-computer communication. Proceedings
of the IEEE, 89(7), 1123-1134.
Ray, W. J., & Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects
emotional and cognitive processes.Science, 228(4700), 750-752.
Rihs, T. A., Michel, C. M., & Thut, G. (2007). Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are
indexed by band EEG synchronization.European Journal of Neuroscience, 25(2), 603-610.
State of the Art Essay (II) - Mario B. Perez
17

Rohenkohl, G., Cravo, A. M., Wyart, V., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Temporal expectation improves the quality of
sensory information. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(24), 8424-8428.
Romei, V., Gross, J., & Thut, G. (2010). On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipito-parietal areas in
visual input regulation: correlation or causation?. The Journal of neuroscience, 30(25), 8692-8697.
Roux, F., Wibral, M., Mohr, H. M., Singer, W., & Uhlhaas, P. J. (2012). Gamma-band activity in human
prefrontal cortex codes for the number of relevant items maintained in working memory. The Journal of
neuroscience, 32(36), 12411-12420.
Roux, F., & Uhlhaas, P. J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: alphagamma versus thetagamma
codes for distinct WM information?. Trends in cognitive sciences, 18(1), 16-25.
Sack, A. T., Kadosh, R. C., Schuhmann, T., Moerel, M., Walsh, V., & Goebel, R. (2009). Optimizing functional
accuracy of TMS in cognitive studies: a comparison of methods. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(2), 207-
221.
Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Stadler, W., Schabus, M., Doppelmayr, M., Hanslmayr, S., ... & Birbaumer, N. (2005).
A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 22(11), 2917-2926.
Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Gerloff, C., & Hummel, F. C. (2009). Spontaneous locally restricted EEG alpha activity
determines cortical excitability in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 284-288.
Scheeringa, R., Petersson, K. M., Oostenveld, R., Norris, D. G., Hagoort, P., & Bastiaansen, M. (2009). Trial-by-
trial coupling between EEG and BOLD identifies networks related to alpha and theta EEG power increases
during working memory maintenance. Neuroimage, 44(3), 1224-1238.
Tuladhar, A. M., Huurne, N. T., Schoffelen, J. M., Maris, E., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2007). Parieto
occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load. Human brain
mapping, 28(8), 785-792.
Van Dijk, H., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2008). Prestimulus oscillatory activity in the alpha
band predicts visual discrimination ability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(8), 1816-18
VanRullen, R., Busch, N. A., Drewes, J., & Dubois, J. (2011). Ongoing EEG phase as a trial-by-trial predictor of
perceptual and attentional variability.Frontiers in psychology, 2.
Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in
controlling access to working memory.Nature, 438(7067), 500-503.
Wilsch, A., Henry, M. J., Herrmann, B., Maess, B., & Obleser, J. (2014). Alpha Oscillatory Dynamics Index
Temporal Expectation Benefits in Working Memory.Cerebral Cortex, bhu004.
Worden, M. S., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G. V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention
indexed by retinotopically specific-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J Neurosci, 20,
1-6.
Zanto, T. P., & Gazzaley, A. (2009). Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies optimal working
memory performance. The Journal of Neuroscience,29(10), 3059-3066.
Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-
down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nature neuroscience, 14(5), 656-661.
Zanto, T. P., Chadick, J. Z., & Gazzaley, A. (2014). Anticipatory alpha phase influences visual working memory
performance. Neuroimage, 85, 794-802.

Potrebbero piacerti anche