Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

Complex Adaptive Systems, Volume 1 Cihan H. Dagli, Editor in Chief Conference Organized by Missouri University of Science and Technology 2011- Chicago, IL

Medical Process Modeling with a Hybrid System Dynamics Zachman Framework


Bharath Dantua, Eric Smithb*
RIMES: Research Institute for Manufacturing and Engineering Systems, University of Texas al El Paso (UTEP), 500 W. University Ave., El Paso, TX 79968, USA
a,b

Abstract Diagnoses are perhaps the most complex and crucial decisions within the modern healthcare enterprise. Models for complex healthcare decisions must incorporate consideration for the usual multiplicity of important factors, interacting feedback loops among these factors, and the dynamic nature of the full diagnostic arena. A diagnoses modeling technique that has the requisite variety of relevant considerations is presented. The technique has the potential to overcome mandatory time criteria, while considering the competence and robustness of high importance diagnostic decisions. In this study, descriptive narratives dictated by examining physicians who were directly involved in the diagnosis and treatment of patients were examined in detail not only to extract key factors involved in medical decision making processes, but also to illustrate the wide ontological origin of key decision making factors. Important factors in the narratives were identified and mapped with a new System Dynamics methodology that incorporates a Zachman Framework for establishing the overall scope and context of the full medical decision making context within the modern medical enterprise. The two techniques produce a synergy that addresses the debilities of the techniques in isolation, allowing enhanced comprehension of diagnostic processes, and their improvement. 2011 Published by Elsevier B.V.
Keywords: Medical decision making, diagnosis, system dynamics, Zachman framework

1, Introduction Substantial documentation about experts making judgments unsupported by rational analysis exists in the field of medical decision making. Groopman and Montgomery have described How Doctors Think, or the narratives and ideas that doctors personally employ during medical decision making. Redelmeier and Shafir (while noting that much medical decision making is seemingly simple and supported by a confirmatory environment of medical colleagues) noted that in decisions with multiple alternatives, physicians: do not always resolve medical decisions by analyzing benefits and harms in a reliable manner. Instead, people may resort to intuitive judgments that render them prone to cognitive biases. Psychological theory
Bharath Dantu. Tel.: 915-240-7571 E-mail address: bbdantu@miners.utep.edu

18770509 2011 Published by Elsevier Ltd. doi:10.1016/j.procs.2011.08.016

Bharath Dantu and Eric Smith / Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

77

predicts that these cognitive biases will appear when people have difficulty deciding between conflicting alternatives, face situations of substantial uncertainty, or consider outcomes that have long-term consequences. Thinking harder will not eliminate a cognitive bias any more than staring intently will make a visual illusion disappear. Instead, physicians need to recognize specific biases and consider possible corrective procedures (p. 302). Redelmeier and Shafir go on to describe how a technique such as considering each alternative in relation to the status quo is more effective than considering all alternatives at once and only in relation to themselves. More biases that result from the consideration of alternatives exist, and are presented below. We encourage pair wise comparisons, as in the Analytic Hierarchy Process (AHP) (although intransitivity can sometimes result). Also, alternatives should always be compared to the status quo. Specific de-biasing techniques have been shown to be effective under specific circumstances. Principally, such techniques involve increasing the decision makers awareness of possible cognitive biases, and then mandating a procedure that has been shown to reduce the particular bias. However, in practice, few professionals remember the presence of biases, and almost none implement proven de-biasing processes. What is needed is a single abstract model of biasing, and the ability to apply the model generally. 2. Attribute Substitution In this example, the target attribute, Happiness, is assessed by mapping the value of another attribute, Number of dates last month, onto the target attribute scale. Figure 1 illustrates the top-level, ill-defined attribute of Happiness, and the well-defined sub-level attribute of Number of dates last month.

Target Attribute: Happiness

fa lse ta rg et at tri bu

te n bu tio tri tu At sti b Su

te

fal

se

tar

ap

ge

pin

ta

ttri

bu

te

ma

pp

ing

Heuristic Attribute: Number of dates last month

Figure 1: Happiness attribute substituted by Number of dates last month This process of attribute substitution will control judgment when these three conditions are satisfied: (1) The target attribute is relatively inaccessible [Happiness]; (2) A semantically and associatively related candidate attribute is highly accessible [Number of dates last month]; and (3) The substitution of the heuristic attribute in the judgment and the immediacy of the response is not rejected by the critical operations of System 2 (p. 54). System 2 is defined by Kahneman as being composed of those mental operations that are slower, serial, effortful, more likely to be consciously monitored and deliberately controlled (p. 698). In general, System 2 consists of explicit cognitive processes, as opposed to mental operations that are automatic, effortless, associative and implicit, which are performed by what Kahneman calls System 1. The division between System 1 and System 2 is typical across mental operations research. The extension of the mental process of attribute substitution to many other heuristics and decisions is quite straightforward. Figure 2 illustrates attribute substitution with a hierarchical tree of attributes. When erroneous attribute substitution holds sway, and a person is asked a question about a top-level attribute, they are likely to substitute the top-level attribute with a lower-level attribute that is less inclusive and more definitely and specifically defined.

78

Bharath Dantu and Eric Smith / Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

Concomitant with the move to a lower-level attribute is the construction of a more easily answered question about the lower-level attribute.
Target Attribute
Heuristic Attribute 1 Heuristic Attribute 2

SubAttribute

SubAttribute

SubAttribute

SubAttribute

SubAttribute

Figure 2: Attribute substitution illustrated with hierarchical decomposition Just as the map is not the territory, a heuristic attribute is not the target attribute. Recognizing the super-heuristic of attribute substitution quickly leads one to question what other mental constructs must be differentiated from the reality they represent. Decision making models, for example, are only abstract representations of real systems. It is interesting to note that the process of attribute substitution involves two steps that reduce complexity: (1) Representation of the real system by the formation of an abstract model with a more definite and possibly reduced set of attributes, and, (2) Attribute substitution, or choosing one of the sub-attributes to represent the abstract model under examination. Step (1) involves the structuring of the decision space, and is reminiscent of Newtons replacement of a naturalistically ambiguous world of causes and effects with a world of distinct variables and mathematical relations among them. 3. Zachman Framework The Zachman Framework was originally an enterprise modeling tool. It is essentially a 6x6 matrix which defines 6 levels relevant to any enterprise, as well as 6 aspects. The structuring provided by the Zachman Framework provides that attention is placed on all the relevant scales, as well as on all relevant aspects, of any situation under consideration. Any Zachman Framework should be calibrated so that all relevant scales occur within its boundaries. A Zachman Framework with an added level for the full medical enterprise is shown in Figure 3.
What Wellbeing Scope Medical practice System How Where Who When Why Strategists

Community

Leaders

Business

Architects

Knowledge

Diagnosis

Doctor

Domains

Specialties

Specialists

Tests

Physiology

Technicians

Attendants

Operations Examined Process Network


Patient

Nurses Timing Motivation

Figure 3: Zachman Framework for the medical enterprise

Bharath Dantu and Eric Smith / Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

79

The circles in this particular depiction of the show the enterprise areas that were involved in the diagnosis of Anne Dodge, a patient whose particularly difficult story of finding health is narrated by Groopman (pp. 1-9). The number and variety of enterprise areas involved in one diagnosis is surprising, and indicates that medical professionals need an expanded awareness of the entire medical enterprise in order to serve patients. 4. Diagnostic Interviewing Doctors interface with patients through three principal means, interviews, observation and testing. While testing based on scientific principles is highly useful, first hand information based on the patients perspective is often available on through interviews with the patient, and, for this reason, medical interviewing has been described and developed by Coulehan and Block, Enelow, Forde and Brummel-Smith, Newell, Berstein and Berstein, and Froelich and Bishop. Interviewing and observation is of course balance with quantitative evidence-based medical practice. Complementary balance in medical decision making is described by Seedhouse in Values-Based Decision-Making for the Caring Professions. An abridged and edited example based on a diagnostic interview from Newell (pp. 89-98) follows: Doctor: Please tell me in general terms the issues that brought you here today. Patient: Well, its about my inhaler. They say I use it far too much. Doctor: What do you think yourself? Patient: I know the amount that Im using it has increased just of late. Doctor: It sounds like controlling its use is a problem. Patient: Definitely. Every time I go anywhere, I have to have it with me just in case. Doctor: What symptoms do you get before attacks? Patient: Tightness in my chest, wheezing, and my heart starts thumping Doctor: And that makes you say certain things to yourself? Patient: Yes, its very frightening. Doctor: And then what do you do? Patient: I take maybe three puffs, and wait and see if the breathlessness will pass. Doctor: When does this happen? Patient: Particularly when I am driving and the kids are in the car. Doctor: You say you have had asthma for 20 years Patient: Yes. Doctor: Our approach will be for you to relax. Patient: Ok Doctor: and when the attacks come on, delay reaching for the inhaler for just a few seconds, to give you a better idea if you can cope with the anxiety that accompanies an attack. Patient: Ok. Doctor: This goes along with helping you reach some of the goals you are setting for yourself in life. Patient: Yes, I want to get to where I enjoy life. Doctor: Gradually, you will lessen the frequency of puffs, and the need for prescriptions. Patient: Sounds like a good plan. 5. System Dynamics Systems Dynamics (SD) is a methodology used to define the influences and relations among many factors that contribute to directed effects and feedback loops. System Dynamics was developed by Forrester, and has been adopted widely as a visualization of complex systems. SD drawings can be used to visualize the many attributes pertinent to a medical diagnosis, as obtained in a medical interview. Additionally, the directed arcs of SD show attributes drew the attention of the interviewing doctor, and which causal attributes were deemed to contribute, either positively or negatively, to affect attributes. The diagnostic interview quoted above can be shown with an SD diagram. As a first option, the SD diagram could show the temporal order of attributes as they arose in the interview, but this would produce a rather tangled diagram. As a second option, the SD diagram could try to sort out the causality among the attributes, as depicted in Figure 4.

80

Bharath Dantu and Eric Smith / Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

Feelings/Emotions Symptoms Frequency of use Driving Relaxation Knowledge of companions Internal Narrative

Inhaler Kids at play Issues

Knowledge of frequency

Knowledge of location

Goals in life Control Timing of attacks

Figure 4: System Dynamics depiction of influences noted during interview 6. System Dynamics within the Zachman Framework System Dynamic elements, within pure SD diagrams, have no guarantee of being arranged in an intuitive fashion that shows the classification of attributes according to their levels and aspects. The use of a Zachman Framework with an overlaying SD diagram accomplishes the purpose of clarifying the inherent differentiations and relative scales of all factors used within an SD diagram. Note how the SD diagram for the medical interview is clarified with the backdrop of a Zachman Framework in Figure 5.
What Wellbeing How Where Who When Why
Goals in life

Issues

Cognitive

Internal Narrative

Knowledge of location

Knowledge companions

Knowledge of frequency

Behavioural Autonomic / Physical Treatment Aids

Control

Driving

Kids at play

Relaxation

Symptoms

Timing of attacks

Feelings / Emotions

Inhaler

Frequency of use

Things

Process

Network

People

Timing

Motivation

Figure 5: Medical interview shows with a System Dynamics diagram overlaid on a Zachman Framework Conclusion Medical decision making quality is critical throughout the modern medical enterprise. Despite the availability of analytic techniques, most decision making is done sole with the human mind. Mental decision making is subject to many mistakes of a great variety, which can be abstractly described as the mistake of attribute substitution. A diagnostic process modeled with system dynamics shows that humans are susceptible to a collapse of attention in which they focus on only a few attributes and the relations among them. In fact, excessive focus on the degree of relation among the few attributes can lead to amnesia as to the full spectrum of relevant attributes. The principal amelioration for attribute substitution is a widening of attention to a degree where awareness of the entire enterprise is maintained. The Zachman Framework can form the backdrop for a decision making process, ensuring that no mistaken collapse of attention occurs.

Bharath Dantu and Eric Smith / Procedia Computer Science 6 (2011) 7681

81

References Groopman, J., 2007, How doctors think, New York: Houghton Mifflin Co. Montgomery, K., 2006, How doctors think: Clinical judgment and the practice of medicine, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Redelmeier, D., & Shafir, E., 1995, Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives, Journal of the American Medical Association, 273(4), 302-305. Saaty, T. L., 1980, The analytic hierarchy process: Planning, priority setting, resource allocation, New York: McGraw-Hill. Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Schwarz, N., 1988, Priming and communication: The social determinants of information use in judgements of life-satisfaction, European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 429-442. Kahneman, D., 2003, A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality,. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697-720. Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S., 2002, Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment, In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and Biases (pp. 49-81). New York: Cambridge University Press. Miller, G. A., 1956, The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review, 63, 81-87. Kahneman, D., & Riepe, M., 1998, Aspects of investor psychology: Beliefs, preferences, and biases investment advisors should know about, Journal of Porfolio Management, 24(4), 52-65. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D., 1982, Judgments of and by representativeness, In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 84-98). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Smith, E. D., Son, Y. J., Piattelli-Palmarini, M., & Bahill, A. T., 2007, Ameliorating mental mistakes in tradeoff studies, Systems Engineering, 10(3), 222-240. Korzybski, A., 1948, Science and Sanity: An introduction to non-Aristotelian systems and general semantics, Lakeville, CT: The International Non-Aristotelian Library Publishing Company. Gilb, T., 2005, Competitive engineering: A Handbook for systems engineering, requirements engineering, and software engineering using Planguage, New York: Elsevier. Keeney, R. L., 1992, Value-focused thinking: A path to creative decisionmaking, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kelvin, L., 1891, Electrical units of measurement, In Popular lectures and addresses (Vol. 1, pp. 86-87). Zachman, J. A., 1987, A framework for information systems architecture, IBM Systems Journal, 26(3). Coulehan, J. L., & Block, M. R., 2006, The medical interview: Mastering skills for clinical practice, Philadelphia: F. A. Davis Company. Enelow, A. J., Forde, D. L., & Brummel-Smith, K., 1996, Interviewing & patient care, New York: Oxford University Press. Newell, R., 1994, Interviewing skills for nurses and other health care professionals: A structured approach, London: Routledge. Bernstein, L., & Bernstein, R. S., 1985, Interviewing: A guide for health professionals, Norwalk, CT: AppletonCentury-Crofts. Froelich, R. E., & Bishop, F. M., 1969, Medical interviewing: A programmatic manual, Saint Louis: The C. V. Mosby Company. Aveyard, H., & Sharp, P., 2009, A beginner' guide to evidence based practice in health and social care, Berkshire, England: Open University Press, McGraw Hill. Seedhouse, D., 2005, Values-based decision-making for the caring professions, New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Forrester, J. W., 1971, Counterintuitive behavior of social systems, Technology Review, 73(3), 52-68. Forrester, J. W., 2009, System dynamics: The classroom experience: Quotations from K-12 teachers, Unpublished manuscript, Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT), Cambridge, MA.

Potrebbero piacerti anche