Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Why the Null Hypothesis is Not Accepted

A null hypothesis is not accepted just because it is not rejected.


Data not sufficient to show convincingly that a difference between
means is not zero do not prove that the difference is zero. Such data
may even suggest that the null hypothesis is false but not be strong
enough to make a convincing case that the null hypothesis is false.
For example, if the probability value were 0.15, then one would not
be ready to present one's case that the null hypothesis is false to
the (properly) skeptical scientific community. More convincing data
would be needed to do that. However, there would be no basis to
conclude that the null hypothesis is true. It may or may not be true,
there just is not strong enough evidence to reject it. Not even in
cases where there is no evidence that the null hypothesis is false is
it valid to conclude the null hypothesis is true. If the null hypothesis
is that µ1 - µ2 is zero then the hypothesis is that the difference is
exactly zero. No experiment can distinguish between the case of no
difference between means and an extremely small difference
between means. If data are consistent with the null hypothesis, they
are also consistent with other similar hypotheses.
Thus, if the data do not provide a basis for rejecting the null
hypothesis that µ1- µ2 = 0 then they almost certainly will not provide
a basis for rejecting the hypothesis that µ1- µ2 = 0.001. The data are
consistent with both hypotheses. When the null hypothesis is not
rejected then it is legitimate to conclude that the data are consistent
with the null hypothesis. It is not legitimate to conclude that the
data support the acceptance of the null hypothesis since the data
are consistent with other hypotheses as well. In some respects,
rejecting the null hypothesis is comparable to a jury finding a
defendant guilty. In both cases, the evidence is convincing beyond a
reasonable doubt. Failing to reject the null hypothesis is comparable
to a finding of not guilty. The defendant is not declared innocent.
There is just not enough evidence to be convincing beyond a
reasonable doubt. In the judicial system, a decision has to be made
and the defendant is set free. In science, no decision has to be made
immediately. More experiments are conducted.
One experiment might provide data sufficient to reject the null
hypothesis, although no experiment can demonstrate that the null
hypothesis is true. Where does this leave the researcher who wishes
to argue that a variable does not have an effect? If the null
hypothesis cannot be accepted, even in principle, then what type of
statistical evidence can be used to support the hypothesis that a
variable does not have an effect. The answer lies in relaxing the
claim a little and arguing not that a variable has no effect
whatsoever but that it has, at most, a negligible effect. This can be
done by constructing a confidence interval around the parameter
value.

Consider a researcher interested in the possible effectiveness of a


new psychotherapeutic drug. The researcher conducted an
experiment comparing a drug-treatment group to a control group
and found no significant difference between them. Although the
experimenter cannot claim the drug has no effect, he or she can
estimate the size of the effect using a confidence interval. If µ1 were
the population mean for the drug group and µ2 were the population
mean for the control group, then the confidence interval would be
on the parameter µ1 - µ2.
Assume the experiment measured "well being" on a 50 point scale
(with higher scores representing more well being) that has a
standard deviation of 10. Further assume the 99% confidence
interval computed from the experimental data was:

-0.5 ≤ µ1- µ2 ≤ 1

This says that one can be confident that the mean "true" drug
treatment effect is somewhere between -0.5 and 1. If it were -0.5
then the drug would, on average, be slightly detrimental; if it were 1
then the drug would, on average, be slightly beneficial. But, how
much benefit is an average improvement of 1? Naturally that is a
question that involves characteristics of the measurement scale.
But, since 1 is only 0.10 standard deviations, it can be presumed to
be a small effect. The overlap between two distributions whose
means differ by 0.10 standard deviations is shown below. Although
the blue distribution is

slightly to the right of the red distribution, the overlap is almost


complete.

So, the finding that the maximum difference that can be expected
(based on a 99% confidence interval) is itself a very small difference
would allow the experimenter to conclude that the drug is not
effective. The claim would not be that it is totally ineffective, but, at
most, its effectiveness is very limited.

Next section: The precise meaning of the p value

Confidence Intervals & Hypothesis Testing


There is an extremely close relationship between confidence
intervals and hypothesis testing. When a 95% confidence interval is
constructed, all values in the interval are considered plausible
values for the parameter being estimated. Values outside the
interval are rejected as relatively implausible. If the value of the
parameter specified by the null hypothesis is contained in the 95%
interval then the null hypothesis cannot be rejected at the 0.05
level. If the value specified by the null hypothesis is not in the
interval then the null hypothesis can be rejected at the 0.05 level. If
a 99% confidence interval is constructed, then values outside the
interval are rejected at the 0.01 level.
Imagine a researcher wishing to test the null hypothesis that the
mean time to respond to an auditory signal is the same as the mean
time to respond to a visual signal. The null hypothesis therefore is:

μvisual - μauditory = 0.

Ten subjects were tested in the visual condition and their scores (in
milliseconds) were: 355, 421, 299, 460, 600, 580, 474, 511, 550,
and 586.
Ten subjects were tested in the auditory condition and their scores
were: 275, 320, 278, 360, 430, 520, 464, 311, 529, and 326.

The 95% confidence interval on the difference between means is:


9 ≤ μvisual - μauditory ≤ 196.
Therefore only values in the interval between 9 and 196 are retained
as plausible values for the difference between population means.
Since zero, the value specified by the null hypothesis, is not in the
interval, the null hypothesis of no difference between auditory and
visual presentation can be rejected at the 0.05 level. The probability
value for this example is 0.034. Any time the parameter specified by
a null hypothesis is not contained in the 95% confidence interval
estimating that parameter, the null hypothesis can be rejected at
the 0.05 level or less. Similarly, if the 99% interval does not contain
the parameter then the null hypothesis can be rejected at the 0.01
level. The null hypothesis is not rejected if the parameter value
specified by the null hypothesis is in the interval since the null
hypothesis would still be plausible.
However, since the null hypothesis would be only one of an infinite
number of values in the confidence interval, accepting the null
hypothesis is not justified.

There are many arguments against accepting the null hypothesis


when it is not rejected. The null hypothesis is usually a hypothesis of
no difference. Thus null hypotheses such as:

μ1 - μ 2 = 0
π 1 - π2 = 0

in which the hypothesized value is zero are most common. When the
hypothesized value is zero then there is a simple relationship
between hypothesis testing and confidence intervals:
If the interval contains zero then the null hypothesis cannot be
rejected at the stated level of confidence. If the interval does not
contain zero then the null hypothesis can be rejected.
This is just a special case of the general rule stating that the null
hypothesis can be rejected if the interval does not contain the
hypothesized value of the parameter and cannot be rejected if the
interval contains the hypothesized value.
Since zero is contained in the interval, the null hypothesis that μ1 -
μ2 = 0 cannot be rejected at the0 .05 level since zero is one of the
plausible values of μ1 - μ2. The interval contains both positive and
negative numbers and therefore μ1 may be either larger or smaller
than μ2. None of the three possible relationships between μ1 and μ2:

μ1 - μ2 = 0,
μ1 - μ2 > 0, and
μ1 - μ 2 < 0

can be ruled out. The data are very inconclusive. Whenever a


significance test fails to reject the null hypothesis, the direction of
the effect (if there is one) is unknown.
Now, consider the 95% confidence interval:

6 ≤ μ1 - μ2 ≤ 15.

Since zero is not in the interval, the null hypothesis that μ1 - μ2 = 0


can be rejected at the 0.05 level. Moreover, since all the values in
the interval are positive, the direction of the effect can be inferred:
μ1 > μ2.

Whenever a significance test rejects the null hypothesis that a


parameter is zero, the confidence interval on that parameter will not
contain zero. Therefore either all the values in the interval will be
positive or all the values in the interval will be negative. In either
case, the direction of the effect is known.

Potrebbero piacerti anche