Sei sulla pagina 1di 25

American Sociological Review

78(1) 26 50
American Sociological
Association 2012
DOI: 10.1177/0003122412472340
http://asr.sagepub.com
In the United States, a new narrative is emerg-
ing to describe contemporary marriage. Chal-
lenging the notion of marriage as an institu-
tion ensnared in a stalled gender revolution,
this new perspective asserts that todays mar-
riages are more egalitarian, flexible, and fair
than those of the past (Sullivan 2006; Sulli-
van and Coltrane 2008). The theme of con-
vergence between wives and husbands roles
has taken center stage at high-profile confer-
ences on the family, such as those of the
Council on Contemporary Families, in policy
pieces on marriage and feminism (Gornick
2002; Marshall and Sawhill 2004), and in
academic work predicated on the demise of
the male breadwinner model in the industrial-
ized West (Crompton 1999).
The debate about how much heterosexual
marriage has changed from traditional models
often boils down to changes in the division of
labor (cf. Bianchi et al. 2000). More specifi-
cally, it hinges on whether married mens par-
ticipation in household work has increased
meaningfully. Advocates of the gender-role
convergence perspective argue that recognition
472340ASRXXX10.1177/0003122412472340
American Sociological ReviewKornrich et al.
2012
a
Juan March Institute
b
University of Washington
Corresponding Author:
Sabino Kornrich, Center for Advanced Studies in
the Social Sciences, Juan March Institute, Calle
de Castello, 77, 28006 Madrid, Spain
E-mail: kornrich@gmail.com
Egalitarianism, Housework,
and Sexual Frequency in
Marriage
Sabino Kornrich,
a
Julie Brines,
b

and Katrina Leupp
b
Abstract
Changes in the nature of marriage have spurred a debate about the consequences of shifts
to more egalitarian relationships, and media interest in the debate has crystallized around
claims that men who participate in housework get more sex. However, little systematic or
representative research supports the claim that women, in essence, exchange sex for mens
participation in housework. Although research and theory support the expectation that
egalitarian marriages are higher quality, other studies underscore the ongoing importance of
traditional gender behavior and gender display in marriage. Using data from Wave II of the
National Survey of Families and Households, this study investigates the links between mens
participation in core (traditionally female) and non-core (traditionally male) household tasks
and sexual frequency. Results show that both husbands and wives in couples with more
traditional housework arrangements report higher sexual frequency, suggesting the importance
of gender display rather than marital exchange for sex between heterosexual married partners.
Keywords
gender, household labor, marriage, sexual frequency
Kornrich et al. 27
of change has been lost, because scholars high-
light womens larger share of household work
but fail to recognize married mens greater
participation in housework and childcare as a
response to the dramatic rise in wives employ-
ment and paid labor (Sullivan 2006). Debates
about the importance of houseworkand
under what conditions men and women do
more houseworkhave recently come to
the fore again (England 2011; Risman 2011;
Schneider 2012; Sullivan 2011).
Although this debate can resemble a strug-
gle over whether the glass is half-empty or
half-full, evidence is accumulating that U.S.
husbands are, in fact, doing more unpaid fam-
ily work, particularly in the realm of child-
care, than did their counterparts of yesteryear.
From the 1960s to the beginning of the
twenty-first century, mens contribution to
housework doubled, increasing from about 15
to over 30 percent of the total (Bianchi et al.
2000; Fisher et al. 2006; Robinson and God-
bey 1997). Similar trends are evident for
industrialized countries throughout the world:
mens proportional contribution to family
work (including housework, childcare, and
shopping) increased, on average, from less
than one-fifth in 1965 to more than one-third
by 2003 (Hook 2006).
Accompanying the effort to track secular
change in wives and husbands work patterns
are efforts to document how egalitarian work
arrangements affect other components of
marriage. Interest in the connections among
role similarity or complementarity, a couples
cohesiveness, and marital well-being is long-
standing in social science (Becker 1981; Par-
sons and Bales 1955), but it seems to have
intensified in tandem with the recent claims
of work-role convergence (Amato et al. 2003,
2007; Brines and Joyner 1999). Here again,
special attention is devoted to the household
division of unpaid family work. For example,
research shows that when men do more
housework, wives perceptions of fairness
and marital satisfaction tend to rise (Amato
et al. 2003; Stevens, Kiger, and Mannon
2005) and couples experience less marital
conflict (Coltrane 2000).
1
Other research
shows that U.S. couples who have more equal
divisions of labor are less likely to divorce
than are couples where one partner special-
izes in breadwinning and the other partner
specializes in family work (Cooke 2006).
The claim that couples who share house-
work have more sex has captured substantial
public attention. In the popular imagination,
husbands contributions to housework seem
decisive, the implications of which were
recently spun in a headline: Men: Want More
Sex? Do the Laundry! This claim appears to
have originated in an unpublished survey con-
ducted by Chethik (2006). It so captured the
popular imagination (or at least that of report-
ers) that it led to an Associated Press story
subsequently featured online by media giants
ABC, NBC, CBS, CNN, FOX, and smaller
sites like the Huffington Post and China Daily.
Sex is an understudied but important com-
ponent of marriage, continuing to be a central
area of spousal concern and conflict (Elliott
and Umberson 2008). Sexual activity is linked
to marital satisfaction, but there have been few
recent attempts to understand the organization
of sexual frequency in marriage (Call, Spre-
cher, and Schwartz 1995; although see Gager
and Yabiku 2010; Yabiku and Gager 2009).
Romantic and sexual scripts are often highly
gendered outside marriage (Udry and Chantala
2004), and we suspect they remain so within
marriage. Sexual activity, in addition to being
important in its own right, also offers a view
about the functioning of gender relations in
marriage at the close of the twentieth century.
Although the notion that egalitarian mar-
riages are sexier was widely broadcast in the
media, there is little empirical support for this
view. The claim rests on results of a small-
scale (N = 300) survey and reports of couples
in therapy conducted by Chethik, which, while
intriguing, are difficult to evaluate (Chethik
2006; cf. North 2007). Moreover, other
research suggests that for all the benefits of
peer marriage, more egalitarian couples are
more likely to have unsatisfactory sex lives
and experience a lack of passion due to habitu-
ation, and these differences are not explained
by a shortage of time (Schwartz 1995). While
28 American Sociological Review 78(1)
couples in more traditional marriages may
experience a range of marital difficulties,
lower sexual interest is especially a problem
among egalitarian couples (Schwartz 1995).
More recent research finds that husbands
housework is positively linked to sexual fre-
quency, but womens own housework hours
are even more strongly associated with sexual
frequency, suggesting that greater egalitarian-
ism may not be associated with higher sexual
frequency (Gager and Yabiku 2010).
In this article, we begin by outlining two
bodies of theory that offer competing predic-
tions about the relationship between sexual
frequency and the household division of labor
among heterosexual married couples. We first
discuss predictions derived from exchange
theory, then predictions from an approach that
stresses the gendered nature of sexual scripts,
and finally turn to a range of important con-
trol variables derived from the existing litera-
ture that emphasizes constraints and
opportunities for sex. One key innovation is
that rather than consider all housework as
identical, we separately examine mens and
womens time spent in traditionally mascu-
line and traditionally feminine tasks. We use
nationally representative data to test whether
and how sexual frequency is linked to the
household division of labor. Our results do
not support the notion that more egalitarian
divisions of labor are associated with higher
sexual frequency. Instead, we find that house-
holds in which men do more traditionally
male labor and women do more traditionally
female labor report higher sexual frequency.
This suggests that among heterosexual cou-
ples, the relationship between housework and
a couples sex life is governed by a gendered
set of sexual scripts.
SEX IN MARRIAGE:
EGALITARIANISM AND
EXCHANGE
Sex in marriage, and what leads to more or
less of it, reliably excites the popular imagina-
tion, but interest in these questions has a more
uneven history in the social sciences. Kinseys
early attempts to develop a science of the terra
incognita of human sexual behavior found
that marital intercourse was, as described by
Blumer (1948:522), the chief medium of sex
outlet for the adults in his samples (Kinsey,
Pomeroy, and Martin 1948). However, chang-
ing family demographics and related policy
preoccupations have arguably steered atten-
tion away from research on sexual frequency
in marriage over the last quarter-century. Over
the past few decades, scholars have noted the
scarcity of research on sexual activity among
married and committed couples (Call et al.
1995; Christopher and Sprecher 2000;
Greenblat 1983), despite the emergence of
several nationally representative surveys that
gathered data on respondents sexual behavior
in the late 1980s and 1990s.
Nonetheless, sex is an important compo-
nent of marriage. Blumstein and Schwartzs
classic, American Couples (1983), identified
sex (in addition to money and power) as a key
good around which marriagesindeed all
intimate partnershipsare organized. Sexual
frequency is of interest for researchers
because it is positively linked to emotional
satisfaction and physical pleasure, and cou-
ples with greater sexual frequency are less
likely to divorce or break up (Waite and
Joyner 2001; Yabiku and Gager 2009; Yeh
et al. 2006). Throughout this article, we
assume that greater sexual frequency is gen-
erally a desired good: conflict may exist over
the timing and frequency of sex (Elliott and
Umberson 2008), but more frequent sex is
linked to higher sexual and marital satisfac-
tion for both men and women.
2
Couples
believe sex is an important part of marriage,
but there is also substantial marital conflict
over sex, largely because men and women
differ in their desire for sex. This suggests
caution in a straightforward interpretation of
sexual frequency as purely unproblematic or
reflecting desire (Elliott and Umberson 2008).
The difference in mens and womens
desire for sex underpins a key perspective on
sex: sex can be used as a resource for exchange.
Predictions of social exchange theory are of
Kornrich et al. 29
particular interest (Homans 1961; Sprecher
1998).
3
Because spouses (the parties to the
exchange) possess different resources, they
benefit from exchanging a resource one pos-
sesses for another scarce resource the other
possesses. Sex, in this view, is a resource
that partners might use for exchange. A self-
interested view of social exchange suggests
that individuals exchange when each party
benefits. Partners thus trade sex for other
scarce resources such as time, money, com-
mitment, or other goods when they both ben-
efit (Baumeister and Vohs 2004).
Although the condition of mutual benefit
suggests a gender-free venue for exchange,
both popular and scholarly understandings see
sex as a female, rather than male, resource.
Baumeister and Vohs (2004) argue compel-
lingly that sex should be seen as a female
resource due to the principle of least interest
if men want sex more than women, they must
induce women to engage in sex by offering
other benefits. A review of a wide variety of
measures of sex drive suggests that men want
sex more than women (Baumeister, Catanese,
and Vohs 2001).
4
Whether mens greater sex-
ual desire results from biological or cultural
factors is immaterial; either condition results
in womens possession of a scarce resource.
An exchange perspective, combined with
the assumption that men desire sex more than
women, suggests that women could trade sex
for resources men control. This could apply to
any set of bargaining goals (e.g., decision-
making, monetary or gift exchange, or time
spent on any task), but we focus here on the
application to household labor, because labor
has been at the center of a discussion about
how much marriages have changed. In addi-
tion, a long research tradition investigates
whether and how women exchange another
resource they controltheir earningsfor
mens participation in housework (Brines
1994; Greenstein 2000; Gupta 2007; Lund-
berg and Pollak 1993).
5
A central assumption
of this line of research, which we follow, is
that both men and women prefer to avoid
housework, but housework is more likely to
fall into womens sphere of responsibilities by
default (cf. Lundberg and Pollak 1993). The
implication is that women are likely to use
their resourcesin this case, sexto bargain
their way out of performing housework.
Qualitative evidence supports the view that
some women exchange sex for mens partici-
pation in household labor, or, alternatively,
withhold sex when men do not participate in
household labor. In Hochschild and Machungs
(1989:45) The Second Shift, one respondent
(Nancy) notes, When Evan refused to carry
his load at home . . . I used sex. I said, Look,
Evan, I would not be this exhausted and
asexual every night if I didnt have so much to
face every morning. Similarly, one of Elliott
and Umbersons (2008:401) respondents
(Chantelle) said, [I tell Anthony,] If I have
had a really good day, and you have been
helpful, I would say you took out the trash and
you brought the trashcans in and you mowed
the lawn and everything. Those are the things
that work for me to kind of get me going.
6
Rather than direct exchange, it is possible
that sexual frequency and an egalitarian divi-
sion of household labor are linked via marital
satisfaction. Recent studies show that hus-
bands participation in household labor is
often associated with wives reports of higher
marital quality (Amato et al. 2003; Stevens et al.
2005). Other work (Chethik 2006) appears to
draw from this result to explain why hus-
bands sharing of housework might lead to
greater frequency of sex in marriage: wives
feel more supported and happier in their mar-
riages when their husbands do more chores,
and these positive feelings promote more sex
as a side benefit. More generally, theoretical
work ranging from the stipulation that a sense
of distributive justice in marriage promotes
coital frequency (Jasso 1987) to economic
models that locate todays marital gains in
partner similarities that maximize joint con-
sumption rather than joint production (Lam
1988; Lundberg and Pollak 1996) also lend
credibility to the idea that an egalitarian divi-
sion of labor results in a happier marriage and
is more conducive to sexual activity.
An exchange perspective would predict a
positive relationship between mens household
30 American Sociological Review 78(1)
labor and sexual frequency: sexual frequency
should be high when husbands do more house-
work and low when husbands do less. This
prediction reflects an understanding of mar-
riage as a site characterized by the exchange of
scarce resources between partners, and is con-
cordant with popular and scholarly under-
standings of sex in marriage. Nevertheless,
given research linking marital satisfaction to
husbands participation in household labor and
some research that suggests the importance of
marital satisfaction for sexual frequency (Rao
and Demaris 1995), we are open to the possi-
bility that egalitarian arrangements increase
satisfaction in relationships and thus lead to
greater sexual frequency. We include controls
for marital satisfaction to test this possibility.
SEXUAL SCRIPTS: GENDER,
DIFFERENCE, AND DESIRE
There are reasons to predict a very different
relationship between the division of house-
hold labor and sexual frequency. First, gender
continues to play a central role in organizing
the division of household labor. Women con-
tinue to do more housework than men, and
differences are not explicable by a range of
economic factors. The importance of gender
in organizing labor and marriage suggests that
housework itself may lie outside the realm of
conventional possibilities for exchange.
Second, heterosexual attraction and intimacy
seem to be organized around the enactment of
difference or complementarity between the
sexes (Goffman 1977; Rich 1980). Among
heterosexual couples in their teens, pairs with
a self-rated very masculine boy and self-rated
very feminine girl are most likely to have sex,
and to have sex sooner, than are other roman-
tic pairs (Udry and Chantala 2004). Genders
role in marital sex is less well documented,
but Schwartz (1995, 2007:2) reports that
egalitarianism in committed heterosexual
adult relationships is associated with occa-
sional boredom and a sibling-like tonality to
the relationship that undermines sexual desire.
Schwartz (2007:2) avers that introducing
more distance or difference, rather than
connection and similarity, helps to resurrect
passion in long-term, stable relationships.
These observations suggest a conceptual-
ization of heterosexual marriage as an institu-
tion in which gender still plays a central role
(Berk 1985; Coltrane 1998). Drawing on this
central insight and on a sexual scripts approach
(Gagnon and Simon 1973), we argue that
sexual activity is more likely in households
with more gender-traditional divisions of
household labor. A sexual script approach sug-
gests that for intercourse to occur, a script
must exist that defines a situation as sexual
(Gagnon and Simon 1973). Sexual scripts
specify when, why, and how individuals
should act sexually (Laumann et al. 1994). As
a simple example of a script, intercourse typi-
cally takes place in a series of relatively
tightly delineated stages, moving from kissing
to fondling and then to coitus (Gagnon and
Simon 1973). The approach suggests that
scripts exist at three levels: the cultural or col-
lective, which broadly defines available sets
of scripts; the interpersonal, used when indi-
viduals improvise or adapt cultural scripts for
particular scenarios; and the intrapsychic,
which helps individuals script their own
behaviors and align their own desires (Simon
and Gagnon 1986). In this article, we assume
that internalized dominant cultural scripts
govern sexual behavior, although interper-
sonal and intrapsychic scripts may also struc-
ture sexual behavior in marriage.
How might sexual scripts work in mar-
riage? There is relatively little work on this
topic, but the logic of a sexual scripting argu-
ment generally suggests that womens and
mens sexual activity is governed by internal-
ized cultural scripts.
7
Among teens, sexual
scripts are highly gendered and link sexual
activity to masculinity and femininity (Storms
et al. 1981; Udry and Chantala 2004). Other
recent research finds that men experience
greater sexual dysfunction when their partners
spend more time with the mens friends than
men do themselves, suggesting that behaviors
that threaten mens independence and mascu-
linity lead to greater sexual dysfunction (Corn-
well and Laumann 2011). Given the general
Kornrich et al. 31
importance of gender, we suspect that scripts
continue to link sexual behavior to masculin-
ity or femininity among heterosexual married
couples. If so, expressions of gender differ-
ence should help to create sexual desire.
Household labor and its performanceor lack
thereofis centrally tied to notions of what
constitutes appropriate behavior for men and
women and thus masculinity and femininity
(Berk 1985; Bittman et al. 2003; Brines 1994;
Greenstein 2000; South and Spitze 1994). If
appropriate performances of masculinity and
femininity are prerequisites for sexual behav-
ior or sexual desire in marriage, and house-
work is a key way of engaging in these
performances, then the extent to which hus-
bands and wives do housework in ways that
signify masculinity or femininity should be
linked to sexual frequency.
We note three plausible mechanisms that
might link sexual frequency to gender-
traditional divisions of housework: (1) gen-
der-traditional divisions of labor increase
sexual desire and thus sexual frequency, (2)
both result from gender traditional beliefs or
are ways of doing gender, and (3) gender-
traditional arrangements may increase rela-
tionship satisfaction, which in itself leads to
greater sexual frequency.
The first mechanismthat sexual scripts
activate desire and sexual behavior in the pres-
ence of gendered activitywould operate in
the following fashion. Traditional gender per-
formances serve as cues of masculine and
feminine behavior; these cues activate indi-
viduals internalized cultural sex scripts, creat-
ing sexual desire and activity. In essence,
traditionally masculine and feminine behav-
iors consciously or unconsciously serve as
turn-ons for individuals. We do not argue that
this takes place instantly, but rather over time,
individuals perceive their spouse as more
masculine or feminine as they engage in gen-
der-traditional behaviors, and this increases
sexual attraction. To the extent that masculin-
ity and femininity are central parts of both the
household division of labor and sexual attrac-
tion and activity, we expect that households
with more traditionally gendered divisions of
labor will experience greater sexual frequency.
We note that this argumentthat sexual
behavior is linked to gender identity and
expressionis entirely consistent with a
mechanism proposed by Cornwell and Lau-
mann (2011:17778): in the context of sexual
relationships, masculinity is expressed through
erection, penetration, and climax, so it is
possible that threats to gender identity . . .
manifest as sexual problems.
A second possibility is that couples with
more gender-traditional divisions of house-
work hold more traditional beliefs and act in
more gender-typical ways, which leads to
more frequent sex. More masculine-identified
men may value more frequent sex, and more
highly feminine-identified wives may refuse
sex less often because they view providing
sex as part of being a good wife. Thus, men
may initiate sex more frequently, and wives
refuse less, with no link to desire. In essence,
this mechanism suggests that both housework
and sexual behavior are ways that couples do
gender, and any observed relationship
between the two would reflect couples under-
lying orientations toward gender rather than
causal influence. An alternative possibility
reflecting similar intuitions is that there is
greater coercion among households with tra-
ditional divisions of labor, leading to greater
sexual frequency. However, as we show in the
Appendix, wives reported satisfaction with
their sex life has the same relationship to
mens participation in housework as sexual
frequency. This suggests coercion is not an
important mechanism, because coercion
should lead to higher sexual frequency but
lower sexual satisfaction among women.
A third possibility is simply that gender-
traditional arrangements are linked to sexual
activity because couples perceive greater
affection and love when partners do more
(albeit in traditionally gendered ways) in the
household. Rather than couples engaging in
more sexual activity because traditional divi-
sions of housework act as signals of mascu-
linity and femininity, couples may instead
feel more affection and satisfaction within
their relationships under traditional gender
32 American Sociological Review 78(1)
divisions of labor, and this leads to more
frequent sex. Doing housework can convey
affection, although often in traditionally gen-
dered ways. As DeVault (1991:324) notes,
the gender relations of feeding and eating
seem to convey the message that giving ser-
vice is part of being a woman, and receiving
it fundamentally part of being a man.
Gender-traditional beliefs and practices are
often associated with greater marital happi-
ness and mens emotion work in the family
(Wilcox and Nock 2006).
How does a sexual scripts approach trans-
late into testable hypotheses about the link
between beliefs about gender, the division of
housework, and sexual frequency in marriage?
In short, a sexual scripts perspective (and the
associated alternative mechanisms we noted)
suggests that couples with more egalitarian
divisions of household labor will have less
active sex lives. Because these couples engage
in less traditionally feminine and masculine
behaviors, they are less likely to activate scripts
linking displays of difference to desire. In con-
trast, couples in which husbands and wives
engage in more gender-traditional behaviors
should report more frequent sexual activity.
We attempt to determine whether the alter-
native mechanisms we noted could explain any
association we find. We thus test for two addi-
tional effects. First, to check whether gender
ideology is responsible for any association, we
include measures of gender ideology and reli-
gious affiliation, because religion is often cor-
related with gender ideology and traditional
behavior. Second, we include measures of sat-
isfaction with marriage and with a spouses
contribution to housework to check whether
the division of household labor is associated
with sexual frequency only because it increases
satisfaction in marriage. We are thus able to
offer tests for alternative mechanisms.
SEXUAL OPPORTUNITIES
AND CONSTRAINTS
The perspectives outlined above have much
to say about theoretical relationships between
sexual activity and marital characteristics, but
little existing research focuses on these theo-
ries. Instead, research typically looks at the
role of opportunities and constraints for sex in
marriage, focusing on a variety of demo-
graphic correlates. What we do know about
sexual frequency in marriage is that older
couples report lower sexual frequencies than
younger couples (Blumstein and Schwartz
1983; Brewis and Meyer 2005; Call et al.
1995; Greeley 1991; Greenblat 1983; Rao
and Demaris 1995). Biological aging is the
most common explanation for this decline.
The negative correlation between age and
sexual frequency has also been attributed to
marital duration and habituation (Blumstein
and Schwartz 1983; James 1981). However,
marital duration has no significant effect after
the first year of marriage in models that con-
trol for other time-related variables (Call et al.
1995). Marital satisfaction, in contrast, is the
strongest correlate of sexual frequency, after
age (Call et al. 1995).
Constraints and opportunity also play a
central role in understanding sexual fre-
quency, although results are inconsistent
(Christopher and Sprecher 2000). Certainly,
some constraints matter, such as the presence
of young children or pregnancy (Call et al.
1995; Gager and Yabiku 2010; Greeley 1991).
Time constraints appear to be less important.
Sexual frequency does not decline when both
partners are employed full-time or with the
number of hours husbands and wives spend in
paid work (Call et al. 1995; Gager and Yabiku
2010; Greeley 1991; Hyde, DeLamater, and
Hewitt 1998). Non-standard work, however,
is associated with more sexual problems and
dissatisfaction (White and Keith 1990), sug-
gesting that some, but not all, opportunity
constraints impose costs on couples sex lives.
In addition, a recent article by Gager and
Yabiku (2010) explicitly takes up the relation-
ship between time spent in housework and
sexual frequency, asking whether time spent
in housework serves as a constraint prevent-
ing couples from engaging in sex. Instead,
they find that both mens and womens time
in housework is related to greater sexual fre-
quency. They conclude that this relationship
Kornrich et al. 33
is due to unmeasured tendencies toward
greater activity in both areas: individuals who
work hard also play hard. As we discuss
below, our theoretical approach leads us to
focus on different measures of the extent to
which particular types of housework are gen-
dered. However, to account for the theoretical
relationship Gager and Yabiku suggest, we
also include measures of the total amount of
time spent in housework. Our discussion of
results further compares our model with
theirs.
DATA
To investigate the relationship between sex-
ual frequency and division of household labor
among married couples, we use data from
Wave II of the National Survey of Families
and Households (NSFH) (Sweet and Bumpass
1996).
8
The age of the data may limit gener-
alizability to the present day (interviews
occurred from 1992 to 1994), but to our
knowledge, it is the only dataset with detailed
measures of both sexual frequency and actual
participation in household labor.
9
Despite the
age of the data, we consider these results rel-
evant for contemporary discussions of mar-
riage and the family. We return to the topic of
generalizability to the present day in the
conclusion.
Given the sensitive nature of our depend-
ent variableself-reports of frequency of
sexone problem we encounter is missing
data. Roughly 10 percent of respondents have
missing values for sexual frequency, includ-
ing those who report dont know, and nearly
25 percent of respondents have missing data
on this or another variable in our analysis.
Rather than lose these cases, we used two
procedures for missing data: for housework
variables, we used procedures developed by
South and Spitze (1994); for other missing
data, we relied on multiple imputation.
We dealt with missing and extreme values
on the housework variables using a slight
modification of procedures described by
South and Spitze (1994). First, we excluded
respondents in which both members of a
couple had missing values on more than eight
items of housework. This eliminated 444
respondents, many of whom had missing val-
ues on other key variables.
10
We then recoded
reports of hours spent beyond the 95th per-
centile of the distribution for each housework
item to the 95th percentile of the distribution
for each gender. For respondents with missing
values, we replaced missing values with the
mean for each item for other respondents. For
respondents who gave a value of zero to all
core or non-core items, we placed mens
share at zero.
11
We also experimented with
multiple imputation for missing housework
items. Results were nearly identical, so we
chose the simpler method.
We used multiple imputation for other
missing values because missingness on sex-
ual frequency is likely correlated with ones
actual sexual frequency. Multiple imputation
uses correlations between variables in an
analysis to generate replacement values for
missing values, adding in an error term and
generating multiple estimates to capture the
variability. Estimates from each imputation
are then generated and combined. We used all
variables in our analysis for multiple imputa-
tion, using the ICE program in Stata. ICE is a
regression-based program for imputation,
meaning that variables are imputed using all
other variables as regressors for each of the
other variables. We included a partners
reports of sexual frequency as an auxiliary
variable to improve imputation, but no other
variables because inclusion of auxiliary vari-
ables does little to reduce bias unless the cor-
relations between auxiliary variables and
variables with missing data are high (.9) and
the proportion of missing data is high (e.g., 50
percent missing) (Collins, Schafer, and Kam
2001). We generated 20 imputations because
the rule of thumb of three to five imputations
is often insufficient (Graham, Olchowski, and
Gilreath 2007). We used logistic regression
and ordered logit models to impute non-
continuous variables because using linear
methods and rounding to maintain categorical
or binary variables creates biased estimates
(Horton, Lipsitz, and Parzen 2003).
34 American Sociological Review 78(1)
Another potential source of missing data in
Wave II of the NSFH is attrition from the
original sample interviewed at Wave I,
roughly five to seven years earlier. Roughly
18 percent of Wave I respondents were lost by
Wave II because they could not be found,
were too ill to be interviewed, or did not par-
ticipate for another reason. Attrition could
lead to bias if these couples had lower sexual
frequency or less egalitarian divisions of
labor. Additionally, some respondents
divorced between Waves I and II. As with
those lost from the sample, if these marriages
were different than other couples on measures
of sexual frequency or the division of house-
hold labor, our results could be biased. To test
for this possibility, we performed t-tests for
differences in sexual frequency and the
amount of housework done by men and
women in NSFH Wave I. T-tests (not shown)
indicate no significant differences between
respondents who remained married, remained
in the sample but divorced, and were missing
at Wave II but had been married at Wave I.
MEASURES
We measured the share of household labor
performed by men across two types of house-
hold labor forming a rough approximation of
male and female typed labor. Following exist-
ing literature, we separated tasks into core
and non-core categories (Bianchi et al. 2000).
Core tasks include preparing meals, washing
dishes, cleaning house, shopping, and wash-
ing and ironing; non-core tasks include out-
door work, paying bills, auto maintenance,
and driving. In some sense, core tasks are
female simply because women do more of
them, and non-core tasks are male for the
same reason. Yet beyond this, traditional
notions of masculinity and femininity are
attached to these tasks. Core and non-core
tasks are roughly divided along indoor/
outdoor, nurturing/worldly, and private/public
dimensions that reflect gendered expectations
in place since development of the separate
spheres ideology (Padavic and Reskin 2002).
We calculated share measures using
respondents reports of their own and their
spouses time spent on these activities. We
relied only on self-reports of individuals and
their spouses labor because husbands and
wives did not complete the survey at the same
time, so their reports may diverge because of
differences in the labor performed during the
reported week. Table 1 shows womens and
mens hours, and the ratio of womens to
mens time, for core and non-core household
labor using womens and mens reports. As
other research has demonstrated (Bianchi et al.
2000), women do the majority of core tasks
that need to be done on a daily basis, and men
do relatively more non-core tasks. Similarly,
we find that mens and womens reports
diverge slightly: mens reports indicate more
time on both core and non-core activities
than their wives reports of the same work.
Interestingly, men report that women spend
more time on non-core housework but less
time on core housework than women report
for themselves.
Our measure of sexual frequency is a self-
reported response to the question, About
how often did you and your husband/wife
have sex during the past month? We recoded
Table 1. Wives and Husbands Time in Household Labor
Time Spent on . . . Wives Hours Husbands Hours Husbands Share
Core Labor
Wifes Report 27.9 6.6 19.1%
Husbands Report 26.6 7.7 22.5%
Non-core Labor
Wifes Report 8.3 9.3 52.8%
Husbands Report 8.9 11.1 55.5%
Kornrich et al. 35
values of sexual frequency past the 95th per-
centile to values at the 95th percentile and
imputed values for cases with missing data,
including cases where respondents did not
know or refused to answer. Table 2 shows
descriptive statistics for sexual frequency and
other variables. As Table 2 shows, women
reported having sex with their spouses slightly
more than five and a half times in the past
month, and men reported lower frequencies,
about .4 times fewer over the past month.
Although it may appear surprising that hus-
bands reports are lower than their wives,
existing research comparing husbands and
wives reports has found similar results (Clark
and Wallin 1964; Kinsey et al. 1948).
As we noted earlier, other mechanisms
could explain a relationship between the divi-
sion of household labor and sexual frequency.
We thus included variables to test for the pres-
ence of some of these mechanisms. As a meas-
ure of the extent to which men and women
engage in gender-traditional behaviors, we
included measures of husbands and wives
participation in paid labor. In the models we
present, we relied simply on the number of
hours spent by husbands and wives in paid
work, because these are also measures of poten-
tial constraints on time availability. In other
models, we tested whether male-breadwinner/
female-homemaker households were signifi-
cantly different and found no significant results.
We also tested to see whether gender ide-
ology and gender beliefs dictate housework
arrangements and sexual frequency by includ-
ing two sets of variables. First, we included a
measure of gender ideology, consisting of the
sum of a respondents agreement or disagree-
ment with the following four statements: It
is much better for everyone if the man earns
the main living and the woman takes care of
the home and family; A husband whose
wife is working full-time should spend just as
many hours doing housework as his wife;
Both the husband and wife should contribute
to family income; and It is all right for
mothers to work full-time when their young-
est child is under age 5. In addition, we
controlled for religious affiliation and church
attendance as another way to tap into gender
traditionalism and distinctive patterns of sex
and housework. Earlier research indicates that
Catholics report lower sexual frequency, and
conservative Protestants have more tradition-
ally gendered divisions of labor and distinct
sexual patterns (Call et al. 1995; Wilcox
2004). We thus included dichotomous varia-
bles for respondents religious affiliation, fol-
lowing the coding scheme suggested by
Steensland and colleagues (2000) as closely
as possible using the NSFH data. We ended
up with black Protestant, evangelical Protes-
tant, mainline Protestant, Jewish, Catholic,
conservative Christians, and an other cate-
gory combining the remaining smaller cate-
gories from the coding scheme (nonreligious
is the reference category).
Finally, to control for the possibility that
any relationship between wives and husbands
share of housework functions through its
effects on marital quality, we included controls
for happiness in marriage. We measured this
with responses on a seven-point scale to the
question, Taking things all together, how
would you describe your marriage? and with
spouses housework contributions, measured
with responses to the question, How happy
are you with the work your spouse does around
the house? Additionally, because joint reli-
gious attendance is a good predictor of rela-
tionship quality (Ellison, Burdette, and Wilcox
2010), we included a dichotomous variable
measuring whether both spouses attend church
weekly or more frequently.
We also included measures of family
structure and stage in the life cycle, because
these may be important for sexual frequency
and the division of household labor. First, we
included a measure of marriage within the
past year to control for the possibility of a
honeymoon effect in recent marriages leading
to greater sexual frequency; 3 percent of cou-
ples in our sample were married within the
previous year. We also included measures of
the wifes and husbands age because age is
often an important predictor of sexual fre-
quency and is related to the division of house-
hold labor. We included measures of the
36 American Sociological Review 78(1)
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations of Sexual Frequency and Other Couple Characteristics
Husbands Reports Wives Reports
N Mean SD N Mean SD
Sexual Frequency 4184 5.16 4.54 4153 5.59 4.91
Husbands Share of Core House-
work
4561 .25 .19 4561 .21 .18
Husbands Share of Non-core
Housework
4561 .55 .19 4561 .51 .20
Total Hours Spent on Core House-
work
4561 34.16 16.70 4561 34.30 17.05
Total Hours Spent on Non-core
Housework
4561 20.01 11.24 4561 17.48 9.69
Husbands Hours in Paid Work 4549 35.51 22.94 4549 35.51 22.94
Wifes Hours in Paid Work 4553 21.25 20.46 4553 21.25 20.46
Gender Ideology (higher values are
more conservative)
4403 11.20 2.47 4427 10.47 2.55
Religion
Black Protestant 4529 .05 .22 4529 .05 .22
Evangelical Protestant 4529 .22 .41 4529 .23 .42
Mainline Protestant 4529 .28 .45 4529 .30 .46
Catholic 4529 .24 .43 4529 .24 .43
Jewish 4529 .02 .15 4529 .02 .14
Conservative Christian 4529 .04 .19 4529 .04 .20
Other Religious or Spiritual 4529 .12 .33 4529 .09 .28
Happiness with Marriage (1 is
unhappy, 7 is happy)
4190 5.97 1.27 4244 5.93 1.33
Happiness with Spouses Contribu-
tion to Housework
4529 5.88 1.40 4508 5.10 1.80
Both Spouses Attend Church
Weekly or More Often
4532 .46 .50 4532 .46 .50
Recently Married 4559 .03 .17 4559 .03 .17
Wifes Age 4558 43.76 13.64 4558 43.76 13.64
Husbands Age 4559 46.25 14.10 4559 46.25 14.10
# of Children < 2 Years in House-
hold
4561 .19 .44 4561 .19 .44
# of Children Age 2 to 6 in House-
hold
4561 .21 .47 4561 .21 .47
# of Children Age 6 to 13 in
Household
4561 .48 .78 4561 .48 .78
Wifes Share of Income 4389 .31 .26 4389 .31 .26
Total Household Income 4535 48.85 40.19 4535 48.85 40.19
How Often Spent Time Alone with
Spouse in Past Month

Never (reference category) 4499 .02 .14 4498 .03 .18
Between Once a Month and
Once a Week
4499 .37 .48 4498 .36 .48
Two or Three Times a Week 4499 .23 .42 4498 .19 .39
Almost Every Day 4499 .38 .49 4498 .42 .49
Education
Did Not Complete High School
(reference category)
4545 .16 .36 4549 .14 .34
High School Graduate 4545 .33 .47 4549 .36 .48
Completed Some College 4545 .24 .43 4549 .27 .44
College Degree 4545 .28 .45 4549 .23 .42
Self-rated Health 4499 3.96 .80 4496 3.98 .81
Kornrich et al. 37
number of children living in the household
below age 2 years, between 2 and 6 years, and
between 6 and 13 years. We controlled for
husbands and wives economic contribu-
tions, using the share of the income provided
by the wife and total household income.
12
For
these measures, values in Table 2 are the same
for men and women. This is because they are
based on either primary respondents
responses, as in the case of the number of
children, or self-reports from each spouse, as
with wifes age and husbands age.
As an additional control for opportunity,
we included a measure of time spent alone
with the spouse over the past month. Respond-
ents answered the question, During the past
month, about how often did you and your
husband/wife spend time alone with each
other, talking, or sharing an activity? and
were given six response options: never, about
once a month, two or three times a month,
about once a week, two or three times a week,
and almost every day. We collapsed these six
categories to four. Interestingly, nearly 40
percent of respondents said they spent time
alone with their spouse once a week or less
during the previous month.
In addition, we included controls for edu-
cation. We measured education using a series
of dichotomous variables for completion of
high school, attendance of some college, and
completion of a college degree using hus-
bands and wives self-reports. Finally, we
included measures of wifes and husbands
self-rated health. Health was self-reported
using a five-point scale, with higher values
representing greater perceived health.
METHODS
Because sexual frequency is distributed as a
count variable but is overdispersedthat is,
the distribution of sexual frequency violates
the assumption in Poisson regression that the
mean and variance are equalthe use of ordi-
nary least squares or Poisson regression is
inappropriate. We used negative binomial
regression to assess links between mens and
womens participation in different types of
household labor and sexual frequency.
Negative binomial regression models the
count-generating process but relaxes the
assumption that variance of the distribution is
equal to the mean.
The model takes the following form:
log(l
i
) = x
i
b, var(Y) = lt
where l
i
= E(Y), Y is sexual frequency, b

is a
set of coefficients produced by the model
including an intercept, x
i

is the set of inde-
pendent variables, and t is a shape parameter
modifying the variance of Y.
The NSFH has two types of respondents:
primary respondents for a household and their
spouses. To take full advantage of the availa-
ble data, we relied on both respondents. We
present four regression models: men sepa-
rately, women separately, one pooling men
and women into a single analysis, and one
using pooled data but relying on the opposite
spouses report of sexual frequency. Our anal-
ysis using womens reports alone thus relies
on female primary respondents and female
spouses of male primary respondents, and
uses womens reports of most individual and
household characteristics, including sexual
frequency and their and their spouses hours
spent in housework. Similarly, our analysis
for men relies on male primary respondents
and male spouses and uses mens reports of
key variables.
We present two additional results that lev-
erage the fact that we have reports from both
husbands and wives. To account for the fact
that husbands and wives are located within
the same households and standard errors may
be biased by unobserved shared characteris-
tics, we present results from a regression in
which we pool male and female respondents
into a single analysis and use cluster-robust
standard errors. To deal with the possibility of
same-source biasthat our independent and
dependent variables may be correlated
because they are reported by the same indi-
vidualwe conducted a pooled analysis
where the dependent variable is not a self-
report of sexual frequency but spouses
38 American Sociological Review 78(1)
reports of sexual frequency (cf. Amato and
Rivera 1999). Because husbands and wives
do not necessarily complete the questionnaire
on sexual frequency and hours spent in house-
work at the same time, we restricted the sam-
ple to partners who completed the survey in
the same month. This reduces the sample size
to 7,002 for this analysis.
We first present results for the overall
models. We then present results showing
whether variables representing alternative
mechanisms mediate the relationship between
sexual frequency and the household division
of labor, as well as models investigating
whether this link varies with respondents
gender ideologies.
DETERMINANTS OF SEXUAL
FREQUENCY
Table 3 shows results from the regression
models described earlier. Column 1 shows
coefficients and p-values generated using
cluster-robust standard errors from the pooled
analysis of husbands and wives, using self-
reports of their own and their spouses hours
spent in housework and sexual frequency. For
purposes of brevity, we limit discussion of
results that do not bear on our main theoreti-
cal question of interest. These findings are
similar to much previous research on sexual
frequency: self-rated health, wifes and hus-
bands age, young children in the home, and
the amount of time respondents reported
spending alone with their spouse are all sig-
nificant predictors of sexual frequency. In
addition, we find a positive and significant
effect of household income in pooled results,
although the effect does not reach signifi-
cance using only womens reports and is sig-
nificant only at the .05 level using mens
reports.
Our main question of interest, however, is
whether and how mens participation in
household labor is linked to sexual frequency.
Our results suggest that sexual frequency is
highest in households with traditionally gen-
dered divisions of labor. As Table 3 shows,
the coefficient for mens share of core house-
hold labor is negative: households in which
men do more female-typed (core) tasks report
lower sexual frequency. The coefficient for
mens share of non-core household labor, on
the other hand, is positive: households in
which men do more male-typed (non-core)
tasks report more sex. These effects are statis-
tically significant and substantively large.
Overall, these results suggest that sexuality is
governed by enactments of femininity and
masculinity through appropriately gendered
performances of household labor that coin-
cide with sexual scripts organizing hetero-
sexual desire.
To illustrate the substantive size of these
effects, Figure 1 shows predicted values for
sexual frequency, varying the share of house-
hold labor performed by men while setting all
other variables to their means. As the figure
shows, shifting from a household in which
women perform all of the core household
tasks to one where women perform none of
the core household tasks is associated with a
decline in sexual frequency of nearly 1.6
times per month. Given a mean sexual fre-
quency in this sample of slightly over five,
this is a large difference. The figure repre-
sents two extreme values, but even house-
holds in which men do 40 percent of core
household task hours report substantially
lower sexual frequency than households in
which women perform all core housework.
The effect for mens share of non-core house-
work is similar although somewhat smaller.
These models include variables that repre-
sent possible common causes of both a tradi-
tional gender division of labor and higher
sexual frequency: mens and womens work
hours, which may serve as a measure of
broader masculinity or femininity; religious
affiliation; and gender ideology. Not only do
these variables fail to reduce the relationship
between mens share of both types of house-
work and sexual frequency to zero, most are
not statistically significant in their own right,
with the exception of two religious categories:
black Protestants and conservative Christians
report higher sexual frequency. Still, most
important is not these specific differences, but
Kornrich et al. 39
Table 3. Effects of Husbands Share of Core and Non-core Housework on Sexual Frequency
Pooled
Mens and
Womens
Self-reports
Spouses
Report of
Sexual
Frequency
Womens
Self-reports
Only
Mens
Self-reports
Only
b b b b
Husbands Share of Core Housework .416
***
.391
***
.427
***
.403
***
Husbands Share of Non-core House-
work
.167
**
.162
*
.213
**
.091
Total Hours Spent on Core House-
work
a
.259
***
.187
*
.263
**
.233
*
Total Hours Spent on Non-core
Housework
a
.289
*
.303
*
.636
***
.015
Husbands Hours in Paid Work .000 .000 .001 .000
Wifes Hours in Paid Work .001 .000 .000 .001
Gender Ideology .003 .001 .008 .000
Religion (nonreligious and other is
ref. category)

Black Protestant .177
**
.138
*
.192
**
.151
*
Evangelical Protestant .069 .059 .077 .067
Mainline Protestant .006 .013 .037 .030
Catholic .002 .050 .013 .003
Jewish .075 .013 .041 .110
Conservative Christian .158
*
.105 .177
*
.139
Both Spouses Attend Church Weekly
or More
.022 .050 .039 .007
Recently Married .023 .011 .076 .023
Wifes Age .023
***
.022
***
.022
***
.023
***
Husbands Age .015
***
.015
***
.014
***
.016
***
# of Children < 2 Years in Household .179
***
.191
***
.191
***
.182
***
# of Children Age 2 to 6 in House-
hold
.029 .031 .003 .063
*
# of Children Age 6 to 13 in House-
hold
.076
***
.080
***
.083
***
.063
***
Wifes Share of Income .066 .122 .059 .063
Total Household Income .001
*
.001
**
.001 .001
*
How Often Spent Time Alone with
Spouse in Past Month (never is ref.
category)

Once a Month to Once a Week .369
***
.122 .368
***
.342
***
Two or Three Times a Week .606
***
.289
***
.611
***
.569
***
Almost Every Day .744
***
.407
***
.730
***
.725
***
Education (no high school degree is
ref. category)

High School Graduate .051 .039 .059 .063
Completed Some College .009 .015 .017 .009
College Degree .105
**
.124
**
.093 .107
*
Self-rated Health .093
***
.082
***
.105
***
.084
***
Female .045
***
.074
***
(omitted) (omitted)
Intercept 2.051
***
2.436
***
1.955
***
2.247
***
Dispersion Parameter .663
***
.599
***
.662
***
.680
***
N 9,122 7,022 4,561 4,561
a
Coefficient multiplied by 100.
*
p < .05;
**
p < .01;
***
p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
40 American Sociological Review 78(1)
that their existence does not eliminate the rela-
tionship of theoretical interest.
Other models largely confirm findings from
the analysis pooling mens and womens self-
reports. Column 2 of Table 3 relies on reports
of independent variables from one partner and
a report of sexual frequency from the opposite
partner. This eliminates the possibility of same-
source bias, that correlations between inde-
pendent and dependent variables exist solely
because both are reported by the same indi-
vidual. Coefficients and levels of significance
are nearly identical, with the exception of
estimates for how often individuals spent time
alone with their spouse in the past month. This
may be because spouses who filled out the
survey in the same month are more likely to
share activities even if they do not share time,
reducing the size of this effect. Finally, we
present models using mens and womens self-
reports of all items except household-level
measures. These demonstrate whether there
are differences between effects reported by
women and men. Column 3 presents results
using womens self-reports and column 4 pre-
sents results using mens self-reports. There
are few differences between these and earlier
estimates, although results using only mens
reports show no significant effects of non-core
housework. Still, the coefficient remains posi-
tive and husbands share of core housework is
still negative and significant.
These resultswhether using both mens
and womens reports in a pooled analysis,
relying on opposite spouses for reports of our
key variables, or relying on mens or wom-
ens results aloneshow that households
with a more gender-traditional division of
labor report having more sex. The pattern of
results suggests the existence of a gendered
set of sexual scripts, in which the traditional
performance and display of gender is impor-
tant for creation of sexual desire and perfor-
mance of sexual activity. Because we lack
data on sexual desire or related variables, it is
difficult for us to untangle mechanisms link-
ing sex to a traditional division of labor.
Existing data do, however, allow addi-
tional tests for the possibilities that the rela-
tionship between sexual frequency and a
traditional division of labor is mediated by
marital satisfaction or linked by common
causes. If spouses are happier with each oth-
ers contributions in more gender-traditional
3
4
5
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1
S
e
x
u
a
l

F
r
e
q
u
e
n
c
y
Men's Share
Non-core Housework Core Housework
Figure 1. Predicted Sexual Frequency by Mens Share of Core and Non-core Housework,
Results from Pooled Self-report Model (column 1 of Table 3)
Kornrich et al. 41
divisions of household labor, and happiness
leads to greater sexual frequency, then any
relationship between sexual frequency and a
traditional division of labor should disappear
with inclusion of measures of happiness, and
the link would have little to do with sexual
scripts. We test for this possibility and varia-
tion by gender ideology. To the extent that
individuals hold different beliefs about gen-
der, their reaction to gender-traditional behav-
iors could vary. Table 4 shows tests for these
two possibilities and includes coefficients for
our main variables of interest: mens share of
both types of housework, happiness with mar-
riage and with a spouses contribution to
housework, and gender ideology interactions
with mens share of housework. We show
results from the pooled analysis for purposes
of brevity.
13
Finally, we show results using
mens and womens total hours in core and
non-core work rather than shares to show that
results are robust to alternative specifications
of contributions to housework.
These results do not show support for the
possibilities of mediation or an interaction. The
first model shows the effect of including happi-
ness in marriage and happiness with a spouses
contribution to housework. The effect of happi-
ness in marriage is positiveindicating that
individuals who are happier with their marriage
report higher sexual frequencybut it does not
reduce the effect of mens share of these two
types of housework to nonsignificance. In con-
trast, the effect of happiness with spouses
contribution to housework is not significantly
different from zero. Although happiness in
marriage has an important link to sexual fre-
quency, we conclude that it does not account
for the association observed. The second model
shows the effect of including interactions
between our measure of gender ideology and
mens share of core and non-core housework.
Neither of these interactions reaches statistical
significance. Their inclusion does lead the pri-
mary share of housework variables to be non-
significant, but this lack of significance appears
to reflect collinearity; when we subtract means
of variables before generating interaction terms,
the share of housework variables remain sig-
nificant and in the expected direction.
Finally, the alternative specification of the
model using mens and womens hours in house-
work is consistent with our earlier findings.
Mens hours in corefemale-typedhouse-
work are negatively associated with sexual
frequency, and womens hours in core house-
work are positively associated. For non-core
housework, only mens hours are significantly
Table 4. Selected Coefficients: Tests of Alternative Specifications, Happiness as a Mediator
and Interaction with Gender Ideology Using Pooled Self-reports
b b b
Husbands Share of Core Housework .434
***
.393
Husbands Share of Non-core Housework .143
*
.028
Total Hours Spent on Core Housework .224
**
.223
**

Total Hours Spent on Non-core Housework .292
**
.293
**

Happiness with Marriage .112
***
.112
***

Happiness with Spouses Contribution to Housework .005 .005
Gender Ideology .005 .010
Husbands Share of Core Housework Ideology .004
Husbands Share of Non-core Housework Ideology .010
Husbands Hours of Core Housework .007
***
Husbands Hours of Non-core Housework .005
***
Wifes Hours of Core Housework .004
***
Wifes Hours of Non-core Housework .002
a
Coefficient multiplied by 100.
*
p < .05;
**
p < .01;
***
p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
42 American Sociological Review 78(1)
associated with sexual frequency, and the coef-
ficient is positive. Checking effects of the total
number of hours is important, because we could
see a negative effect of a share if coefficients for
mens and womens hours are similarly signed
but one is simply larger than the other. In this
case, however, we find different effects of mens
and womens work, and these effects differ by
task. Thus, when men do more core work,
reported sexual frequency is lower; when men
do more non-core work, reported sexual fre-
quency is higher, consistent with the notion of
sexual scripts. To compare our model to another
recent paper measuring the effects of housework
hours (Gager and Yabiku 2010), we considered
regression models in which we used measures of
mens and womens total housework hours,
combining core and non-core hours into a single
measure. Our results are very similar to those
previously reported: we find significant and
positive relationships between total hours and
reports of sexual frequency, likely because most
of womens hours are in core labor, which is
positively signed, and most of mens hours are in
non-core labor, which is also positively signed.
We note, however, that measures of model fit are
better using our measures than using total num-
ber of hours, and we suggest these measures
better capture the relationship between sexual
frequency and household labor.
The lack of significance for tests of marital
happiness and gender ideology leads us to
conclude that the arrayed evidencethat
households with more traditional arrange-
ments report more frequent sexual activity,
and that this relationship is not mediated by
happiness, religion, gender ideology, or a
range of other variablesis concordant with
a gendered sexual scripts perspective. The
lack of interactions or mediation lends sup-
port, we argue, to the notion that the operating
mechanism is one that links within-couple
displays of masculinity and femininity to
sexual scripts leading to sexual frequency.
Still, our understanding of the exact dynamics
is limited due to the use of quantitative data.
Men or women may, in essence, be turned on
(however indirectly) when partners in a mar-
riage do more gender-traditional work. Of
course, men and women could also be turned
off by doing work that is not traditional for
their gender. Similarly, it is unclear whether
womens or mens reactions to these perfor-
mances are more important. These questions
cannot be untangled with existing quantita-
tive data.
CONCLUSIONS
This article began by noting that American
marriages are more egalitarian today than
they were in the past, but scholars have found
it difficult to offer a clear interpretation of
how egalitarianism has changed the nature of
marriage itself. One broad interpretation of
egalitarianism is that couples exchange
resources across various domains. Moves
toward more equality in one area, such as
earnings, might thus induce more equal distri-
butions in other areas, like housework, a sug-
gestion that has certainly received extensive
investigation. In this article, we asked whether
men and women use housework and sex as
resources for exchange, or whether other log-
ics govern sexual frequency within marriage.
Following up on the widely publicized
claim that by doing more housework, hus-
bands in more egalitarian marriages got more
sex, we sought to investigate the links between
mens participation in housework and sexual
frequency using nationally representative data.
Our findings suggest the importance of gender
display for sexual frequency in heterosexual
marriage: couples where men participate more
in core taskswork typically done by
womenreport lower sexual frequency. Simi-
larly, couples where men participate more in
non-core, traditionally masculine tasks report
higher sexual frequency, suggesting the impor-
tance of gender-typed participation in house-
hold labor. Additionally, although our main
results examined core and non-core labor sepa-
rately, we note that regressions using the share
of total housework (core and non-core com-
bined) also show a negative and significant
coefficient for mens share of housework.
Because the bulk of housework done in
U.S. households involves the traditionally
Kornrich et al. 43
female or core tasks of cooking, cleaning, and
laundry (Bianchi et al. 2000), our findings
stand in marked contrast to the published
claim motivating this study: that husbands
who do more housework get more sex. At the
same time, one can understand how this claim
might have gained currency. First, mens con-
tributions are important for wives satisfac-
tion in marriage. Marital satisfaction is
associated with sexual frequency, and it may
be the case that husbands in more satisfied
relationships qualitatively perceive that they
have more frequent sex even though they
quantitatively do not. Second, to the casual
observer, husbands who do more of the tradi-
tionally masculine tasks in a marriage may in
fact populate the mental category of husbands
who do more to help around the house.
Although men who do more yard work, car
maintenance, household repairs, and the like
might make sizable contributions to the divi-
sion of labor at home, to characterize these
efforts as emblematic of egalitarianism is
misleading. At the very least, our results are
difficult to reconcile with the idea that women
trade sex to men for doing what is tradition-
ally viewed as womens work. Based on our
findings, sex seems to lie outside the realm of
conventional exchange.
The data we selectedWave II of the
National Survey of Families and House-
holdsare the most recent data we are aware
of that include objective measures of both
sexual frequency and the division of household
labor. These data are dated, though, as they
were collected roughly 20 years prior to the
time of writing. Because these data are older,
there is a possibility that the relationships we
document have changed. In particular, the
script that men exchange housework for sex is
a relatively recent one and, as such, may not
have been evident at the time of this research.
However, given the durability of some features
of marriage, including the gendered division of
labor, we suspect our results would still hold
despite the time that has passed since the data
were collected. Additionally, conclusions
about the shift to egalitarianism and effects of
this shift are often based on similarly aged data
from the late 1980s and early 1990s. Although
we may be unable to comment specifically on
patterns of marriage in the present day, our
results are easily applicable to claims about
shifts in marriage.
Sexual frequency appears to lie in the
realm of sexual scripts, but couples are not
purely interested in the amount of sex they
havethey undoubtedly also care about the
quality of sex. Although sexual frequency is
correlated with sexual satisfaction, the corre-
lation is far from perfect. We focus on sexual
frequency in this article in part as a response
to existing media claims about the topic, but
also because sex and housework are enduring
components of marriage, historically predat-
ing romantic love and sexual satisfaction
(Coontz 2005). The importance of sexual fre-
quency for sexual satisfaction, marital stabil-
ity, and marital satisfaction for egalitarian
versus traditional marriages are testable ques-
tions, but not the ones this article asks. If
scripts define a moment as sexual, and govern
sexual initiation, then the sexual scripts theory
explains sexual frequency, not sexual satisfac-
tion. Even if egalitarian couples have the least
but most satisfying sex, the scripts perspective
would not be invalidated. Still, the question of
satisfaction is undoubtedly important and
should be pursued in future research.
In addition to encouraging further research
on the relevancy of sexual scripts for other
components of couples sexual relationships,
our research also brings up questions about
the relationships among sexual scripts, sexual
frequency, and housework for other types of
couples, including same-sex spouses and
cohabiting partners. The past several decades
have seen shifts in whether couples marry or
cohabitate, and there are now more same-sex
partners in the pool of married spouses and
long-term committed partnerships. We sus-
pect the saliency of sexual scripts and house-
work for sexual frequency within cohabiting
and same-sex couples hinges on whether
sexual activity and housework have a similar
meaning for them as they do for married het-
erosexual couples. Research suggests that
the division of household labor among gay,
44 American Sociological Review 78(1)
lesbian, and cohabiting couples is influenced
by earnings and gender, but differences remain
in how these couples divide household labor
compared to married heterosexual couples
(Carrington 1999; South and Spitze1994). We
thus caution against assuming that our find-
ings apply to other types of couples, but we
encourage further investigation into the role of
housework and sexual scripts in shaping sex-
ual behavior across different types of couples.
One contribution of this study is to offer
and test models of the role of sexual activity
within marriage. Existing research acknowl-
edges that sex lives and the frequency of sex
are important concerns for couples, even if
they remain contested terrain (Elliott and
Umberson 2008), but little research suggests
how sex is organized. This article offers a
systematic test of exchange perspectives on
sex in marriage and provides a new perspec-
tive to explain sexual behavior in marriage:
namely, one that emphasizes the continued
importance of gendered sexual scripts.
Our research indicates that changes in
sexual scripts have not kept pace with changes
in the division of household labor. In some
ways, this finding should not be surprising.
Scholars continue to assert that shifts toward
gender equality across multiple arenas occur
at uneven paces, with the organization of
romantic relationships being particularly
stagnant (England 2010). The association we
observed between sex and traditional gender
behavior corresponds with the persistence of
other traditional gender mores within hetero-
sexual romance, including the double stand-
ard that penalizes young women and rewards
young men for sexual agency (England,
Shafer, and Fogarty 2008; Hamilton and
Armstrong 2009). One area we did not inves-
tigate that could offer promise for the future
is mens and womens work behaviors outside
the home, for example, whether the gender-
type of ones occupation also influences sex-
ual frequency (Schneider 2012).
The notion that sex within marriage is bound
to traditional sexual scripts does not necessarily
put egalitarianism at odds with sexual fre-
quency. Rather, the saliency of traditional sexual
scripts suggests that if maintaining certain fea-
tures of marriage, such as sexual frequency, is
desired, increased egalitarianism in one area of
marriage must be paired with comparable shifts
away from traditional gender behaviors, atti-
tudes, and scripts in others. One potential change
may be womens sexual agency. As we noted
earlier, Baumeister and colleagues (2001) docu-
ment substantial differences in sexual interest
and activity between men and women, reflect-
ing double standards that penalize girls and
young women for sexual activity while often
rewarding sexually active young men. To the
extent these double standards become internal-
ized, heterosexual women may subjugate their
own desires and may not feel as free to initiate
sex. One potential interpretation of our results is
that husbands participation in core housework
increases their stress levels and makes them less
likely to initiate sex. If wives do not feel
empowered to initiate sex, then husbands
housework and ensuing fatigue would reduce
the frequency of intercourse. In this interpreta-
tion, it is not necessarily the case that egalitari-
anism in household labor is incompatible with
sexual activity itself, but rather that egalitarian-
ism is incompatible with current sexual scripts.
Gendered sexual scripts punish women for
being sexually agentic and encourage men to be
sexual initiators. If these scripts were to change
and both men and women initiated intercourse,
then the division of household labor would pre-
sumably be less consequential.
14
In conclusion, these results shed new light
on an area of marriagesexthat has
received relatively little recent attention.
More broadly, they expand our understanding
of how couples make bargains in households,
suggesting that straightforward exchange
relations do not govern sexual behavior in
marriage. Instead, a more complex, socialized
set of beliefs and scripts related to gender link
wives and husbands performances of house-
hold labor and sexual frequency, much as
gendered scripts govern a wide range of
behavior. The importance of gender has
declined over time, but it continues to exert a
strong influence over individual behaviors,
including sexual frequency within marriage.
Kornrich et al. 45
APPENDIX
Sexual Satisfaction and Housework
This appendix examines the relationship
between housework and satisfaction with ones
sex life. We present these additional results as
a potential way to gain leverage on two ques-
tions. First, as we mentioned in the main text,
one possible concern is that households with
more traditional gender divisions of labor may
have higher sexual frequency due to coercive
sexual behavior. To the extent this is the case,
wives in more traditional households should
have lower satisfaction with their sex lives,
and men in these households should have
greater satisfaction. As Tables A1 and A2
show, however, this is not what we find.
Instead, wives are more likely to report greater
sexual satisfaction when their husbands report
higher shares of housework, and husbands
sexual satisfaction is unrelated to their wives
reports of mens share of housework.
Another possibility is simply that more
egalitarian households are likely to engage in
a range of behaviors that would not count as
sex but that might lead to greater sexual satis-
faction. If couples with more egalitarian divi-
sions of household labor are more likely to
engage in (unreported) sexual activities that
prioritize womens sexual satisfaction, we
would expect to find the opposite relationship
between womens sexual satisfaction and
mens participation in household labor.
Instead, we still find the same relationship as
for sexual frequency, suggesting this possibil-
ity is unlikely.
These multinomial logistic regression
analyses separately examine the relationship
between housework and sexual satisfaction
for husbands and wives using opposite
spouse reports of sexual satisfaction and
housework. For wives results, the depend-
ent variable of satisfaction is reported by
wives and housework measures are reported
by husbands; for husbands results, the
dependent variable is reported by husbands
and housework measures are reported by
wives.
Table A1. Wives Sexual Satisfaction and Housework
b
Husbands Share of Core Housework .659
***
Husbands Share of Non-core Housework .629
**
Total Hours Spent on Core Housework .001
Total Hours Spent on Non-core Housework .002
Husbands Hours in Paid Work .004
Wifes Hours in Paid Work .001
Gender Ideology .009
Religion (nonreligious and other is ref. category)
Black Protestant .456
*
Evangelical Protestant .248
*
Mainline Protestant .052
Catholic .030
Jewish .518
*
Conservative Christian .166
Both Spouses Attend Church Weekly or More .152
*
Recently Married .147
Wifes Age .019
***
Husbands Age .001
# of Children < 2 Years in Household .125
# of Children Age 2 to 6 in Household .018
# of Children Age 6 to 13 in Household .059
Wifes Share of Income .377
*
(continued)
46 American Sociological Review 78(1)
b
Total Household Income .000
How Often Spent Time Alone with Spouse in Past Month
(never is ref. category)

Once a Month to Once a Week .323
Two or Three Times a Week .603
*
Almost Every Day 1.03
***
Education (no high school degree is ref. category)
High School Graduate .180
Completed Some College .281
*
College Degree .404
***
Self-rated Health .225
***
Cut Points (7 is ref. category)
1 2.915
***
2 2.156
***
3 1.513
***
4 .538
5 .363
6 1.591
***
*
p < .05;
**
p < .01;
***
p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
Table A1. (continued)
Table A2. Husbands Sexual Satisfaction and Housework
b
Husbands Share of Core Housework .314
Husbands Share of Non-core Housework .203
Total Hours Spent on Core Housework .001
Total Hours Spent on Non-core Housework .006
Husbands Hours in Paid Work .001
Wifes Hours in Paid Work .002
Gender Ideology .014
Religion (nonreligious and other is ref. category)
Black Protestant .292
Evangelical Protestant .061
Mainline Protestant .179
Catholic .065
Jewish .460
*
Conservative Christian .006
Both Spouses Attend Church Weekly or More .268
***
Recently Married .202
Wifes Age .027
***
Husbands Age .007
# of Children < 2 Years in Household .195
*
# of Children Age 2 to 6 in Household .104
# of Children Age 6 to 13 in Household .025
Wifes Share of Income .025
Total Household Income .000
How Often Spent Time Alone with Spouse in Past Month (never is
ref. category)

(continued)
Kornrich et al. 47
Acknowledgments
We presented a previous version of this work at the 2009
American Sociological Association conference, and we
would like to thank the audience and the presider from
that session for their valuable comments. We would also
like to thank Pepper Schwartz, Rebecca Sheehan, Jon
Agnone, Tyler Corwin, and anonymous reviewers for
their helpful comments, although we note that mistakes
and conclusions are solely our own.
Notes
1. Amato and colleagues (2003) also show, however,
that mens increasing share of housework seems to
depress their own marital satisfaction.
2. Authors calculations from National Survey of
Families and Households data are available on
request.
3. Although we rely on social exchange theory, similar
predictions could be generated using economic or
quasi-economic theories of household bargaining,
such as separate-spheres bargaining models or a
dependency model (Brines 1994; Lundberg and
Pollak 1993). Sprecher (1998) also proposes a
model based on equity, rather than exchange, that
would lead to similar predictions.
4. To list only a few differences, Baumeister and col-
leagues (2001) found that men desired sex more
often, were more frequently aroused, initiated sex
more frequently, refused sex less frequently, and
had more permissive attitudes. Other evidence on
initiation and refusal can be found in Byers and
Heinlein (1989).
5. There is certainly debate over these findings. Gupta
(2007) argues that a better model is one of womens
autonomy. Recent research continues to investigate
when different models work (Killewald and Gough
2010).
6. Note that the gender-typical work described is con-
sistent with the sexual scripts approach we will
develop.
7. To the extent that gender works through sexual
scripts, it suggests that individuals have internalized
gendered scripts. This is in contrast to some theo-
retical perspectives on gender that suggest gender is
performed to meet others expectations (e.g., West
and Zimmerman 1987).
8. We exclude cohabiting couples because some evi-
dence suggests relationships among cohabitors are
different from those among married couples.
9. Other datasets typically contain measures of satisfac-
tion with the division of household labor and ones
sex life, but not measures of sexual frequency or the
actual amount of time spent on household labor.
10. South and Spitze excluded respondents with more
than four missing items. Because we consider indi-
viduals reports of their own and their spouses
activities, we double the number of potential miss-
ing items to eight. Including respondents with
different numbers of items has little substantive
impact on results.
11. Few couples fell into this category. Nine men
reported all zeroes for items of core housework for
themselves and their wives, one man reported all
zeroes for non-core housework, and two women
reported all zeroes for their own and their husbands
non-core housework.
b
Once a Month to Once a Week .289
Two or Three Times a Week .081
*
Almost Every Day .297
***
Education (no high school degree is ref. category)
High School Graduate .323
**
Completed Some College .439
***
College Degree .543
***
Self-rated Health .289
***
Cut Points (7 is ref. category)
1 3.427
***
2 2.605
***
3 1.869
***
4 .909
*
5 .014
6 1.148
**
*
p < .05;
**
p < .01;
***
p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
Table A2. (continued)
48 American Sociological Review 78(1)
12. We calculated the share using wives and husbands
reports of their own income, because nearly half of
respondents had missing data for the question about
their spouses income.
13. In results from other models, mens share of core
housework always remains significant, and mens
share of non-core housework is significant except in
the pooled model and using only mens reports.
14. We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for
suggesting this possibility.
References
Amato, Paul R., Alan Booth, David R. Johnson, and
Stacy J. Rogers. 2007. Alone Together: How Mar-
riage in America is Changing. Boston, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Amato, Paul R., David R. Johnson, Alan Booth, and
Stacy J. Rogers. 2003. Continuity and Change in
Marital Quality between 1980 and 2000. Journal of
Marriage and the Family 65:122.
Amato, Paul R. and Fernando Rivera. 1999. Paternal
Involvement and Childrens Behavior Problems.
Journal of Marriage and the Family 61:37584.
Baumeister, Roy F., Kathleen R. Catanese, and Kath-
leen D. Vohs. 2001. Is There a Gender Difference in
Strength of Sex Drive? Theoretical Views, Conceptual
Distinctions, and a Review of Relevant Evidence.
Personality and Psychology Review 5:24273.
Baumeister, Roy F. and Kathleen D. Vohs. 2004. Sex-
ual Economics: Sex as Female Resource for Social
Exchange in Heterosexual Interactions. Personality
and Social Psychology Review 8:33963.
Becker, Gary S. 1981. A Treatise on the Family. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Berk, Sarah F. 1985. The Gender Factory: The Appor-
tionment of Work in American Households. New
York: Plenum Press.
Bianchi, Suzanne M., Melissa A. Milkie, Liana C. Sayer,
and John P. Robinson. 2000. Is Anyone Doing the
Housework? Trends in the Gender Division of House-
hold Labor. Social Forces 79:191228.
Bittman, Michael, Paula England, Nancy Folbre, Liana
Sayer, and George Matheson. 2003. When Does
Gender Trump Money? Bargaining and Time in
Household Work. American Journal of Sociology
109:186214.
Blumer, Herbert. 1948. A Sociologist Looks at the Kin-
sey Report. Ecology 29:52224.
Blumstein, Philip and Pepper Schwartz. 1983. American
Couples: Money, Work, Sex. New York: William Mor-
row Publishers.
Brewis, Alexandra and Mary Meyer. 2005. Marital
Coitus across the Life Course. Journal of Biosocial
Science 37:499518.
Brines, Julie. 1994. Economic Dependency, Gender, and
the Division of Labor at Home. American Journal of
Sociology 100:65288.
Brines, Julie and Kara Joyner. 1999. The Ties that Bind:
Principles of Cohesion in Cohabitation and Mar-
riage. American Sociological Review 64:33365.
Byers, E. Sandra and Larry Heinlein. 1989. Predicting
Initiations and Refusals of Sexual Activities in Mar-
ried and Cohabiting Heterosexual Couples. Journal
of Sex Research 26:21031.
Call, Vaughn, Susan Sprecher, and Pepper Schwartz.
1995. The Incidence and Frequency of Marital Sex
in a National Sample. Journal of Marriage and the
Family 57:63952.
Carrington, Christopher. 1999. No Place Like Home:
Relationships and Family Life among Lesbians and
Gay Men. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Chethik, Neil. 2006. VoiceMale: What Husbands Really
Think about Their Marriages, Their Wives, Sex,
Housework, and Commitment. New York: Simon and
Schuster.
Christopher, F. Scott and Susan Sprecher. 2000. Sexual-
ity in Marriage, Dating, and Other Relationships: A
Decade Review. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
ily 62:9991017.
Clark, Alexander L. and Paul Wallin. 1964. The Accu-
racy of Husbands and Wives Reports on the Fre-
quency of Marital Coitus. Population Studies
18:16573.
Collins, Linda M., Joseph L. Schafer, and Chi-Ming
Kam. 2001. A Comparison of Inclusive and Restric-
tive Strategies in Modern Missing Data Procedures.
Psychological Methods 6:33051.
Coltrane, Scott. 1998. Gender and Families. Newbury
Park, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Coltrane, Scott. 2000. Research on Household Labor:
Modeling and Measuring the Social Embeddedness
of Routine Family Work. Journal of Marriage and
Family 62:12081233.
Cooke, Lynn Prince. 2006. Doing Gender in Context:
Household Bargaining and the Risk of Divorce in
Germany and the United States. American Journal
of Sociology 112:44272.
Coontz, Stephanie. 2005. Marriage, A History: How Love
Conquered Marriage. New York: Viking Penguin.
Cornwell, Benjamin and Edward O. Laumann. 2011.
Network Position and Sexual Dysfunction: Impli-
cations of Partner Betweenness for Men. American
Journal of Sociology 117:172208.
Crompton Rosemary, ed. 1999. Restructuring Gender
Relations and Employment: The Decline of the Male
Breadwinner. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
DeVault, Marjorie L. 1991. Feeding the Family: The
Social Organization of Caring as Gendered Work.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Elliott, Sinikka and Debra Umberson. 2008. The Per-
formance of Desire: Gender and Sexual Negotiation
in Long-Term Marriages. Journal of Marriage and
Family 70:391406.
Ellison, Christopher G., Amy M. Burdette, and W. Brad-
ford Wilcox. 2010. The Couple That Prays Together:
Kornrich et al. 49
Race and Ethnicity, Religion, and Relationship Qual-
ity among Working-Age Adults. Journal of Mar-
riage and Family 72:96375.
England, Paula. 2010. The Gender Revolution: Uneven
and Stalled. Gender & Society 24:14966.
England, Paula. 2011. Missing the Big Picture and Mak-
ing Much Ado about Almost Nothing: Recent Schol-
arship on Gender and Household Work. Journal of
Family Theory and Review 3:2326.
England, Paula, Emily Fitzgibbons Shafer, and Alison C.
K. Fogarty. 2008. Hooking Up and Forming Roman-
tic Relationships on Todays College Campuses. Pp.
53146 in The Gendered Society Reader, 3rd ed.,
edited by M. Kimmel and A. Aronson. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Fisher, Kimberly, Muriel Egerton, Jonathan I. Gershuny,
and John P. Robinson. 2006. Gender Convergence in
the American Heritage Time Use Study (AHTUS).
Social Indicators Research 82:133.
Gager, Constance T. and Scott T. Yabiku. 2010. Who
Has the Time? The Relationship between Household
Labor Time and Sexual Frequency. Journal of Fam-
ily Issues 31:13563.
Gagnon, John H. and William Simon. 1973. Sexual Con-
duct: The Social Sources of Human Sexuality. Chi-
cago: Aldine Publishing Company.
Goffman, Erving. 1977. The Arrangement between the
Sexes. Theory and Society 4:301331.
Gornick, Janet C. 2002. Is the Marriage Debate Over?
James Q. Wilson and Janet C. Gornick Discuss
Whether Liberal and Conservative Takes on Mat-
rimony Can Get Hitched. The American Pros-
pect Online. Retrieved November 29, 2012 (http://
prospect.org/article/marriage-debate-over).
Graham, John W., Allison E. Olchowski, and Tamika D.
Gilreath. 2007. How Many Imputations Are Really
Needed? Some Practical Clarifications of Multiple
Imputation Theory. Prevention Science 8:206213.
Greeley, Andrew M. 1991. Faithful Attraction: Discov-
ering Intimacy, Love and Fidelity in American Mar-
riage. New York: Doherty.
Greenblat, Cathy S. 1983. The Salience of Sexuality in
the Early Years of Marriage. Journal of Marriage
and the Family 45:28999.
Greenstein, Theodore N. 2000. Economic Dependence,
Gender, and the Division of Labor in the Home: A
Replication and Extension. Journal of Marriage and
the Family 62:32235.
Gupta, Sanjiv. 2007. Autonomy, Dependence, or Dis-
play? The Relationship between Married Womens
Earnings and Housework. Journal of Marriage and
Family 69:399417.
Hamilton, Laura and Elizabeth A. Armstrong. 2009.
Gendered Sexuality in Young Adulthood: Double
Binds and Flawed Options. Gender & Society
23:589616.
Hochschild, Arlie and Anne Machung. 1989. The Second
Shift: Working Parents and the Revolution at Home.
New York: Viking.
Homans, George C. 1961. Social Behavior: Its Elemen-
tary Forms. New York: Harcout, Brace, and World.
Hook, Jennifer. 2006. Care in Context: Mens Unpaid
Work in 20 Countries, 19652003. American Socio-
logical Review 71:63960.
Horton, Nicholas J., Stuart R. Lipsitz, and Michael Par-
zen. 2003. A Potential for Bias When Rounding in
Multiple Imputation. American Statistician 57:229
32.
Hyde, Janet Shibley, John D. DeLamater, and Erri C.
Hewitt. 1998. Sexuality and the Dual-Earner Cou-
ple: Multiple Roles and Sexual Functioning. Journal
of Family Psychology 12:35468.
James, William H. 1981. The Honeymoon Effect on
Marital Coitus. Journal of Sex Research 17:11423.
Jasso, Guillermina. 1987. Choosing a Good: Mod-
els Based on the Theory of the Distributive-Justice
Force. Advances in Group Processes: Theory and
Research 4:67108.
Killewald, Alexandra and Margaret Gough. 2010. Money
Isnt Everything: Wives Earnings and Housework
Time. Social Science Research 39:9871003.
Kinsey, Alfred C., Wardell B. Pomeroy, and Clyde E.
Martin. 1948. Sexual Behavior in the Human Male.
Philadelphia: Saunders.
Lam, David. 1988. Marriage Markets and Assortative
Mating with Household Public Goods. Journal of
Human Resources 22:46287.
Laumann, Edward O., John H. Gagnon, Robert T.
Michael, and Stuart Michaels. 1994. The Social
Organization of Sexuality. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Lundberg, Shelly and Robert A. Pollak. 1993. Separate
Spheres Bargaining and the Marriage Market. Jour-
nal of Political Economy 101:9881010.
Lundberg, Shelly and Robert A. Pollak. 1996. Bargain-
ing and Distribution in Marriage. Journal of Eco-
nomic Perspectives 10:13958.
Marshall, Will and Isabel V. Sawhill. 2004. Progressive
Family Policy in the 21st Century. Pp. 198230 in
The Future of the Family, edited by D. P. Moynihan,
T. M. Smeeding, and L. Rainwater. New York: Rus-
sell Sage Foundation.
North, Scott. 2007. Book Review: Chethik, Neil. (2006).
VoiceMale: What Husbands Really Think about Their
Marriages, Their Wives, Sex, Housework, and Com-
mitment. New York: Simon and Schuster. Men and
Masculinities 10:12123.
Padavic, Irene and Barbara Reskin. 2002. Women and
Men at Work, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Pub-
lications.
Parsons, Talcott and Robert F. Bales. 1955. Family
Socialization and Interaction Process. Glencoe, IL:
Free Press.
Rao, K. V. and Alfred Demaris. 1995. Coital Frequency
among Married and Cohabiting Couples in the United
States. Journal of Biosocial Science 27:13550.
Rich, Adrienne. 1980. Compulsory Heterosexuality and
Lesbian Existence. Signs 5:63160.
50 American Sociological Review 78(1)
Risman, Barbara. 2011. Gender as Structure or Trump
Card? Journal of Family Theory and Review 3:1822.
Robinson, John and Geoffrey Godbey. 1997. Time for
Life: The Surprising Ways Americans Use Their Time.
University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Schneider, Daniel. 2012. Gender Deviance and House-
hold Work: The Role of Occupation. American Jour-
nal of Sociology 117:10291072.
Schwartz, Pepper. 1995. Love Between Equals: How Peer
Marriage Really Works. New York: The Free Press.
Schwartz, Pepper. 2007. What Sexual Scientists Know
About . . . Sexual Satisfaction in Committed Relation-
ships. Allentown, PA: The Society for the Scientific
Study of Sexuality. Retrieved January 1, 2009 (http://
www.sexscience.org/dashboard/articleImages/SSSS-
SexualSatisfactionInCommittedRelationships.pdf).
Simon, William and John H. Gagnon. 1986. Sexual
Scripts: Permanence and Change. Archives of Sexual
Behavior 15:97120.
South, Scott J. and Glenna Spitze. 1994. Housework
in Marital and Nonmarital Households. American
Sociological Review 59:32747.
Sprecher, Susan. 1998. Social Exchange Theories and
Sexuality. Journal of Sex Research 35:3243.
Steensland, Brian, Jerry Z. Park, Mark D. Regnerus,
Lynn D. Robinson, W. Bradford Wilcox, and Rob-
ert D. Woodberry. 2000. The Measure of American
Religion: Toward Improving the State of the Art.
Social Forces 79:291318.
Stevens, Daphne Pedersen, Gary Kiger, and Susan E.
Mannon. 2005. Domestic Labor and Marital Satis-
faction: How Much or How Satisfied? Marriage and
Family Review 37:4967.
Storms, Michael D., Margaret L. Stivers, Scott W. Lam-
bers, and Craig A. Hill. 1981. Sexual Scripts for
Women. Sex Roles 7:699707.
Sullivan, Oriel. 2006. Changing Gender Relations,
Changing Families: Tracing the Pace of Change over
Time. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.
Sullivan, Oriel. 2011. An End to Gender Display through
the Performance of Housework? A Review and Reas-
sessment of the Quantitative Literature Using Insights
from the Qualitative Literature. Journal of Family
Theory and Review 3:113.
Sullivan, Oriel and Scott Coltrane. 2008. Mens Chang-
ing Contribution to Housework and Child Care: A
Discussion Paper on Changing Family Roles. Chi-
cago: Council on Contemporary Families. Retrieved
November 1, 2008 (http://www.contemporaryfamilies.
org/marriage-partnership-divorce/menchange.html).
Sweet, James A. and Larry L. Bumpass. 1996. The
National Survey of Families and Households - Waves
1 and 2: Data Description and Documentation. Cen-
ter for Demography and Ecology, University of Wis-
consin-Madison, Madison, WI (http://www.ssc.wisc
.edu/nsfh/home.htm).
Udry, J. Richard and Kim Chantala. 2004. Masculinity-
Femininity Guides Sexual Union Formation in Ado-
lescents. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
30:4455.
Waite, Linda J. and Kara Joyner. 2001. Emotional Satis-
faction and Physical Pleasure in Sexual Unions: Time
Horizon, Sexual Behavior, and Sexual Exclusivity.
Journal of Marriage and Family 63:24764.
West, Candace and Don Zimmerman. 1987. Doing Gen-
der. Gender and Society 1:12551.
White, Lynn and Bruce Keith. 1990. The Effect of Shift
Work on the Quality and Stability of Marital Rela-
tions. Journal of Marriage and the Family 52:45362.
Wilcox, W. Bradford. 2004. Soft Patriarchs, New Men:
How Christianity Shapes Fathers and Husbands.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Wilcox, W. Bradford and Steven L. Nock. 2006. Whats
Love Got to Do with It? Equality, Commitment and
Womens Marital Quality. Social Forces 84:132145.
Yabiku, Scott and Constance T. Gager. 2009. Sexual Fre-
quency and the Stability of Marital and Cohabiting
Unions. Journal of Marriage and Family 7:9831000.
Yeh, Hsiu-Chen, Frederick O. Lorenz, K. A. S. Wick-
rama, Rand D. Conger, and Glen H. Elder Jr. 2006.
Relationships among Sexual Satisfaction, Marital
Quality, and Marital Instability at Midlife. Journal
of Family Psychology 20:33943.
Sabino Kornrich is a Junior Researcher at the Center for
Advanced Studies at the Juan March Institute in Madrid.
He will be Assistant Professor of Sociology at Emory
University beginning 2013. Much of his current research
focuses on parents monetary investments in children and
how these have changed over time. He is also interested
in processes that structure relations between income,
expenditures, time in housework, and the role of gender
for these processes. His earlier research examined dis-
crimination charges in organizations and sources of
black-white labor market inequality.
Julie Brines is Associate Professor of Sociology at the
University of Washington. Her research examines how
the principles that organize work and allocation decisions
within families are tied to durable patterns of inequality.
Current projects analyze the effects of changes in local
labor and housing markets immediately before and during
the Great Recession on county-level rates of filing for
divorce. She is also studying how trends in womens and
mens employment have altered the terms of social com-
parison, the use of power, and perceptions of justice in
marriage.
Katrina Leupp is a PhD Candidate in Sociology and an
NICHD Trainee at the Center for Studies in Demogra-
phy and Ecology at the University of Washington. Her
interests include family demography, work, and gender.
Her dissertation examines the consequences of employ-
ment trajectories and family care demands on health and
well-being. Other projects analyze patterns of nonstan-
dard employment hours across the life course, and the
effects of gender attitudes on mothers employment
outcomes.

Potrebbero piacerti anche