Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain and Cognition


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c

Hemispheric connectivity and the visual–spatial divergent-thinking component


of creativity
Dana W. Moore a,*, Rafeeque A. Bhadelia b, Rebecca L. Billings c, Carl Fulwiler d, Kenneth M. Heilman e,
Kenneth M.J. Rood f, David A. Gansler g
a
Department of Neurology and Neuroscience, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York Presbyterian Hospital, 428 East 72nd Street, Suite 500, New York, NY 10021,
United States
b
Departments of Radiology, Tufts University School of Medicine and Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Boston, MA, United States
c
Department of Psychology, Suffolk University and Judge Baker Children’s Center, Boston, MA, United States
d
Departments of Psychiatry, Lemuel Shattuck Hospital, University of Massachusetts Medical School, and Tufts University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States
e
Department of Neurology, University of Florida College of Medicine and The Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Gainesville, FL, United States
f
Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States
g
Department of Psychology, Suffolk University and Department of Psychiatry, Tufts University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background/hypothesis: Divergent thinking is an important measurable component of creativity. This
Accepted 25 February 2009 study tested the postulate that divergent thinking depends on large distributed inter- and intra-hemi-
Available online 7 April 2009 spheric networks. Although preliminary evidence supports increased brain connectivity during divergent
thinking, the neural correlates of this characteristic have not been entirely specified. It was predicted that
Keywords: visuospatial divergent thinking would correlate with right hemisphere white matter volume (WMV) and
Divergent thinking with the size of the corpus callosum (CC).
Brain Imaging
Methods: Volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) analyses and the Torrance Tests of Creative
Creativity
Creative
Thinking (TTCT) were completed among 21 normal right-handed adult males.
White matter Results: TTCT scores correlated negatively with the size of the CC and were not correlated with right or,
Corpus callosum incidentally, left WMV.
Right hemisphere Conclusions: Although these results were not predicted, perhaps, as suggested by Bogen and Bogen
Volumetric (1988), decreased callosal connectivity enhances hemispheric specialization, which benefits the incuba-
Volumetry tion of ideas that are critical for the divergent-thinking component of creativity, and it is the momentary
Quantitative MRI inhibition of this hemispheric independence that accounts for the illumination that is part of the innova-
tive stage of creativity. Alternatively, decreased CC size may reflect more selective developmental prun-
ing, thereby facilitating efficient functional connectivity.
Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction placed creativity and figural fluency within the retrieval factor on
the second stratum. Guilford (1967) proposed that divergent think-
During the past several decades, researchers have begun to ing is a key aspect of creativity (p. 166). According to Guilford, diver-
examine the neuropsychological, anatomic, and physiological gent production pertains primarily to information retrieval and the
mechanisms that might account for creativity. Divergent thinking call for a number of varied responses to a certain item (p. 138). Con-
is an important and measurable aspect of creativity. However, vergent reasoning, which is also an important component of creativ-
divergent thinking is usually not assessed in most tests of intelli- ity, functions when information is sufficient to determine a unique
gence. The goal of this study was to learn how visual–spatial diver- answer (p. 171). Ryder, Pring, and Hermelin (2002), found among
gent thinking, as measured by performance-based tasks, relates to students matched for intelligence that divergent thinking predicted
anatomic measures derived from structural brain imaging. artistic talent. Kim (2008) using a meta-analysis demonstrated that
Sternberg (1999) operationalized the construct of creativity as divergent-thinking scores have a significantly stronger relationship
one of three components of intelligence (analytical, practical, and with creative achievement than do scores on intelligence tests.
creative). Carroll (1993), in his three-stratum theory of intelligence, These findings support the validity of divergent thinking as predic-
tive of creative ability and as a different construct from crystallized
or fluid intelligence (Horn, 1994).
* Corresponding author. Fax: +1 212 746 5584.
E-mail addresses: dwm2003@med.cornell.edu, dwiebe@alum.suffolk.edu (D.W.
Several researchers have theoretically linked creativity with the
Moore). concepts of novelty, variety, and the ability to conceive of multiple

0278-2626/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.011
268 D.W. Moore et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272

ideas. Divergent thinking would be important in the process of Jausovec and Jausovec (2000a), however, found that highly cre-
generating these novel ideals. Pfenninger and Shubik (2001) sug- ative individuals (again classified as ‘‘creative” by high scores on
gested that creativity is the innate ability to ‘‘associate novel con- the TTCT) also had less cooperation between brain regions than
texts with principles of order” (p. 235). Heilman (2005) defined non-creative individuals during the resting state, particularly in
creativity as the discovery of ‘‘unity in the variety of nature” (p. 6). the right hemisphere. One explanation for these seemingly contra-
The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT; Torrance, 1966) dictory findings is that the callosum might play both facilitative
are among the most widely used assessments of creativity (Cra- and inhibitory roles in interhemispheric cooperation. It may be
mond, Matthews-Morgan, Torrance, & Zuo, 1999). These tests use that the brains of creative people have greater modularity that al-
divergent-thinking production tasks and yield scores for fluency, lows for hemispheric specialization and, as proposed by Bogen and
flexibility, originality, abstractness of titles, resistance to premature clo- Bogen (1988), it is the physiological momentary suspension of this
sure, and elaboration. There is a verbal TTCT and a figural TTCT. Clap- modularity that accounts for the illumination that is characteristic
ham (1998) analyzed the structure of the subscores of the figural of creative innovation.
TTCT among 334 introductory psychology students using principle Heilman (2005) proposed that myelinated axons in the brain
components analysis and found that individual subscores contrib- facilitate both inter- and intra-hemispheric communication and
uted very little unique variance to the total index and therefore that both types of communication may be important for creativity.
likely reflected one general factor. However, the contributions of He conjectured that a larger corpus callosum among left-handers
the different subscores were not completely redundant, so use of (Witelson, 1985) might facilitate interhemispheric communication
all subscores was recommended. Plucker (1999) showed that TTCT and account for the possible relationship between left-handedness
test scores explain almost half of the variance in creative achieve- and creativity. For example, in one study, left-handed participants
ment, as measured by an estimate of the quantity of publicly recog- scored significantly higher on the figural TTCT than right-handed
nized creative achievements such as inventions, articles, and awards participants (Newland, 1981), a finding that could suggest a partic-
for creativity and a list of each participant’s three most creative ular role of both the right hemisphere and the corpus callosum in
achievements rated by judges on overall creativity. The contribution divergent thinking. Extrapolating to both right and left-handers, it
of TTCT scores to creative achievement was three times that of IQ is possible there is a general positive association of atypical hemi-
scores. Kim (2008) found through meta-analysis that TTCT tests pre- spheric dominance and divergent-thinking capacity.
dict creative achievement in areas including art, writing, science, There has been much speculation about the relationship be-
and music (r = .33) better than any other divergent-thinking test. tween hemispheric asymmetries (laterality) and divergent or crea-
Neuropsychological assessments have provided evidence of the tive thinking. The right hemisphere has been identified as
link between resection and agenesis of the corpus callosum and a dominant for attention in individuals with typical patterns of dom-
reduction in cognitive complexity. For example, Lewis (1979) dem- inance (Heilman & Van Den Abell, 1980). Gur et al. (1980) found a
onstrated reduced cognitive complexity in 11 patients who had higher ratio of white to gray matter in the right hemisphere com-
undergone callosal resection. These patients showed a paucity of pared to the left, and Kraus et al. (2007), using diffusion tensor
movement responses and integration of parts and a lack of origi- imaging in normal participants, found higher white matter integ-
nality on the Rorschach test, and they provided stories that were rity in the right hemisphere compared to the left. These findings
short and lacking in elaboration on the Thematic Apperception Test indicate a greater role of intra-hemispheric connectivity in the
(TAT). Paul, Schieffer, and Brown (2004), found significant impair- right than left hemisphere. Additionally, investigators have pro-
ments in story logic, social understanding, and common content on vided evidence that whereas the left hemisphere mediates focal
the TAT among five male patients with complete agenesis of the attention, the right mediates global attention (Martinez et al.,
corpus callosum compared to eight male controls. Brown, Paul, 1997). Heilman (2005) has also proposed that global attention
Symington, and Dietrich (2005) found that individuals with agen- might be more important in creative endeavors than is focal atten-
esis of the corpus callosum have deficits in the comprehension of tion, allowing creators to ‘find the thread that unites’. Weinstein
narrative humor and concluded that interhemispheric connectivity and Graves (2002) studied the relationship between divergent
is important in understanding second-order meanings of events. thinking, measured by the Thurstone Written Fluency Test (TWFT),
The understanding of second-order meanings might be important and right hemisphere functions. In keeping with this laterality pos-
in integrative aspects of creativity. tulate, they found that the participants with a reduced right ear
Electroencephalographic (EEG) coherence studies of healthy advantage in a dichotic listening task, which indicates relatively re-
control participants, which measure interhemispheric as well as duced left hemisphere language dominance, had better perfor-
intra-hemispheric communication of neuronal networks, have pro- mance on the TWFT.
vided evidence that communication and thus connectivity be- Creative people might depend heavily on the right hemisphere
tween brain areas is important for divergent thinking. For during creative innovation, but many creative endeavors require
example, Jausovec and Jausovec (2000b) found that during a pre- increased cooperation between hemispheres. Bogen (2000) pro-
sumably divergent, open-ended task (essay writing), 30 student posed that the corpus callosum is necessary for transferring earlier
teachers (ages 18–19) enrolled in a psychology course showed in- integrative aspects of the creative process, which would include
creased EEG coherence between pairs of electrodes in comparison divergent thinking and would be mediated primarily by the right
to the EEG coherence measured during closed-ended tasks, which hemisphere, to the left hemisphere, which would be essential for
presumably required more convergent thinking. Jausovec (2000) creative output, i.e., verbal and motor output.
used the same tasks and grouped individuals as intelligent and/ While callosal integrity, hemispheric cooperation, handedness,
or creative using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and TTCT and cerebral lateralization have all been implicated as factors that
tests, respectively, and found that creative individuals had more influence creativity, there has been little research that examines
inter- and intra-hemispheric EEG coherence than those who were the role of intra-hemispheric white matter pathways. Diamond,
less creative, especially during the essay task. Intelligent individu- Scheibel, Murphy, and Harvey (1985) calculated neuronal to glial
als who were not particularly creative had less coherence than ratios in the cortical association areas of the superior frontal and
both creative and average individuals. Overall, these EEG studies inferior parietal lobes of Einstein’s brain. Compared to 11 brains
support a relationship between divergent thinking and physiolog- of veterans who died of nonneurologically-related diseases, Ein-
ical interactions between brain regions, and these interactions stein’s brain showed a significantly smaller neuronal to glial ratio
might, at least in part, depend on anatomic connectivity. in the left inferior parietal lobe. Based on this finding, these authors
D.W. Moore et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272 269

proposed that the inferior parietal lobe is important in imagery and ric evaluation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review
conceptual thinking, both of which are likely to be important in Boards at Tufts Medical Center (TMC), Lemuel Shattuck Hospital
creativity. Heilman (2005) speculated that these findings might (LSH), and Suffolk University.
have indicated that Einstein had increased brain connectivity,
and this connectivity might have contributed to Einstein’s creativ- 2.2. Apparatus and procedures
ity. Einstein, however, was also dyslexic, and the region that these
investigators studied has been posited to store orthographic lexical 2.2.1. Assessment of divergent thinking
representations. The figural TTCT (Torrance, 1966) was used to measure divergent
In summary, many investigators have proposed that the ability thinking. This test is comprised of three activities in which the par-
to generate novel ideas or divergent thinking is an essential com- ticipant is given shapes or lines and must draw pictures that incor-
ponent of creativity. Studies of patients with callosal resection or porate these shapes and lines. Furthermore, the participants were
agenesis have revealed a decrement in complex cognitive ability, instructed that they should try to make their pictures tell a story.
potentially related to creativity, and EEG studies suggest an associ- Participants also had to title their drawings, introducing a small ver-
ation between divergent-thinking tasks and physiological coher- bal component to this task. The streamlined scoring system was
ence between brain regions. Based on the postulate that used (Palaniappan & Torrance, 2001), which scores the test perfor-
connectivity is critical for divergent thinking, the purpose of this mance for fluency, originality, elaboration, abstractness of titles, and
study was to use structural imaging to examine the relationships resistance to premature closure, all of which contribute to the total in-
between brain connectivity and visual–spatial divergent thinking. dex score. Bonus points also contribute to the index score, which are
The corpus callosum (CC) was chosen as a region of interest given for a number of other creative strengths such as emotional
(ROI) because of its theoretical implications and the availability expressiveness, story-telling articulateness, movement, synthesis
of an existing protocol for measurement, which increases both of figures, humor, richness of imagery, and fantasy.
the reliability and convenience of measurement. The existing pro- The figural rather than verbal test was used because it was
tocol for CC measurement also includes division of the CC into five thought that the verbal TTCT, in contrast to the figural test would
subregions. For the purpose of posthoc analysis in the case of a po- be more confounded by educational background and could involve
sitive finding of a relationship between the CC and TTCT scores, more convergent intellectual abilities. This verbal-educational pos-
measurement of five CC subregions was performed. Established tulate is supported by the finding that better verbal performance is
protocols also exist for measurement of total white matter volume associated with higher levels of education (Bornstein, Suga, & Prif-
(WMV), and since the right hemisphere appears to play a critical itera, 1987). In addition, Kershner and Ledger (1985) found that the
role in creative innovation as well as visual–spatial abilities, right originality subscore on the verbal TTCT was significantly correlated
hemisphere WMV was chosen as a second ROI. It was predicted with IQ scores, but none of the figural scores correlated with IQ. In
that CC area and right WMV but not left WMV would be associated addition, verbal but not non-verbal creative abilities have been
with the facility for visual–spatial divergent thinking. shown to be influenced by convergent thinking among fifth grade
children (Cristante, 1982). Furthermore, Cooper (1991) argued that
2. Method the verbal TTCT relies too heavily on analysis rather than creative
or divergent thinking.
2.1. Participants Raw TTCT subscores for the five subscales were converted to
age-normalized standard scores using the technical manual (Tor-
Participants (n = 21) were recruited through Craig’s List and ads rance, 1998). Total TTCT index scores are calculated by adding
placed in newspapers as part of a larger investigation comparing cog- the bonus points to the average of these five subscale standard
nitive and behavioral function between psychiatric participants and scores. Percentile equivalents of total index scores are provided
age and gender-matched controls (Gansler et al., 2009), with only in the technical manual. The existing normative data for the TTCT
the matched controls reported here. Craig’s List (www.craigslist.com) was established predominantly among elementary and high school
is a community moderated website with local classifieds for major cit- students. While there are normative data for adults, they are not
ies around the world and is available to the public largely free of cost. stratified by age or education. For the purpose of this study, the
Because of gender matching to the psychiatric group, this study’s sam- general reference group of all adults over the age of 18 was used
ple was comprised entirely of males. Participants were between the to calculate total index scores and percentiles, and age and educa-
ages of 25 and 52 (mean age = 39.81, SD = 8.87), right-handed, and tion were analyzed as potential confounding variables during data
able to give informed consent. Exclusion criteria were sensory and/ pre-analyses.
or motor deficits that precluded participation in evaluation proce- To establish inter-rater reliability, ten participants’ TTCT tests
dures, non-English speakers, history or indication of mental retarda- were scored by two separate raters, the primary investigator and a
tion or dementia, contraindication for magenetic resonance imaging trained research assistant, both of whom were unaware (blinded)
(MRI) (e.g., metal in head, cardiac pacemaker, etc.), major Axis I diag- of the MRI results. Both raters were trained through careful reading
nosis, indication on the MRI of neuropathology, history of problems of the figural TTCT manual. An acceptable level of inter-rater reli-
with aggression, anger, or impulse control, and history of substance ability was obtained for scoring of the TTCT (r = .82). In addition, se-
abuse. On average, participants were college educated (mean years ven TTCT tests were sent in to Scholastic Testing Service (STS),
of education = 15.62, SD = 2.36). Table 1 provides information about where a fee-based scoring service is available. The inter-rater reli-
the participants’ characteristics. Each participant underwent an in- ability coefficient for the primary investigator’s and STS scores
formed consent procedure to undergo brain imaging and psychomet- was high (r = .94). The primary investigator scored the remainder
of the tests, and only the primary investigator’s scores were used
in final analyses.
Table 1
Sample characteristics. 2.2.2. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
Mean age (SD) 39.81 (8.87) Brain MRIs were obtained at TMC. After 17 participants had com-
Age range 25–52 pleted procedures, a change in scanner availability forced a change in
Mean years of education (SD) 15.62 (2.36) the MRI protocol. The first 16 participants underwent scans on a 1.5
Years of education range 11–21
Tesla superconducting magnet (Siemens, Iselin, NJ) using the follow-
270 D.W. Moore et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272

ing scanning protocol: (1) Three-dimensional (3D) magnetization tify outliers and normality of distributions for variables of interest.
prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) coronal images Correlation matrices were examined to determine if age or educa-
with TR = 11.1 ms, TE = 4.3 ms, slice thickness/interslice gap = 1.5 tion were related to variables of interest, and bivariate Pearson cor-
mm/0 mm, matrix = 256  256, FOV = 25 cm2, flip angle 15°, and relations were used to test hypotheses. For significant relationships
acquisition time of 6 min. (2) Conventional dual echo Proton density that were found between TTCT total index scores and ROIs, corre-
and T2-weighted images with TR = 2230 ms, TE1 = 20 ms, TE2 = lations were conducted posthoc between these ROIs and TTCT sub-
80 ms; field of view = 23 cm2; slice thickness = 5 mm, no gaps; scores to determine possible differences in contributions of the
matrix = 256  256; and acquisition time 6 min 30 s. The subsequent various subscores. Following a finding of a significant relationship
five participants underwent scans on a 3.0 Tesla superconducting between CC/WMV area and TTCT total scores, posthoc analyses
magnet (Philips, Achieva) using the following scanning protocol: were conducted examining the relationship between CC/WMV
(1) Three-dimensional (3D) T1-weighted coronal Turbo Field Echo subregions and TTCT total scores.
(TFE) images with TR = 8.1 ms, TE = 3.7 ms, slice thickness/interslice
gap = 1.0 mm/0 mm, matrix = 240  240, FOV = 24 cm2, flip angle 8°, 3. Results
and acquisition time of 5 min. (2) Conventional dual echo Proton
density and T2-weighted images with TR = 3200 ms, TE1 = 15 ms, T-tests comparing ROI values between the two scanner proto-
TE2 = 80 ms; field of view = 23 cm2; slice thickness = 4 mm, no gaps; cols showed no significant differences or trends for differences.
matrix = 256  256; and acquisition time 6 min. Thus, change in scanning protocol was not considered a confound-
ing variable. There were no outliers with z-scores with an absolute
2.2.3. MRI image analysis value greater than or equal to three on any variables of interest. All
Regions of interest were measured using Analyze 6.0 image variables met criteria for normality of distribution, with skewness
software, developed by the Mayo Clinic Biomedical Imaging Re- and kurtosis falling between 2 and +2, and the Kolmogorov–
source (Mayo Clinic, 2004), for the CC and WMV. Total WMV was Smirnov statistic was nonsignificant for all variables of interest,
measured through an automatic thresholding method, previously indicating that the data was normally distributed. See Table 2 for
described by Chua et al. (2000) and established by those authors descriptive data for variables of interest. The mean TTCT total in-
as having good reliability and convergent validity with measure- dex score was 113, which falls at the 55th percentile, with a range
ments of phantom MRI. Total intracranial volume (TICV) measure- of 83 (5%ile)–133 (91%ile) and a standard deviation of 12.
ments had been completed with MEDx image analysis software A correlation matrix was created to examine the potential con-
(Sensor Systems, 2002) as part of Gansler et al.’s (2009) study using founding variables of age and education. TTCT scores did not corre-
a manually based procedure described by Blatter et al. (1995), ap- late significantly with age (r = .07, p = .776) or education (r = .20,
plied to T2 images, and these values were used in the current p = .384), so analyses were conducted without covarying for these
study. To prevent introducing confounds due to inter-individual variables. TTCT scores did not correlate with TICV (r = .002,
variation, TICV was used to normalize volumes (Whitwell, Crum, p = .992).
Watt, & Fox, 2001), and ratios of left WMV to TICV and right Table 3 shows the correlations between ROIs and TTCT scores.
WMV to TICV were used in analyses. WMV values are therefore ex- There was no relationship between TTCT scores and WMV/TICV
pressed as fractions (WMV/TICV). in the right (r = .27, p = .235) or left (r = .15, p = .527) hemispheres.
Total CC area and five subregions were measured using a proce- There was, however, a significant, moderate negative correlation
dure described by Hampel et al. (1998) in which an outline trace is between TTCT scores and the CC/WMV size, (r = .45, p = .042).
made on the mid-sagittal section. First, T1-weighted 3D images To analyze for possible differences in contributions of the five
were manually re-oriented in the sagittal plane. Next, the mid-sag- subregions of the CC, correlations between TTCT total index scores
ittal slice was selected by choosing the slice with minimal white and ratios of CC subregions to WMV were conducted. TTCT scores
matter surrounding the CC and where the thalamus appears the showed a significant relationship only with the most posterior CC/
smallest. The CC was traced on the selected slice using the ‘‘auto WMV segment (r = .49, p = .026). To examine the contributions of
trace” thresholding method in which the edge of the callosum is the various TTCT subscores, correlations were conducted between
detected and then followed. In some cases, the tracing needed to CC/WMV and standard scores of the five subscales of the TTCT. The
be edited manually to exclude the fornix. Next, the CC was divided CC/WMV showed a significant relationship only with the resis-
into five subregions using a radial divider composed of ten equally tance to premature closure subscore (r = .47, p = .030). After Bon-
spaced rays. The center of the divider was placed over the midpoint ferroni correction for multiple comparisons, however, which sets
of the lower side of a rectangle surrounding the CC, which was cre-
ated using the ‘‘minimal enclosing rectangle” function. The upper
arms of the radial divider divided the CC into five subregions. In-
ter-rater reliability was established for total CC area (r = .98) and Table 2
on the five subregions (r ranged from .89 to .99) on six brains mea- Descriptive data for variables of interest.
sured by two independent raters, the primary investigator and a Mean SD Range
research assistant. The primary investigator’s values were used in
TTCT total index scores 113 12 83–133
final analyses, and the primary investigator completed the rest of Left WMV (cm3) 278 42 196–359
the CC tracings. During all tracings, both raters were unaware Right WMV (cm3) 271 42 206–342
(blinded) of all participants’ identifying information and TTCT CC area (cm2) 6.53 .9 5.32–8.83
scores. To prevent introducing confounds due to inter-individual
variation and to isolate the CC from total WMV, the ratio of CC size
to total WMV was used in analyses, and CC values are therefore ex-
pressed as fractions (CC/WMV). Table 3
Correlations between TTCT scores and ROIs.
2.3. Data analysis Right WMV/TICV Left WMV/TICV CC/WMV
TTCT total index scores .27 .15 .45*
T-tests were conducted to determine whether scan protocol ef-
*
fected ROI measurements. Descriptive statistics were used to iden- p < .05, Two-tail.
D.W. Moore et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272 271

the new critical alpha level at .005, neither of these posthoc find- concluded that the role of the CC in interhemispheric transfer
ings is significant. seems to be predominantly excitatory, i.e. larger size indicates less
laterality in function and reduced connectivity leads to enhanced
4. Discussion modular organization. Thus, reduced connectivity that allows the
hemispheres to operate more independently might enhance a
The results of this study provide evidence for the relationship number of cognitive abilities including divergent thinking.
between visual–spatial divergent thinking and the size of the CC, Heilman, Nadeau, and Beversdorf (2003) suggested that crea-
but not the WMV of the right hemisphere. It was demonstrated tive acts likely require the integration of different forms of knowl-
that smaller CC size in ratio to total WMV is associated with higher edge stored in independent cortical modules that have not been
divergent-thinking scores. Furthermore, while posthoc analyses previously associated, and a smaller CC might allow the develop-
showed that the posterior CC was the only subregion to correlate ment of independent modular networks that store different forms
significantly with divergent-thinking scores and that only the of knowledge. The lateralized processing of the different forms and
resistance to premature closure subscore correlates significantly types of knowledge stored in the right and left hemispheres may be
with CC size, these findings did not survive experiment-wise error particularly important during incubation and generation of ideas
correction. (divergent thinking). While the binding of the lateralized cognitive
The interpretation of this finding to some extent is contingent modules would require interhemispheric communication, a large
upon further development of our understanding of the relationship CC might not be required for this interhemispheric binding. The
between white matter morphology and cognitive function in coherence observed on EEG during creative endeavors may be
healthy individuals. Certainly, the reduction–demyelination and/ influenced by other physiological or neurochemical-transmitter
or infection–destruction of white matter is understood as the path- factors, and as suggested by Bogen and Bogen (1988), it may be
ological basis for multiple sclerosis and HIV-associated dementia the momentary suspension of this hemispheric modularity that ac-
(Compston & Coles, 2002; Jarvik et al., 1988; Noseworthy, Lucchi- counts for the illumination that is characteristic of creativity.
netti, Rodriguez, & Weinshenker, 2000), but the meaning of WMV While much of the evidence seems to indicate that greater CC
in both normal functioning individuals and those with putative size is associated with increased interhemispheric connectivity,
neuro-developmental disorders is not well understood. the possibility exists that reduced size is actually indicative of
Some research supports the idea that in certain cases smaller more efficient connectivity. This explanation would be consistent
WMV may indicate enhanced functioning, and larger volumes with the hypothesis that divergent thinking is associated with in-
could represent aberrant development with impaired cognitive creased interhemispheric connectivity. Further research using
functioning. For example, many children with autism have in- techniques that can provide information about the nature of white
creased volume, or overgrowth, of white matter (Bashat et al., matter connections, such as diffusion tensor imaging, will help to
2007; Bloss & Courchesne, 2007) possibly as a failure of proper explain the mechanism by which a smaller CC may enhance diver-
pruning. Nagel et al. (2006) found that prefrontal white matter vol- gent thinking. It is also important to note that as the figural TTCT is
umes normally decrease during late adolescence as part of a pre- a predominantly visual–spatial task, interhemispheric connectivity
sumed healthy/normal developmental pruning process. It may be may impact TTCT performance by affecting visuo-motor abilities,
that smaller WMV is indicative of more successful pruning during so divergent-thinking abilities cannot be entirely separated from
development, which might be associated with enhanced brain visuo-motor abilities in this study.
organization through either increased efficiency of connections or Gazzaniga (2005) maintains that a combination of split-brain
through reduced connectivity allowing for more modular organiza- research, diffusion tensor imaging, and functional neuroimaging
tion. Thus, reduced CC size may enhance divergent thinking will advance our understanding of the regional-specificity of the
through a reduction in hemispheric connectivity that allows the CC. While posthoc analyses for the influence that size of different
hemispheres to function more independently and perhaps more regions of the CC might have on the divergent-thinking component
efficiently. Alternatively, this reduction in CC size may be indica- of creativity revealed no significance after correcting for multiple
tive of more selective pruning of pathways leading to stronger comparisons, future studies with larger samples, including women
and more efficient connectivity. and men, as well as more advanced imaging techniques might be
Other research, however, has suggested that increased rather able to identify a role of specific CC regions in divergent and crea-
than decreased CC size is associated with faster information pro- tive thinking. Methods such as diffusion tensor imaging (Le Bihan
cessing and better cognitive performance. For example, Jäncke et al., 2001) and the brain-wise method of voxel-based morphom-
and Steinmetz (1994) found that CC size was negatively correlated etry (Ashburner & Friston, 2000) will be useful in examining the
with hand reaction time in response to auditory stimuli. Hines, role of more regional-specific white matter pathway integrity in
Chiu, McAdams, Bentler, and Lipcamon (1992) found that verbal divergent and creative thinking. For example, white matter con-
fluency was positively correlated with the size of the splenium of nections underlying the prefrontal cortex, which is involved in
the CC. It has also been demonstrated that WMV correlates with executive functioning and higher order thinking, and the parietal
higher performance on a test of visual retention (Allen, Tranel, lobe, important in visual imagery and the forming of associations,
Bruss, & Damasio, 2006). would be relevant areas to target.
Hines et al. (1992) also found that language laterality was inver-
sely related to the size of the CC. Further evidence that increased References
CC size is associated with reduced laterality of brain function
Allen, J. S., Tranel, D., Bruss, J., & Damasio, H. (2006). Correlations between regional
comes from the studies of Morton and Rafto (2006) and Yazgan, brain volumes and memory performance in anoxia. Journal of Clinical and
Wexler, Kinsbourne, Peterson, and Leckman (1995) who, using Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 457–476.
dichotic listening to examine laterality of function, demonstrated Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry – The methods.
Neuroimage, 11, 805–821.
that the participants with less lateralized brains have larger corpus Bashat, D. B., Kronfeld-Duenias, V., Zachor, D. A., Ekstein, P. M., Hendler, T., Tarrasch,
callosa. From the phylogenetic perspective, Hopkins and Rilling R., et al. (2007). Accelerated maturation of white matter in young children with
(2000) found that among different primate groups, including hu- autism: A high b value DWI study. Neuroimage, 37, 40–47.
Blatter, D. D., Bigler, E. D., Gale, S. D., Johnson, S. C., Anderson, C. V., Burnett, B. M.,
mans, as laterality increases across species, CC size in proportion
et al. (1995). Quantitative volumetric analysis of brain MR: Normative database
to total brain volume decreases. Finally Bloom and Hynd (2005) spanning five decades. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 16, 241–251.
272 D.W. Moore et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) 267–272

Bloom, J. S., & Hynd, G. W. (2005). The role of the corpus callosum in brain involvement with pathologic correlation. Archives of Neurology, 45,
interhemispheric transfer of information: Excitation or inhibition? 731–736.
Neuropsychology Review, 15, 59–71. Jausovec, N. (2000). Differences in cognitive processes between gifted, intelligent,
Bloss, C. S., & Courchesne, E. (2007). MRI neuroanatomy in young girls with autism: creative, and average individuals while solving complex problems: An EEG
A preliminary study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent study. Intelligence, 28, 213–237.
Psychiatry, 46, 515–523. Jausovec, N., & Jausovec, K. (2000a). Differences in resting EEG related to ability.
Bogen, J. E. (2000). Split-brain basics: Relevance for the concept of one’s other mind. Brain Topography, 12, 229–240.
Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 28, 341–369. Jausovec, N., & Jausovec, K. (2000b). EEG activity during the performance of
Bogen, J. E., & Bogen, G. M. (1988). Creativity and the corpus callosum. The complex mental problems. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 73–88.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 11, 293–301. Kershner, J. R., & Ledger, G. (1985). Effect of sex, intelligence, and style of thinking
Bornstein, R. A., Suga, L., & Prifitera, A. (1987). Incidence of verbal IQ–performance on creativity: A comparison of gifted and average IQ children. Journal of
IQ discrepancies at various levels of education. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1033–1040.
387–389. Kim, K. H. (2008). Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achievement to both
Brown, W. S., Paul, L. K., Symington, M., & Dietrich, R. (2005). Comprehension of IQ and divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 106–130.
humor in primary agenesis of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia, 43, Kraus, M. F., Susmaras, T., Caughlin, B. P., Walker, C. J., Sweeney, J. A., & Little, D. M.
906–916. (2007). White matter integrity and cognition in chronic traumatic brain injury:
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. New A diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain, 130, 2508–2519.
York: Cambridge University Press. Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J. F., Poupon, C., Clark, C. A., Pappata, S., Molko, N., et al. (2001).
Chua, S. E., Lam, I. W. S., Tai, K. S., Tang, W. N., Chen, E. Y. H., Lee, P. W. H., et al. Diffusion tensor imaging: Concepts and applications. Journal of Magnetic
(2000). A method for rapid volumetric analysis of structural magnetic Resonance Imaging, 13, 534–546.
resonance images of the brain. Hong Kong Journal of Psychiatry, 10, 19–27. Lewis, R. T. (1979). Organic signs, creativity, and personality characteristics of
Clapham, M. M. (1998). Structure of figural forms A and B of the Torrance Tests of patients following cerebral commissurotomy. Clinical Neuropsychology, 1,
Creative Thinking. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 275–283. 29–33.
Compston, A., & Coles, A. (2002). Multiple sclerosis. Lancet, 359, 1221–1231. Martinez, A., Moses, P., Frank, L., Buxton, R., Wong, E., & Stiles, J. (1997).
Cooper, E. (1991). A critique of six measures for assessing creativity. Journal of Hemispheric asymmetries in global and local processing: Evidence from fMRI.
Creative Behavior, 25, 194–204. NeuroReport, 8, 1685–1689.
Cramond, B., Matthews-Morgan, J., Torrance, E. P., & Zuo, L. (1999). Why should the Mayo Clinic (2004). Analyze 6.0. [Computer software]. Kansas: Mayo Clinic.
Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking be used to assess creativity? The Korean Morton, B. E., & Rafto, S. E. (2006). Corpus callosum size is linked to dichotic
Journal of Thinking and Problem Solving, 9, 77–101. deafness and hemisphericity, not sex or handedness. Brain and Cognition, 62,
Cristante, F. (1982). The effects of divergent thinking and convergent thinking upon 1–8.
creative abilities: A multivariate approach. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Nagel, B. J., Medina, K. L., Yoshii, J., Schweinsburg, A. D., Moadab, I., & Tapert, S. F.
Psichiatria, 43, 202–211. (2006). Age related changes in prefrontal white matter volume across
Diamond, M. C., Scheibel, A. B., Murphy, G. M., Jr., & Harvey, T. (1985). On the brain adolescence. Neuroreport, 17, 1427–1431.
of a scientist: Albert Einstein. Experimental Neurology, 88, 198–204. Newland, G. A. (1981). Differences between left- and right-handers on a measure of
Gansler, D. A., McLaughlin, N.C. R., Iguchi, L., Jerram, M., Moore, D. W., Bhadelia, R., creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 787–792.
et al. (2009). A multivariate approach to aggression and the orbital frontal Noseworthy, J. H., Lucchinetti, C., Rodriguez, M., & Weinshenker, B. G. (2000).
cortex in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 171, 145–154. Multiple sclerosis. New England Journal of Medicine, 343, 938–952.
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Essay: Forty-five years of split-brain research and still Palaniappan, A. K., & Torrance, E. P. (2001). Comparison between regular and
going strong. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience, 6, 653–659. streamlined versions of scoring of Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. The
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. Korean Journal of Thinking and Problem Solving, 11, 5–7.
Gur, R. C., Packer, I. K., Hungerbuhler, J. P., Reivich, M., Obrist, W. D., Amarnek, W. S., Paul, L. K., Schieffer, B., & Brown, W. S. (2004). Social processing deficits in primary
et al. (1980). Differences in the distribution of gray and white matter in human agenesis of the corpus callosum: Narratives from the Thematic Apperception
cerebral hemispheres. Science, 207, 1226–1228. Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 215–225.
Hampel, H., Teipel, S. J., Alexander, G. E., Horwitz, B., Teichberg, D., Shapiro, M. B., Pfenninger, K. H., & Shubik, V. R. (2001). The origins of creativity. New York: Oxford
et al. (1998). Corpus callosum atrophy is a possible indicator of region- and cell University Press.
type-specific neuronal degeneration in Alzheimer disease: A magnetic Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance’s (1958 to
resonance imaging analysis. Archives of Neurology, 55, 193–198. present) longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 103–114.
Heilman, K. M. (2005). Creativity and the brain. New York: Psychology Press. Ryder, N., Pring, L., & Hermelin, B. (2002). Lack of coherence and divergent thinking:
Heilman, K. M., Nadeau, S. E., & Beversdorf, D. O. (2003). Creative innovation: Two sides of the same coin in artistic talent? Current Psychology, 21, 168–175.
Possible brain mechanisms. Neurocase, 9, 369–379. Sensor Systems (2002). Medical products division, MEDx (Version 3.4.2) [Computer
Heilman, K. M., & Van Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere dominance for software and manual]. <http://medx.sensor.com> Retrieved.
attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention Sternberg, R. J. (1999). The theory of successful intelligence. Review of General
(neglect). Neurology, 30, 327–330. Psychology, 3, 292–316.
Hines, M., Chiu, L., McAdams, L. A., Bentler, P. M., & Lipcamon, J. (1992). Cognition Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic
and the corpus callosum: Verbal fluency, visuospatial ability, and language Testing Service.
lateralization related to midsagittal surface areas of callosal subregions. Torrance, E. P. (1998). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms-technical manual
Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 3–14. figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
Hopkins, W. D., & Rilling, J. K. (2000). A comparative MRI study of the relationship Weinstein, S., & Graves, R. E. (2002). Are creativity and schizotypy products of a
between neuroanatomical asymmetry and interhemispheric connectivity in right hemisphere bias? Brain and Cognition, 49, 138–151.
primates: Implication for the evolution of functional asymmetries. Behavioral Whitwell, J. L., Crum, W. R., Watt, H. C., & Fox, N. C. (2001). Normalization of cerebral
Neuroscience, 114, 739–748. volumes by use of intracranial volume: Implications for longitudinal
Horn, J. L. (1994). The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg quantitative MR imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 22, 1483–1489.
(Ed.), The encyclopedia of human intelligence (pp. 443–445). New York: Witelson, S. F. (1985). The brain connection: the corpus callosum is larger in left-
Macmillan. handers. Science, 229, 665–668.
Jäncke, L., & Steinmetz, H. (1994). Interhemispheric transfer time and corpus Yazgan, M. Y., Wexler, B. E., Kinsbourne, M., Peterson, B., & Leckman, J. F. (1995).
callosum size. Neuroreport, 5, 2385–2388. Functional significance of individual variations in callosal area.
Jarvik, J. G., Hesselink, J. R., Kennedy, C., Teschke, R., Wiley, C., Spector, S., et al. Neuropsychologia, 33, 769–779.
(1988). Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. Magnetic resonance patterns of

Potrebbero piacerti anche